Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Apr 29.
Published in final edited form as: Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol. 2008 Nov;82(11):755–762. doi: 10.1002/bdra.20517

Table 4.

Association of Risk of Having Offspring with NTDs with Maternal Race-Ethnicity and Language Preference Subgroups, Adjusted for NTD Risk Factors

No. subjects
Adjusted OR (95% CI)*
Maternal race-ethnicity and language preference NTDs Anencephaly Spina bifida Controls NTDs Anencephaly Spina bifida
Foreign-born Hispanic, Spanish preference 105 60 58 185 (150) 1.8 (1.0, 3.2) 1.5 (0.7, 3.0) 2.2 (1.1, 4.7)
Foreign-born Hispanic, English preference 23 6 18 50 (47) 1.4 (0.7, 2.8) 0.7 (0.2, 2.0) 2.4 (1.0, 5.7)
US-born Hispanic, Spanish preference 22 12 10 64 (62) Reference Reference Reference
US-born Hispanic, English preference 36 16 20 68 (68) 1.5 (0.8, 2.9) 1.3 (0.5, 2.9) 1.9 (0.8, 4.5)
Non-Hispanic White 69 27 42 109 (109) 2.6 (1.4, 4.7) 1.9 (0.9, 4.2) 3.6 (1.6, 8.0)
*

A11 models are adjusted for intake of folic acid supplements, smoking, and stressful life events; in addition, results for NTDs and spina bifida are adjusted for body mass index, and results for anencephaly are adjusted for food insecurity.

Language preference was defined as follows, based on responses to the question: “In general, what language do you read and speak?” Foreign-born women who responded “only Spanish” or “Spanish more than English” were designated as preferring Spanish; US-born women who responded “only English” or “English more than Spanish” were designated as preferring English.

Number of controls for models for anencephaly are shown first; numbers for models for spina bifida are in parentheses; numbers are different because the set of covariates was different.