Abstract
In women, ovarian hormone loss at menopause has been related to cognitive decline, and some studies suggest that estrogen-containing hormone therapy (HT) can mitigate these effects. Recently, the Women’s Health Initiative study found that conjugated equine estrogens, the most commonly prescribed HT, do not benefit cognition. Isolated components of conjugated equine estrogens (tradename Premarin®) have been evaluated in vitro, with Δ8,9-dehydroestrone (Δ8E1) and equilin showing the strongest neuroprotective profiles. It has not been evaluated whether Δ8E1 or equilin impact cognition or the cholinergic system, which is affected by other estrogens and known to modulate cognition. Here, in middle-aged, ovariectomized rats, we evaluated the effects of Δ8E1 and equilin treatments on a cognitive battery and cholinergic nicotinic receptors (nAChR). Specifically, we used 125I-labeled epibatidine binding to assay the neuronal nicotinic receptor containing 4α and 2β subunits (α4β2-nAChR), since this nicotinic receptor subtype has been shown previously to be sensitive to other estrogens. Δ8E1 enhanced spatial working, recent and reference memory. Δ8E1 also decreased hippocampal and entorhinal cortex α4β2-nAChR expression, which was related to spatial reference memory performance. Equilin treatment did not affect spatial memory or rat α4β2-nAChR expression. Neither estrogen impacted 86Rb+ efflux, indicating lack of direct action on human α4β2 nAChR function. Both estrogens influenced vaginal smear profiles, uterine weights, and serum luteinizing hormone levels, analogous to classic estrogens. The findings indicate that specific isolated Premarin® components differ in their ability to affect cognition and nAChR expression. Taken with the works of others showing Δ8E1-induced benefits on several dimensions of health-related concerns associated with menopause, this identifies Δ8E1 as a new avenue to be investigated as a potential component of HT that may benefit brain health and function during aging.
Keywords: Premarin®; estrogen; Δ8,9-dehydroestrone; equilin; learning; memory; nicotinic acetylcholine receptor; working memory; reference memory
Introduction
By the year 2050, an estimated 45 million postmenopausal women in the United States will have to make the choice of whether to utilize hormone therapy (HT; U.S. Census Bureau, 2007). Ovarian hormone loss due to surgical or natural menopause has been associated with cognitive decline in women (Nappi et al., 1999; Phillips and Sherwin, 1992; Sherwin, 1988); however, the question of how HT impacts cognition is unclear. Premarin®, a complex conjugated equine estrogen preparation synthesized from the urine of pregnant mares, has been given since 1942 and is the most widely used estrogen component of HT in the United States (Hersh et al., 2004; Stefanick, 2005). Some studies in women demonstrate that Premarin®-containing HT improved memory (Campbell and Whitehead, 1977; Kantor et al., 1973; Ohkura et al., 1995). However, the largest placebo-controlled double blind study conducted to date evaluating HT effects on all-cause dementia, the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS), found that conjugated equine estrogen treatment given alone did not reduce incidence of dementia or mild cognitive impairment (Shumaker et al., 2004), and that treatment with conjugated equine estrogens plus the progestin medroxyprogesterone acetate increased the risk of cognitive decline and probable dementia, and did not prevent mild cognitive impairment (Rapp et al., 2003; Shumaker et al., 2003). A recent follow-up study evaluating WHIMS participants found that Premarin® treatment, with or without medroxyprogesterone acetate, was associated with brain atrophy as assessed via MRI scans (Resnick et al., 2009). Thus, while noting many factors likely affected the WHIMS findings, including the global nature of the cognitive measure, older age of participants, and duration of hormone deprivation before treatment initiation (for discussion see Sherwin, 2005), the collected findings suggest that although the absence of ovarian hormones is not optimal for cognition, neither is the most commonly utilized HT.
Premarin® contains the sulfates of more than 10 estrogens, is over 50% estrone, 20–25% equilin, 3.5% Δ8,9-dehydroestrone (Δ8E1), and contains only trace amounts of 17β-estradiol (the most potent naturally circulating estrogen in women and rats); after metabolism, the resulting biologically active circulating hormones are primarily estrone and equilin, and after estrone’s conversion, 17β-estradiol (Bhavnani, 2003; Bhavnani et al., 1998; Mayer et al., 2008; Sitruk-Ware, 2002). In the last decade, landmark basic science research from the Brinton laboratory has led to several discoveries regarding the neuroprotective properties of estrogens. This work demonstrated that some components of Premarin® enhanced markers of neuroprotection, while others showed little benefit (Brinton et al., 1997; Zhao and Brinton, 2006). The estrogens Δ8E1 and equilin, found naturally in horses but not in women or rats, were the two primary Premarin® components that showed the most consistent and potent neuroprotective effects in vitro (Zhao and Brinton, 2006). Δ8E1- and equilin-mediated neuroprotection in vitro could potentially translate to improved function of neural networks and brain regions mediating cognitive function, resulting in mnemonic enhancements in vivo.
Basal forebrain cholinergic neurons project to the hippocampus and surrounding cortical areas, and play an important role in learning and memory (Hasselmo, 2006). 17β-estradiol enhances basal forebrain cholinergic function, as evidenced by expression of different cholinergic markers and pharmacological cholinergic challenges (e.g., Gibbs, 2000a; Markowska and Savonenko, 2002; Packard and Teather, 1997). In fact, choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) increases in the basal forebrain months after transient exposure to 17β-estradiol, noting effects are sensitive to several variables including timing and dose (Bohacek et al., 2008; Gibbs, 1997; Rodgers et al., 2010). Our laboratory has demonstrated Premarin®-induced benefits to the cholinergic system, whereby Premarin® treatment prevented scopolamine-induced amnesia and increased number of ChAT positive neurons in the vertical diagonal band of the basal forebrain in ovariectomized (Ovx) rats (Acosta et al., 2009b). Cholinergic signals are mediated by muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (mAChR) and nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChR), which are present in brain regions mediating memory (Clarke et al., 1985; Court and Clementi, 1995; Tice et al., 1996; Vaucher et al., 2002) and are implicated in mnemonic processing (Hasselmo, 2006; Konopacki et al., 1992; Rouse et al., 1999). Nicotine administration improves memory in humans (Buccafusco et al., 2005), and we and others have noted nicotine-induced memory enhancement in rats across multiple memory domains (Arendash et al., 1995; French et al., 2006; Riekkinen and Riekkinen, 1997; Socci et al., 1995). 17β-estradiol and ethinyl β-estradiol bind to the α4β2-nAChR, the most abundant nAChR subtype in the brain, and directly potentiate the function of human α4β2-nAChRs (hα4β2-nAChR); ethinyl β-estradiol, but not 17β-estradiol, potentiates rat α4β2-nAChRs (rα4β2-nAChR) (Curtis et al., 2002; Paradiso et al., 2001). Recently, in vivo research has demonstrated that nicotine co-administered with estradiol potentiates visual spatial memory in Ovx rats, beyond that of either compound administered alone (Taylor and Maloney, 2010). Thus, there is a link between estrogens, nAChRs, and memory.
