Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Cogn. 2009 May 7;71(2):72–83. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.04.004

Table 4.

Functional activations demonstrating a significant difference between SRRT cognitive control greater than automatic condition preparatory-related activity compared to Go/NoGo variant cognitive control greater than automatic condition response-related activity.

Region of Interest BA Coordinates Clus. Max t Condition

X Y Z
Right middle temporal 21 43 −26 −8 13 5.57 NoGoCog>NoGoAuto
Left middle temporal 21 −54 −27 −2 13 4.05 NoGoCog>NoGoAuto
NoGoCog>SRRTCog
Right parietal 40 36 −34 44 14 4.68 NoGoAuto>NoGoCog
NoGoAuto>SRRTAuto
Left parietal 40 −43 −35 51 55 5.54 NoGoAuto>NoGoCog
NoGoAuto>SRRTAuto
Left posterior cingulate 29 −2 −43 7 58 6.95 NoGoAuto>NoGoCog
SRRTCog>NoGoCog
Left middle temporal 21/37 −49 −47 −1 18 5.59 NoGoCog>NoGoAuto
SRRTAuto>SRRTCog
NoGoCog>SRRTCog
Left middle/superior temporal 39/40 −59 −53 30 34 4.84 NoGoCog>NoGo Auto
SRRTAuto>SRRTCog

Note: Abbreviations: BA: Brodmann’s area; Clus.: Cluster size, number of contiguous voxels; NoGoCog:Go/NoGo variant cognitive control condition; NoGoAuto:Go/NoGo variant automatic condition; SRRTCog:SRRT cognitive control condition; SRRTAuto:SRRT automatic condition.

The contrast column lists the paired t-tests that were significantly different for the following four tests conducted: cognitive control vs. automatic condition for Go/NoGo variant; cognitive control vs. automatic condition for SRRT; automatic condition for SRRT vs. Go/NoGo; cognitive control condition for SRRT vs. Go/NoGo. Analyses were thresholded at p<0.005, 11 degrees of freedom, with a contiguity threshold of 8.