Numerous studies have demonstrated that 17β-estradiol benefits spatial working and reference memory in young Ovx rats (Bimonte and Denenberg, 1999; Daniel et al., 1997; Daniel et al., 2005; El-Bakri et al., 2004; Fader et al., 1998; Feng et al., 2004; Gibbs, 2007; Hruska and Dohanich, 2007; Korol and Kolo, 2002; Luine and Rodriguez, 1994), as well as in middle-aged Ovx rats (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2006; Foster et al., 2003; Markham et al., 2002; Talboom et al., 2008). Premarin® can also benefit cognition in middle-aged Ovx rats, although these effects are dose and task specific (Acosta et al., 2009b; Engler-Chiurazzi et al., 2009; Walf and Frye, 2008). The current study utilized a similar model, the middle-aged Ovx rat, to test the impact of Δ8E1 and equilin on memory and rα4β2-nAChR expression and function. We used a battery of water-escape mazes previously shown to be influenced by age as well as ovarian hormone loss and estrogen replacement (Acosta et al., 2009a; Acosta et al., 2009b; Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2006; Engler-Chiurazzi et al., 2009; Talboom et al., 2008). These mazes evaluate working memory, which is information that needs to be updated and is pertinent for a short time, and reference memory, which is information that remains constant over time (see Jarrard et al., 1984; Jones, 2002). 125I-labeled epibatidine (I-epi) radioligand binding assays were used to evaluate rα4β2-nAChR expression levels in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, and cell culture was used to evaluate whether Δ8E1 and equilin directly altered hα4β2-nAChR function via 86Rb+ efflux experiments. Lastly, several peripheral markers routinely noted to change with 17β-estradiol treatment, including serum luteinizing hormone (LH) levels, vaginal smears, and uterine weights, were assessed.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Subjects were 50 middle-aged (12–13 month old) Fischer-344 female rats born and raised at the National Institute on Aging colony at Harlan Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN). Animals were acclimated for several weeks to the vivarium at Arizona State University, had exposure to food and water ad-lib, and were maintained on a 12-h light/dark cycle at 23°C. Procedures were approved by Arizona State University IACUC and adhered to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and NIH standards.
Hormone manipulation
Ovariectomy, group assignment and hormone dosing
Thirty days after arrival, all rats received Ovx under isoflurane anesthesia. Dorsolateral incisions were made in the skin and peritoneum, and ovaries and tips of uterine horns were ligated and removed. Rats were randomly assigned into a control group receiving vehicle only or groups receiving one of three doses of hormone delivered via osmotic minipump. Specifically, the groups were: Ovx plus vehicle (Veh, n=8), Ovx plus 2.6 µg/day of Δ8E1 (Δ8E1-Low, n=7), Ovx plus 17.5 µg/day of Δ8E1 (Δ8E1-Med, n=7), Ovx plus 35 µg/day of Δ8E1 (Δ8E1-High, n=7), Ovx plus 2.6 µg/day of equilin (Equilin-Low, n=7), Ovx plus 6.25 µg/day of equilin (Equilin-Med, n=7), and Ovx plus 12.5 µg/day of equilin (Equilin-High, n=7). All hormones were purchased from Steraloids Inc. (Newport, RI). Low doses of Δ8E1 and equilin were derived from our studies demonstrating cognitive enhancement using 3.6 µg of Premarin®. Doses were adjusted to account for Δ8E1 and equilin being in its unconjugated form1, and to approximate the rat body weight equivalent of the 0.625 mg dose, which was used in the WHIMS and is what women are most commonly prescribed (Acosta et al., 2009b; Engler-Chiurazzi et al., 2009). The high doses for Δ8E1 and equilin were based on the only other available reports in the rat model using these hormones, wherein Δ8E1 decreased depolarization-induced cardiac synapse norepinephrine release, and equilin elevated vascular reactivity in the mesenteric vascular bed (Eskin et al., 2003; Mark et al., 2007). The medium doses of Δ8E1 and equilin were half of the high doses.
Hormone treatment procedure
Hormone treatment began approximately 16 days after Ovx (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2006), 12 days before behavior testing began. Vehicle only (polyethylene glycol, PEG, average molecular weight of 300, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, Missouri), or Δ8E1 or equilin in vehicle, was administered via Alzet pumps (Model 2004; Durect Corporation, Cupertino, California). The concentration of Δ8E1 or equilin, suspended in PEG, in the pumps was calculated to deliver the low, medium or high dose for each hormone per day. Pumps were prepared the day before surgery and allowed to equilibrate in physiological saline at 37 °C overnight. For pump insertion, animals were anesthetized under vaporized isoflurane anesthesia, an incision was made in the dorsal scruff of the neck, the pump was inserted, and the skin was stapled. Pumps remained in the subjects throughout behavioral testing and until sacrifice (approximately 4 weeks), which provided consistent exposure to their assigned treatment for the duration of behavioral testing, through sacrifice.
Behavior testing
Win-stay DMS plus maze (spatial working and recent memory)
We used the DMS plus maze task to test spatial working and recent memory. The protocol was based on studies showing 17β-estradiol- and Premarin®- induced improvements in Ovx rats (Acosta et al., 2009b; Gibbs, 2000b; Korol and Kolo, 2002; Sandstrom and Williams, 2004). The black plexiglass maze (each arm was 38.1cm × 12.7cm) was filled with water made opaque with black nontoxic paint. The submerged platform was hidden at the end of one of the four arms. The start location varied across trials, and the platform location stayed in the same arm within a day, but semi-randomly changed arms across days. This a place learning task, whereby animals must learn to navigate to a hidden escape platform by using distal spatial cues (Restle, 1957). Since the platform changed position relative to space each day, this task is differentiated from other match-to-sample or match-to-position tasks where animals solve the task using response learning (i.e., making the same turn regardless of the spatial location) (Restle, 1957). Rats received 6 consecutive trials within a daily session, for 7 days, and were given 90 seconds to locate the platform. Once on the platform, the rat remained on it for 15 seconds, followed by placement into a heated cage for a 30 second inter-trial interval. An arm entry was counted when the tip of a rat’s snout reached a mark delineated on the outside of the arm (11 cm into the arm). Entry into any non-platformed arm was counted as an error, and fewer errors are indicative of better performance. At the start of an animal’s daily testing session, it had no information regarding the platform location. Hence, on trial 1, the rat swam until it located the platform. The rat then needed to remember this platform location and return to it on subsequent trials. Working memory performance was evaluated by assessing the ability to navigate to the new platform location on trial 2. Hence, trial 1 was the information trial, trial 2 was the working memory test trial, and trials 3–6 six were recent memory test trials (Acosta et al., 2009b; Engler-Chiurazzi et al., 2009). The dependent variables were the number of initial and repeat errors committed during trial 2 (as a measure of working memory) or trials 3–6 (as a measure of recent memory). Day 1 was considered the training day, as no prior information regarding task demands or platform location was available to the animals. We routinely treat day 1 as a training day on working memory tasks (Acosta et al., 2009a; Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003a; Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003b; Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2004; Bimonte and Denenberg, 1999; Bimonte et al., 2003; Huentelman et al., 2009).
Morris maze (spatial reference memory)
Using a previously published protocol (Markham et al., 2002), three days after DMS plus maze testing concluded, we tested spatial reference memory via the Morris maze, as we and others have shown enhancements due to 17β-estradiol treatment in Ovx rats on this task (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2006; Markham et al., 2002; Talboom et al., 2008). Similar to the DMS plus maze, the Morris maze task assesses place learning; however, in the Morris maze the platform remains in the same spatial location for all days and trials, making it a spatial reference memory task. This can be contrasted with the DMS plus maze, which is a spatial working memory task since the platform is moved to a new spatial location every day. The Morris maze (Morris et al., 1982) consisted of a round tub (188 cm in diameter) filled with water made opaque with black, non-toxic paint. The rat was placed in the maze from any of four locations (North, South, East, or West) and had 60 seconds to locate a submerged hidden escape platform which remained in a fixed location (Northeast quadrant) throughout testing. After 15 seconds on the platform, the rat was placed into its heated cage until the next trial; the inter-trial-interval was 5–8 minutes. For each rat, the testing session consisted of 6 trials/day for 3 days. A video camera recorded each rat, and a tracking system (Ethovision XT 5.1, Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands) analyzed each rat’s path. The dependent measure was swim distance (cm), with less swim distance interpreted as better spatial reference memory performance. To assess possible differences in the learning rates among our treatment groups, data were collapsed into 3-trial blocks so that Treatment × Block interactions could be tested. To assess platform localization, a probe trial was given on an additional trial (trial 7) on the last day of testing, whereby the platform was removed from the maze. For the probe trial, percent of total swim distance (cm) in the target Northeast quadrant (i.e., quadrant that contained the platform on the test trials) as compared the opposite Southwest quadrant, was the dependent measure.
Visible platform (motoric and visual competence)
One day after Morris maze testing, we evaluated motoric and visual competence using the visible platform task. A rectangular tub (39 × 23 in) was filled with clear room temperature water. A black platform (10 cm wide) was positioned 1.5” above the water surface following previously published methods (Hunter et al., 2003). Opaque curtains covered extramaze cues. Animals were given 6 trials in 1 day. The drop off location remained the same across trials, and the platform location for each trial varied semi-randomly. Each rat had 90 seconds to locate the platform, where it remained for 15 seconds before being placed back into its heated cage awaiting the next trial. The inter-trial-interval was 5–8 minutes. Latency (seconds) to reach the platform was the dependent measure.
Evaluation of vaginal smears, uteri, and serum LH
We evaluated the effects of Δ8E1 or equilin treatments on several peripheral markers that are routinely noted to change with 17β-estradiol treatment. To confirm Ovx as well as Δ8E1 and equilin treatments, vaginal smears were taken prior to Ovx and pump surgery, immediately before behavior testing, and the day before sacrifice. Vaginal cytology via smears was classified as proestrus, estrous, metestrus or diestrus (Goldman et al., 2007). Final vaginal smears were stained with eosin Y and hematoxylin (Sigma) for further brightfield microscopic examinationa. At sacrifice, animals were anesthetized with isoflurane. We confirmed complete Ovx via visual inspection of uterine horns, and we evaluated uterine weights (Acosta et al., 2009b). A ventral incision was made in the abdominal region, and the uterus was cut above the junction with the cervix and on the uterine horn below the ligature remaining from Ovx (Ashby et al., 1997). Uteri were trimmed of all visible fat and were immediately weighed to obtain wet weight, which was the dependent measure. Also, at this time, blood was collected via cardiocentesis. Five subjects per treatment group were randomly chosen to obtain serum LH levels. LH was determined at the Core Endocrine Laboratory at Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine by competitive radioimmunoassay using reagents obtained from ALPCO Diagnostics (Salem, NH), based on prior protocols (Roman et al., 2003). The assay uses 125I-labeled rat LH tracer for binding to a highly specific rabbit polyclonal antibody and separation of bound from free trace with an anti-rabbit IgG precipitating antibody. Results are expressed in ng/ml. Lower limit of quantification for the assay is 0.5 ng/ml and within run and between run accuracy averaged 7% and 9% respectively at a concentration of 4.2 ng/ml.
Brain dissection and nAChR agonist binding
Using radioligand binding assays, we investigated whether treatment with Δ8E1 or equilin affected rα4β2-nAChR expression levels.I-epi binding was used to estimate number of β2*- and β4*-nAChRs in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex (Whiteaker et al., 2000; Whiteaker et al., 2008). At sacrifice, brains were rapidly dissected; one subject was excluded due to technical error. Each brain was cut on the coronal plane. Next, the entorhinal cortex (taking a 2- to 3- mm sample ventral to the hippocampus) and the CA1/CA2 region of the hippocampus (dentate gyrus and alveus excluded) were dissected. Dissected tissues were immediately placed in microcentrifuge tubes, frozen on dry ice, and stored at −70 °C until analysis. Hippocampal and entorhinal cortex tissues were separately homogenized in ice-cold 1× Ringer’s buffer with sodium azide (in mM : 144 NaCl, 2 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4, 50 Tris-base; and 0.02 % (w/v) NaN3; pH 7.4; binding buffer). Homogenates were washed three times by centrifugation (12,000 × g; 15 minutes; 4 °C) and rehomogenized into 1× binding buffer and stored, pelleted, at 4 °C. I-epi binding was assessed at 1 nM. Non-specific binding was assessed in the presence of 300 mM carbamylcholine. Specific binding was defined as the difference between total and non-specific binding as fmol per mg of protein; this served as the dependent variable for statistical analyses. Since spatial memory processing is dependent on both the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, and the functioning of these regions is intimately linked (for review see Jarrard, 1993; Zola-Morgan et al., 1994), specific I-epi binding data in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex were analyzed together creating a single measure; the combined hippocampal and entorhinal cortex data will now be referred to as the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex. In the hippocampus/entorhinal cortex the contribution of β4*-nAChR was minimal (data not shown). Accordingly, I-epi binding populations in the hippocampus/entorhinal cortex are considered to represent β2 subunit-containing nAChR, likely α4β2-nAChR (Whiteaker et al., 2000), as such, specific I-epi binding will be referred to as rα4β2-nAChR expression.
86Rb+ efflux for Δ8E1 and equilin effects on hα4β2-nAChR function
Routine culture of SH-EP1 cells
To ascertain whether hormone effects could be due to direct actions on receptor function, we evaluated whether Δ8E1 or equilin exposure directly affected function of heterologously expressed, hα4β2-nAChR assessed using 86Rb+ efflux experiments. SH-EP1 cells stably expressing hα4β2-nAChR were maintained as previously described (Eaton et al., 2003). Briefly, cells were grown in Dulbecco’s modified Eagle’s medium (high glucose, bicarbonate-buffered, with 1 mM sodium pyruvate and 8 mM L-glutamine) supplemented with 10% horse serum, 100 U/ml penicillin, 100 µg/ml streptomycin, and 0.25 µg/ml amphotericin B (all from Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) plus 5% fetal bovine serum (Hyclone, Logan, UT) on 100-mm diameter plates in a humidified atmosphere containing 5% CO2 in air. Positive selection for the human α4 and β2 subunits (in pcDNA3.1/zeo and pcDNA3.1/Hygro, respectively; also from Invitrogen) was maintained by further supplementation of the growth medium with 0.25 mg/ml zeocin (Invitrogen), and 0.4 mg/ml hygromycin B (Calbiochem, San Diego, CA). Cells were maintained at low passage numbers (1–26 from frozen stocks), and passaged weekly by splitting confluent cultures 1/20 – 1/40 to maintain cells in proliferative growth.
86Rb+ efflux assays in the presence of acute hormone
Cells were harvested at confluence from 100-mm plates by mild trypsinization before being resuspended in growth medium and evenly seeded at a density of one confluent 100-mm plate per 24-well plate. Cells were allowed to adhere for a minimum of 6 hours. Medium was removed and replaced with 250 µl/well of medium supplemented with approximately 300,000 cpm of 86Rb+ (PerkinElmer; counted at 40 % efficiency using Cerenkov counting, PerkinElmer Tricarb 1900 LSC). After 4 hours of loading, 86Rb+ efflux was measured using the flip-plate technique (Lukas et al., 2002). Following aspiration of the bulk of 86Rb+ loading medium from each well of the cell plate, each well containing cells was rinsed three times with 2 ml of fresh efflux buffer (130 mM NaCl, 5.4 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 5 mM glucose, and 50 mM HEPES, pH 7.4) to remove extracellular 86Rb+ After removal of residual rinse buffer by aspiration, the flip-plate technique was used again to simultaneously introduce fresh efflux buffer containing drugs of choice at indicated final concentrations from a 24-well “efflux/drug plate” into the wells of the cell plate. In the case of the acute hormone effect experiments, the drugs used consisted of a test agonist (carbamylcholine at 100 µM) in the presence of Δ8E1, equilin, and PEG (95 fM-1 µM), representing concentrations below, at, or above physiological relevant brain levels of 17β-estradiol (see Woolley, 2007). After a 5-minute incubation, the solution was “flipped” back into the efflux/ drug plate, any remaining medium was removed by aspiration. Suspensions in each well were then subjected to Cerenkov counting (PerkinElmer Wallac Micobeta Trilux 1450; 25% efficiency) after placement of inserts (PerkinElmerWallac 1450-109) into each well to minimize cross-talk between wells. Specific 86Rb+ efflux was calculated as efflux above the no-agonist control and expressed as a percentage of efflux evoked by a maximally-effective carbamylcholine (1 mM) control stimulation. Increases or decreases in specific agonist-induced efflux (i.e. efflux above non-specific levels) would be interpreted as direct, nAChR-mediated, acute allosteric agonist or antagonist (respectively) effects. Intrinsic agonist activity of Δ8E1, equilin or PEG was calculated in samples containing the same concentrations tested in the presence of agonist. The amount of specific 86Rb+ efflux, expressed as % of control, in response to log dilutions of Δ8E1, equilin, and PEG served as the dependent measure of hα4β2-nAChR receptor function.
86Rb+ efflux assays after chronic hormone treatment
Cell plating for chronic hormone studies were the same as described previously for assessment of acute hormone effects, with the exception that cells were seeded more thinly into the 24-well plates (one half of a confluent 100-mm plate per six 24-well plates), allowing them to be grown for 48-hours in the presence of either Δ8E1 or equilin (9 aM - 1 µM) or 0.1% PEG and to reach confluence at the end of this period. Effects of chronic hormone exposure on 86Rb+ efflux were assessed as previously described for measurement of acute effects with minor exceptions. Briefly, after the 48-hour hormone pretreatment period, media containing the hormone was removed and replaced fresh media containing 86Rb+ but devoid of hormone for the 4 hour isotope loading period. Cells were rinsed free or extracellular isotope, and hormones at desired concentrations or 0.1% PEG (matching conditions used in the preincubation period) were then simultaneously introduced in fresh efflux buffer with or without carbamylcholine from a 24-well “efflux/drug plate” into the wells of the cell plate. After a 5-minute incubation, the solution was “flipped” back into the efflux/drug plate, any remaining medium was removed by aspiration. The dependent measure was identical to the acute experiment (see above).
Statistical Analyses
For behavior assessments, data were analyzed using an omnibus mixed model ANOVA with Treatment as the between variable, and Days, Block, and/or Trials as the within variable(s), as appropriate for the specific maze test. rα4β2-nAChR expression data were analyzed using an omnibus factorial ANOVA with Treatment and Brain region as between variables. Uterine weight and LH levels were analyzed using an omnibus one-way ANOVA with Treatment as between variable. In all ANOVAs conducted, each hormone type was analyzed separately, so each omnibus ANOVA included the Veh group as well as the Low, Med, and High group of either Δ8E1 or equilin. Since our interest was to determine whether each hormone dose enhanced performance relative to the Veh group, all follow-up comparisons employed Fisher post hoc tests when a significant omnibus ANOVA was found, noting that Type I error correction is not necessary with orthogonal planned comparisons (Keppel and Wickens, 2004). For efflux experiments, multiple regression analyses evaluated if the slope of the “line of best fit” differed from 0, followed by a Runs analysis to determine if the data deviated from linearity.
Results
Win-stay DMS plus maze: spatial working and recent memory
Spatial working memory was assessed using the win-stay DMS plus maze task. Analysis of repeat errors committed on trial 2 alone (the working memory trial) for Δ8E1 revealed a significant main effect of Treatment (F(3,25) = 3.02, p = 0.0405). The Δ8E1-Med and Δ8E1-High groups made fewer repeat errors on trial 2 as compared to the Veh group (ps < 0.0107; Figure 1a). The analysis of repeat errors for trials 3–6 (recent memory trials) revealed a marginal Treatment main effect for Δ8E1 (F(3,25) = 2.45, p = 0.0867). When all DMS test trials were combined (trials 2–6, working and recent memory trials) there was a Treatment main effect for Δ8E1 (F(3,25) = 3.61, p = 0.0272). The Δ8E1 -Med and Δ8E1-High groups made fewer repeat errors for all test trials combined as compared to the Veh group (ps < 0.0107). There were no main effects or interactions for repeat errors for equilin (Figure 1b), nor were there effects for initial errors for either hormone.
Morris maze: spatial reference memory
Animals were tested on the Morris maze to evaluate spatial reference memory. There was a main effect of Treatment (F(3,26) = 6.64, p = 0.0018), in the absence of a Block x Treatment interaction (F(3,26) = 0.21, p = 0.8921). Post-hoc analyses revealed that Δ8E1 -Low, Δ8E1 -Med, and Δ8E1-High groups each swam less distance than the Veh group (ps < 0.0027; Figure 2a). For equilin, there were no significant main effects or interactions (Figure 2b). For the probe trial, each treatment group localized to the Northeast quadrant which previously contained the hidden escape platform, with a higher percent distance in the Northeast quadrant (Quadrant main effect: Δ8E1 F(1,26) = 181.21, p < 0.0001; Figure 2c; Equilin F(1,26) = 116.19, p < 0.0001; Figure 2d). This was in the absence of a Treatment x Quadrant interaction for either hormone, indicating that all groups localized to the previously platformed quadrant.
Visible platform task: motoric and visual competence
Motor and visual competence was assessed using a visible platform task. There were no main effects of Treatment for Δ8E1 or equilin, thereby indicating that animals, regardless of hormone status, were able to locate the visible platform (Δ8E1 F(3,26) = 0.83, p = 0.4908; Figure 3a; Equilin F(3,26) = 0.59, p < 0.6302; Figure 3b). All animals found the platform within 10 seconds on the last two trials, thereby indicating that all animals were able to perform the procedural components of a swim maze task.
nAChR agonist binding: rα4β2-nAChR expression levels
We investigated whether treatment with Δ8E1 or equilin affected nAChR expression levels using a radioligand binding assay. Analysis of Δ8E1 in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex revealed a significant main effect of Treatment (F(3,48) = 4.33, p = 0.0089), with each Δ8E1 group showing decreased ra4p2-nAChR expression as compared to the Veh group (ps ≤ .05; Figure 4a). Moreover, when Δ8E1 -Low, Δ8E1-Med and Δ8E1-High groups were combined into one Δ8E1 group, there was a Treatment main effect (F(1,54) = 5.25, p = 0.0259; Figure 4a). There were also greater rα4β2-nAChR levels in the entorhinal cortex compared to the hippocampus (Brain Region main effect: (F(1,48) = 59.81, p < 0.0001; Figure 4a), and there was a null Treatment x Brain Region interaction indicating this pattern was shown for all Δ8E1 all doses. In the equilin analysis, there was no significant Treatment main effect (F(3,50) = 1.84, p = 0.1517); however, as seen in the Δ8E1 omnibus ANOVA, there were greater rα4β2-nAChR levels in the entorhinal cortex compared to the hippocampus (Brain Region main effect: (F(1,50) = 41.63, p < 0.0001; Figure 4b), in the absence of a Treatment x Brain Region interaction.
Regression: relation between nicotinic receptor agonist binding and memory
Since a decrease in rα4β2-nAChR expression was noted with Δ8E1 treatment in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex, we conducted primary regression analyses relating rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex to maze performance. Specifically, rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex was the predictor variable in the model, and Morris maze swim distance collapsed across all days, repeat DMS plus maze errors on trial 2 alone (collapsed across days 2–7), or repeat DMS plus maze errors on trials 2–6 (collapsed across days 2–7), was the outcome variable. There was a positive relationship between rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex and Morris maze swim distance (b = 0.03, r = 0.64, z(28) = 3.75, p = 0.0002; Figure 5). Since greater swim distance indicates poorer performance, better spatial reference memory performance was associated with less rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex. When the data were centered to control for group membership (for more detailed methods and rationale see Enders and Tofighi, 2007; Hallahan and Rosenthal, 2000), the regression analysis remained significant (b = 7.71, r = 0.44, z(28) = 2.36, p = 0.0182). There were no significant regression analyses relating rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex to working or recent memory repeat errors on the win-stay DMS plus maze (trial 2: b = 0.001, r = 0.06, z(28) = 0.26, p = 0.7829; trials 2–6: b = 0.001, r = 0.15, z(28) = 0.78, p = 0.4365).
Efflux experiments: evaluation of direct estrogenic actions on hα4β2-nAChR function
We evaluated whether Δ8E1 or equilin exposure directly affected the function of hα4β2-nAChRs assessed using 86Rb+ efflux experiments in vitro. For either acute or chronic administration, regression analyses revealed that slopes of the best fit line did not differ from zero (ps > 0.05), and the data did not deviate from linearity (Runs test, all ps > 0.05; Figure 6a, 6b and 6c). PEG did not affect or interact with Δ8E1 or equilin in altering 86Rb+ efflux during acute, or after chronic exposure. This indicates that neither acute nor chronic effects of hormone or vehicle on hα4β2-nAChR function occur at the concentrations tested.
Vaginal smears, uterine weights and serum LH: classic estrogenic actions
We tested peripheral markers that are routinely noted to change with 17β-estradiol treatment. Before Ovx surgeries, all animals demonstrated normal cyclicity. After Ovx (but before hormone treatment), all rats consistently exhibited leukocytic, diestrus smears, as expected. Two days prior to behavior testing, after assigned vehicle or hormone had been administered, all Veh-treated rats presented consistent diestrus smears, whereas hormone-treated rats showed constant cornified, estrous smears demonstrating uterine stimulation. These cytological profiles remained stable through sacrifice (Figure 7). Furthermore, each Δ8E1 group, and each Equilin group, had increased uterine weight compared to the Veh group (Δ8E1 F(3,25) = 13.51., p < 0.0001; Figure 8a; Equilin F(3,25) = 34.96, p < 0.0001; Figure 8b; Fisher ps < 0.0028), and lower LH levels as compared to the Veh group (Δ8E1 F(3,15) = 4.95, p = 0.0139; Figure 8c; Equilin F(3,15) = 8.67, p = 0.0014; Figure 8d; Fisher ps < 0.0143).
Discussion
The present study is the first to evaluate whether Δ8E1 and equilin, two primary components of Premarin® shown to have the most neuroprotective effects in vitro (Brinton et al., 1997; Zemlyak et al., 2002; Zhao and Brinton, 2006), influence cognition. In middle-aged surgically menopausal rats, Δ8E1 treatment enhanced spatial reference, working, and recent memory. In these same animals, Δ8E1 treatment decreased rα4β2-nAChR expression (i.e., specific I-epi binding) in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, which was related to better spatial reference memory performance on the Morris maze. Equilin treatment did not impact spatial memory nor rα4β2-nAChR expression. There were no direct effects on hα4β2-nAChR function with acute or prolonged exposure to Δ8E1, equilin or PEG (vehicle).
Δ8E1 and equilin effects on spatial working and recent memory
The Δ8E1 medium and high doses enhanced working and recent memory performance on the DMS plus maze task, while equilin did not. These Δ8E1-induced spatial working memory enhancements corroborate previous findings whereby 17β-estradiol, and higher doses of Premarin®, benefited spatial working memory in Ovx rats (Acosta et al., 2009b; Bimonte and Denenberg, 1999). It is conceivable that the Δ8E1 medium and high doses facilitated better performance on this task by predisposing these animals to use a place strategy, as this is seen with 17β-estradiol treatment (Korol and Kolo, 2002). In our DMS plus maze task, animals using a response strategy (i.e., turn left or turn right) would have been inefficient at locating the platform, as the start location was varied and the goal arm remained in the same place in space within a day, leading to more errors. We speculate that the use of a place strategy allowed Δ8E1 medium and high dose treated animals to accumulate fewer errors by creating a more stable cognitive map, in part by having a better ability to hold the spatial location of the platform in working memory (Barnes et al., 1997; Wilson et al., 2004).
Δ8E1 and equilin effects on spatial reference memory
We found that all doses of Δ8E1 enhanced spatial reference memory performance. This was evidenced by less overall Morris maze swim distance in the Δ8E1 treated groups as compared to the Vehicle group. Equilin treatment did not affect Morris maze performance. Tonic subcutaneous Δ8E1-induced spatial reference memory enhancements are in accordance with Morris maze improvements shown by others after tonic subcutaneous 17β-estradiol treatment (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2006; Foster et al., 2003; Talboom et al., 2008), and after cyclic subcutaneous Premarin® treatment (Acosta et al., 2009b), in middle-aged Ovx animals. Interestingly, Δ8E1’s spatial reference memory enhancement did not vary within the dose range studied, and the effect was robust. In fact, Δ8E1’s effects noted here are relatively more pronounced as compared to other reports using 17β-estradiol or Premarin® where effects were specific to aiding overnight retention or performance during the first 6 test trials (Acosta et al., 2009b; Markham et al., 2002; Talboom et al., 2008).
It is noteworthy that the Morris maze learning curves and probe trial data suggest that by the last testing day all groups learned the task and localized to the platform quadrant. Further, all animals readily learned the visible platform task with no group differences. That all animals could learn these two tasks suggests that our observed Treatment effects were not likely due to differences in visual competence or motoric ability.
Δ8E1 and equilin effects on α4β2-nAChR expression and function
All doses of Δ8E1 decreased rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, while equilin did not alter rα4β2-nAChR expression in any region. Our hippocampus and entorhinal cortex nAChR data are in agreement with other studies demonstrating that 17β-estradiol treatment decreased the number of nAChRs and mAChRs in the central nervous system of Ovx rodents (Cardoso et al., 2004; El-Bakri et al., 2002), although not all studies show this effect (see Centeno et al., 2006; Lapchak et al., 1990). The current results also indicate that hippocampus+entorhinal cortex rα4β2-nAChR expression has implications for cognition. Indeed, a decrease in rα4β2-nAChR expression in the hippocampus+entorhinal cortex was associated with better spatial reference memory performance (i.e., r = 0.64), representing a large effect size as defined by Cohen (Cohen, 1988).
We performed a follow-up study to assess possible direct actions of Δ8E1and equilin on hα4β2-nAChR, as ethinyl β-estradiol has been found to directly potentiate both hα4β2- and rα4β2-nAChR function (i.e., ion efflux) in vitro (Paradiso et al., 2001). We found that exposure to Δ8E1 or equilin, below, at, or above physiologically relevant brain concentrations of 17β-estradiol (95 fM-1 µM for our acute experiments, and 9 aM-1 µM for our chronic experiments) (see Woolley, 2007), had no detectable effect on function of hα4β2-nAChRs. Previous in vitro studies reporting potentiation of α4β2-nAChRs by estrogens found that in some cases, rα4β2-nAChRs and hα4β2-nAChR do not show comparable responses to the same estrogens, and the concentrations of estrogens used were higher in comparison to the current evaluations (Paradiso et al., 2001). These higher concentrations may not be physiologically relevant in the context of endogenous estrogen levels (Paradiso et al., 2001). This previous report also suggests that the lack of a hydroxyl group at the 17th position on Δ8E1 may prevent it from potentiating hα4β2-nAChR (Paradiso et al., 2001). Taken together with our data indicating the absence of direct Δ8E1 effects on hα4β2-nAChR, we hypothesize that Δ8E1-mediated alterations in rα4β2-nAChRs expression reflect adaptation to Δ8E1-mediated changes via other aspects of the cholinergic system. It is possible that, similar to 17β-estradiol (Marriott and Korol, 2003), Δ8E1 treatment leads to enhanced ACh release in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex which may, in turn, result in reduced expression of α4β2-nAChRs as a compensatory mechanism.
Mediation of Δ8E1’s effects and future directions
Δ8E1 and equilin bind to, and produce biological activity via, both estrogen receptor (ER) subtypes (Bhavnani et al., 2008). It is likely that Δ8E1-mediated spatial memory enhancements are primarily, but not solely, mediated by ER activation, as 17β-estradiol’s place learning enhancement is influenced by hippocampal ER activation (Zurkovsky et al., 2006). Recent evidence also suggests that estrogenic effects are influenced by the putative G-protein coupled membrane-bound ER (ER-X; reviewed in Toran-Allerand, 2005) and/or GPR30, a G-protein coupled receptor that responds rapidly to estrogen (reviewed in Prossnitz et al., 2008). It is possible that Δ8E1’s cognitive effects are mediated in part by these non-classic ERs. In fact, recently, Gibbs and colleagues found that the GPR30 agonist G1 enhanced DMP learning (Hammond et al., 2009). It is also noted that Δ8E1-induced mnemonic enhancements could be related to non-genomic mechanisms as well, as estrogens impart robust non-genomic effects (Prokai and Simpkins, 2007)
Our current experimental design did not allow us to decipher the effects of behavior testing from the effects of hormone treatment on rα4β2-nAChRs expression. Indeed, rα4β2-nAChR expression may have been influenced by behavior testing alone, in a similar manner as that previously noted for hippocampal dendritic spines (see Frick et al., 2004). In addition, the regulation of nicotinic receptor expression by ligands occurs by diverse mechanisms, and these are conserved across different systems (for review see Gaimarri et al., 2007; Govind et al., 2009; Lester et al., 2009). However, the use of a model system allowed us to study hormone effects on a single, well-defined subtype. In our in vitro model system, whole-animal pharmacokinetics and the effects of brain-region differences was an obviated issue. The lack of effect in the model system (where there is no synaptic function), in contrast to the effects in vivo, allows us to conclude that our in vivo effects on 4β2-nAChRs likely reflect a functional adaptation in response to the Δ8E1 treatment, and not a direct effect of the hormones on the receptors themselves. Further studies are necessary to detail the specific contributions of Δ8E1 on cholinergic function and rα4β2-nAChR expression, independent of behavioral testing effects, as well as to fully identify the mechanisms of Δ8E1-mediated mnemonic enhancements both at the cellular and the systems level.
Conclusions
There is increasing evidence that Δ8E1 demonstrates unique properties compared to more widely-studied estrogens such as 17α- and 17β- estradiol (Baracat et al., 1999; Bhavnani, 1998). Data from other laboratories have demonstrated that Δ8E1 has an attenuated toxic potential compared to other Premarin® components (Zhang et al., 2001), shows distinct tissue and cell-specific estrogenic properties in relation to estrone (Baracat et al., 1999), and appears to be converted to only one metabolite in women, giving Δ8E1 a unique pharmacokinetic profile (Bhavnani, 1998). Two studies have evaluated Δ8E1 in women. These works found that Δ8E1 exerted an overall beneficial profile for health-related concerns associated with menopause (Baracat et al., 1999; Bhavnani et al., 1998; for review see Utian et al., 2006). Together with the data presented here, the collected evidence suggests that Δ8E1 is a uniquely beneficial estrogen that warrants further study as a potential nootropic therapy. In summary, Δ8E1 enhanced multiple domains of learning and memory, with a link to the cholinergic system as shown by changes in nicotinic receptor expression. Equilin did not affect spatial memory and had no effects on nicotinic receptor expression. Select components of Premarin® may offer promising new HT options that positively impact brain health during aging. An exciting new direction might be to further define the individual components of Premarin®. This would allow composition of a novel combined estrogen therapy, with the ultimate goal to optimize the potential benefits, and obviate the risks, of currently used estrogen therapies.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by grants awarded to HAB-N from the National Institute on Aging (AG028084), state of Arizona, ADHS, and the Arizona Alzheimer’s Disease Core Center, as well as to PW (National Institute of Drug Abuse Grant DA12242 and the Barrow Neurological Foundation) and to ARS (Canadian Institutes of Health Research post-doctoral fellowship). LP is the Robert A. Welch Professor of the University of North Texas Health Science Center (endowment number BK-0031). We extend our greatest appreciation to Cynthia Zay, Candy Tsang, Bronson Bowman, Brittany Blair Braden, Michael Cosand, Sarah Mennenga, Alexandra Garcia and Madeline Andrews for excellent experimental assistance. We are grateful to Dr. Laurence M. Demers and the Core Endocrinology Laboratory (Pennsylvania State Hershey Medical Center, Hershey, PA) for performing the LH assays.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
The 20–36 µg of Premarin® powder that we have shown to enhance cognition was actually 2.0–3.6 µg of estrogens, respectively, since it was ≈ 10% hormone and ≈ 90% filler (Acosta et al., 2009b; Engler-Chiurazzi et al., 2009). Premarin® is a mixture of estrogens conjugated to sulfate; the hormones must be deconjugated by the liver to become bioactive (Bhavnani et al., 1998). We used bioactive, unconjugated Δ8E1 and equilin in this study. To attain an approximately equivalent amount of estrogen molecules between our current Δ8E1 and equilin low dose, and our previous Premarin dose, we had to account for the fact that estrogens in Premarin are conjugated, and conjugated estrogens weigh more. Since the molecular weights of Δ8E1 sulfate and equilin sulfate (the conjugated estrogens) are each 370.08 (as monosodium salts), while that of Δ8E1 and equilin (as unconjugated estrogens) are each 268.35, the final concentrations for the low doses were reduced by 28% (i.e., 3.6 µg/day - 28% ≈ 2.6 µg/day).
Contributor Information
Joshua S. Talboom, Email: Joshua.Talboom@asu.edu.
Elizabeth B. Engler-Chiurazzi, Email: Eengler@asu.edu.
Paul Whiteaker, Email: Paul.Whiteaker@chw.edu.
Alain R. Simard, Email: Alain.simard@chw.edu.
Ronald Lukas, Email: Ron.Lukas@chw.edu.
Jazmin I. Acosta, Email: Jazmin.acosta@asu.edu.
Laszlo Prokai, Email: Lprokai@hsc.unt.edu.
References
- U.S. Census Bureau Word population information. 2007 Retrieved on April 21. 2007, from http://www.census.gov/ipc/www/world.html.
- Acosta JI, et al. Transitional versus surgical menopause in a rodent model: etiology of ovarian hormone loss impacts memory and the acetylcholine system. Endocrinology. 2009a;150:4248–4259. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-1802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Acosta JI, et al. Premarin improves memory, prevents scopolamine-induced amnesia and increases number of basal forebrain choline acetyltransferase positive cells in middle-aged surgically menopausal rats. Horm Behav. 2009b;55:454–464. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arendash GW, et al. Nicotine enhances the learning and memory of aged rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1995;52:517–523. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00119-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashby J, et al. Activity of raloxifene in immature and ovariectomized rat uterotrophic assays. Regul Toxicol Pharmacol. 1997;25:226–231. doi: 10.1006/rtph.1997.1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baracat E, et al. Estrogen activity and novel tissue selectivity of delta8,9-dehydroestrone sulfate in postmenopausal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1999;84:2020–2027. doi: 10.1210/jcem.84.6.5800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes CA, et al. Multistability of cognitive maps in the hippocampus of old rats. Nature. 1997;388:272–275. doi: 10.1038/40859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhavnani BR. Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of conjugated equine estrogens: chemistry and metabolism. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med. 1998;217:6–16. doi: 10.3181/00379727-217-44199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhavnani BR. Estrogens and menopause: pharmacology of conjugated equine estrogens and their potential role in the prevention of neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2003;85:473–482. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(03)00220-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhavnani BR, et al. Biologic effects of delta-8-estrone sulfate in postmenopausal women. J Soc Gynecol Investig. 1998;5:156–160. doi: 10.1016/s1071-5576(98)00003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhavnani BR, et al. Structure activity relationships and differential interactions and functional activity of various equine estrogens mediated via estrogen receptors (ERs) ERalpha and ERbeta. Endocrinology. 2008;149:4857–4870. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte-Nelson HA, et al. Progesterone reverses the spatial memory enhancements initiated by tonic and cyclic oestrogen therapy in middle-aged ovariectomized female rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:229–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04867.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte-Nelson HA, et al. Frontal cortex BDNF levels correlate with working memory in an animal model of Down syndrome. Behav Brain Res. 2003a;139:47–57. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00082-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte-Nelson HA, et al. Ovarian hormones and cognition in the aged female rat: I. Long-term, but not short-term, ovariectomy enhances spatial performance. Behav Neurosci. 2003b;117:1395–1406. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte-Nelson HA, et al. Ovarian hormones and cognition in the aged female rat: II. progesterone supplementation reverses the cognitive enhancing effects of ovariectomy. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:707–714. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte HA, Denenberg VH. Estradiol facilitates performance as working memory load increases. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24:161–173. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00068-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bimonte HA, et al. Age-related deficits as working memory load increases: relationships with growth factors. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:37–48. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00015-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohacek J, et al. Long-term ovarian hormone deprivation alters the ability of subsequent oestradiol replacement to regulate choline acetyltransferase protein levels in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of middle-aged rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20:1023–1027. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01752.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brinton RD, et al. Equilin, a principal component of the estrogen replacement therapy premarin, increases the growth of cortical neurons via an NMDA receptor-dependent mechanism. Exp Neurol. 1997;147:211–220. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1997.6619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buccafusco JJ, et al. Long-lasting cognitive improvement with nicotinic receptor agonists: mechanisms of pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic discordance. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2005;26:352–360. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell S, Whitehead M. Oestrogen therapy and the menopausal syndrome. Clin Obstet Gynaecol. 1977;4:31–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso CC, et al. Effects of estrogen on muscarinic acetylcholine receptors in the rat hippocampus. Neuroendocrinology. 2004;80:379–386. doi: 10.1159/000084202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centeno ML, et al. Estradiol increases alpha7 nicotinic receptor in serotonergic dorsal raphe and noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons of macaques. J Comp Neurol. 2006;497:489–501. doi: 10.1002/cne.21026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke PB, et al. Nicotinic binding in rat brain: autoradiographic comparison of [3H]acetylcholine, [3H]nicotine, and [125I]-alpha-bungarotoxin. J Neurosci. 1985;5:1307–1315. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-05-01307.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Court J, Clementi F. Distribution of nicotinic subtypes in human brain. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1995;(9 Suppl 2):6–14. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199501002-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis L, et al. Potentiation of human alpha4beta2 neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptor by estradiol. Mol Pharmacol. 2002;61:127–135. doi: 10.1124/mol.61.1.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel JM, et al. Estrogen enhances performance of female rats during acquisition of a radial arm maze. Horm Behav. 1997;32:217–225. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1997.1433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel JM, et al. Role of hippocampal M2 muscarinic receptors in the estrogen-induced enhancement of working memory. Neuroscience. 2005;132:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton JB, et al. Characterization of human alpha 4 beta 2-nicotinic acetylcholine receptors stably and heterologously expressed in native nicotinic receptor-null SH-EP1 human epithelial cells. Mol Pharmacol. 2003;64:1283–1294. doi: 10.1124/mol.64.6.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Bakri NK, et al. Estrogen and progesterone treatment: effects on muscarinic M(4) receptor subtype in the rat brain. Brain Res. 2002;948:131–137. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02962-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Bakri NK, et al. Effects of estrogen and progesterone treatment on rat hippocampal NMDA receptors: relationship to Morris water maze performance. J Cell Mol Med. 2004;8:537–544. doi: 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2004.tb00478.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engler-Chiurazzi E, et al. Tonic Premarin dose-dependently enhances memory, affects neurotrophin protein levels and alters gene expression in middle-aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskin BA, et al. Cardiac norepinephrine release: modulation by ovariectomy and estrogen. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 2003;228:194–199. doi: 10.1177/153537020322800210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fader AJ, et al. Estrogen improves performance of reinforced T-maze alternation and prevents the amnestic effects of scopolamine administered systemically or intrahippocampally. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69:225–240. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng Z, et al. Long-term effects of melatonin or 17 beta-estradiol on improving spatial memory performance in cognitively impaired, ovariectomized adult rats. J Pineal Res. 2004;37:198–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-079X.2004.00158.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster TC, et al. Interaction of age and chronic estradiol replacement on memory and markers of brain aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:839–852. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00014-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French KL, et al. Chronic nicotine improves working and reference memory performance and reduces hippocampal NGF in aged female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;169:256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick KM, et al. Behavioral training interferes with the ability of gonadal hormones to increase CA1 spine synapse density in ovariectomized female rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:3026–3032. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03427.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaimarri A, et al. Regulation of neuronal nicotinic receptor traffic and expression. Brain Res Rev. 2007;55:134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RB. Effects of estrogen on basal forebrain cholinergic neurons vary as a function of dose and duration of treatment. Brain Res. 1997;757:10–16. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(96)01432-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RB. Effects of gonadal hormone replacement on measures of basal forebrain cholinergic function. Neuroscience. 2000a;101:931–938. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00433-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RB. Long-term treatment with estrogen and progesterone enhances acquisition of a spatial memory task by ovariectomized aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2000b;21:107–116. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00103-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RB. Estradiol enhances DMP acquisition via a mechanism not mediated by turning strategy but which requires intact basal forebrain cholinergic projections. Horm Behav. 2007;52:352–359. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman JM, et al. The rodent estrous cycle: characterization of vaginal cytology and its utility in toxicological studies. Birth Defects Res B Dev Reprod Toxicol. 2007;80:84–97. doi: 10.1002/bdrb.20106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Govind AP, et al. Nicotine-induced upregulation of nicotinic receptors: underlying mechanisms and relevance to nicotine addiction. Biochem Pharmacol. 2009;78:756–765. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2009.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallahan M, Rosenthal R. Interpreting and reporting results. In: Tinsley HEA, Brown SD, editors. Handbook of multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000. pp. 183–208. [Google Scholar]
- Hammond R, et al. Chronic treatment with estrogen receptor agonists restores acquisition of a spatial learning task in young ovariectomized rats. Horm Behav. 2009;56:309–314. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselmo ME. The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:710–715. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hersh AL, et al. National use of postmenopausal hormone therapy: annual trends and response to recent evidence. JAMA. 2004;291:47–53. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hruska Z, Dohanich GP. The effects of chronic estradiol treatment on working memory deficits induced by combined infusion of beta-amyloid (1–42) and ibotenic acid. Horm Behav. 2007;52:297–306. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huentelman MJ, et al. Peripheral delivery of a ROCK inhibitor improves learning and working memory. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:218–223. doi: 10.1037/a0014260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter CL, et al. Behavioral comparison of 4 and 6 month-old Ts65Dn mice: age-related impairments in working and reference memory. Behav Brain Res. 2003;138:121–131. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00275-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrard LE. On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behav Neural Biol. 1993;60:9–26. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90664-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrard LE, et al. On the role of hippocampal connections in the performance of place and cue tasks: comparisons with damage to hippocampus. Behav Neurosci. 1984;98:946–954. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.98.6.946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones MW. A comparative review of rodent prefrontal cortex and working memory. Curr Mol Med. 2002;2:639–647. doi: 10.2174/1566524023361989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kantor HI, et al. Estrogen for older women. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1973;116:115–118. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(73)90894-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keppel G, Wickens TD. Design and analysis : a researcher's handbook. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Konopacki J, et al. Carbachol-induced theta-like activity in entorhinal cortex slices. Brain Res. 1992;572:76–80. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90453-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korol DL, Kolo LL. Estrogen-induced changes in place and response learning in young adult female rats. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:411–420. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lapchak PA, et al. Chronic estradiol treatment alters central cholinergic function in the female rat: effect on choline acetyltransferase activity, acetylcholine content, and nicotinic autoreceptor function. Brain Res. 1990;525:249–255. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90871-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lester HA, et al. Nicotine is a selective pharmacological chaperone of acetylcholine receptor number and stoichiometry. Implications for drug discovery. AAPS J. 2009;11:167–177. doi: 10.1208/s12248-009-9090-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luine V, Rodriguez M. Effects of estradiol on radial arm maze performance of young and aged rats. Behav Neural Biol. 1994;62:230–236. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80021-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas RJ, et al. Some methods for studies of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor pharmacology. In: Levin ED, editor. Nicotinic Receptors and the Nervous System. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2002. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- Mark CJ, et al. Effects of estrogens and selective estrogen receptor modulators on vascular reactivity in the perfused mesenteric vascular bed. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2007;293:R1969–R1975. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00260.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markham JA, et al. Ovarian hormone replacement to aged ovariectomized female rats benefits acquisition of the morris water maze. Horm Behav. 2002;42:284–293. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markowska AL, Savonenko AV. Effectiveness of estrogen replacement in restoration of cognitive function after long-term estrogen replacement withdrawal in aging rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:10985–10995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10985.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marriott LK, Korol DL. Short-term estrogen treatment in ovariectomized rats augments hippocampal acetylcholine release during place learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2003;80:315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer P, et al. Steady-state pharmacokinetics of conjugated equine estrogens in healthy, postmenopausal women. J Reprod Med. 2008;53:97–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RG, et al. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982;297:681–683. doi: 10.1038/297681a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nappi RE, et al. Memory functioning at menopause: impact of age in ovariectomized women. Gynecol Obstet Invest. 1999;47:29–36. doi: 10.1159/000010058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohkura T, et al. Long-term estrogen replacement therapy in female patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type: 7 case reports. Dementia. 1995;6:99–107. doi: 10.1159/000106929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, Teather LA. Posttraining estradiol injections enhance memory in ovariectomized rats: cholinergic blockade and synergism. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68:172–188. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradiso K, et al. The C terminus of the human nicotinic alpha4beta2 receptor forms a binding site required for potentiation by an estrogenic steroid. J Neurosci. 2001;21:6561–6568. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-17-06561.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Effects of estrogen on memory function in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1992;17:485–495. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90007-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prokai L, Simpkins JW. Structure-nongenomic neuroprotection relationship of estrogens and estrogen-derived compounds. Pharmacol Ther. 2007;114:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2007.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prossnitz ER, et al. Estrogen signaling through the transmembrane G protein-coupled receptor GPR30. Annu Rev Physiol. 2008;70:165–190. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.70.113006.100518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapp SR, et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289:2663–2672. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnick SM, et al. Postmenopausal hormone therapy and regional brain volumes: the WHIMS-MRI Study. Neurology. 2009;72:135–142. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000339037.76336.cf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Restle F. Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. Psychol Rev. 1957;64:217–228. doi: 10.1037/h0040678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riekkinen M, Riekkinen P., Jr Nicotine and D-cycloserine enhance acquisition of water maze spatial navigation in aged rats. Neuroreport. 1997;8:699–703. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199702100-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers SP, et al. Transient estradiol exposure during middle age in ovariectomized rats exerts lasting effects on cognitive function and the hippocampus. Endocrinology. 2010;151:1194–1203. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roman O, et al. Daily profiles of plasma prolactin (PRL), growth hormone (GH), insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1), luteinizing hormone (LH), testosterone, and melatonin, and of pituitary PRL mRNA and GH mRNA in male Long Evans rats in acute phase of adjuvant arthritis. Chronobiol Int. 2003;20:823–836. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120021085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouse ST, et al. Muscarinic receptor subtypes involved in hippocampal circuits. Life Sci. 1999;64:501–509. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(98)00594-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandstrom NJ, Williams CL. Spatial memory retention is enhanced by acute and continuous estradiol replacement. Horm Behav. 2004;45:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwin BB. Estrogen and/or androgen replacement therapy and cognitive functioning in surgically menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1988;13:345–357. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(88)90060-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwin BB. Estrogen and memory in women: how can we reconcile the findings? Horm Behav. 2005;47:371–375. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shumaker SA, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004;291:2947–2958. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shumaker SA, et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289:2651–2662. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sitruk-Ware R. Hormonal replacement therapy. Rev Endocr Metab Disord. 2002;3:243–256. doi: 10.1023/a:1020028510797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Socci DJ, et al. Nicotine enhances Morris water maze performance of young and aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16:857–860. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00091-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanick ML. Estrogens and progestins: background and history, trends in use, and guidelines and regimens approved by the US Food and Drug Administration. Am J Med. 2005;(118 Suppl 12B):64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2005.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talboom JS, et al. Higher levels of estradiol replacement correlate with better spatial memory in surgically menopausal young and middle-aged rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor GT, Maloney S. Gender differences and the role of estrogen in cognitive enhancements with nicotine in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2010;95:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tice MA, et al. Distribution of muscarinic receptor subtypes in rat brain from postnatal to old age. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1996;92:70–76. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(95)01515-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toran-Allerand CD. Estrogen and the brain: beyond ER-alpha, ER-beta, and 17beta-estradiol. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005;1052:136–144. doi: 10.1196/annals.1347.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Utian WH, et al. The Clinical Significance of Δ8,9-Dehydroestrone Sulfate in Treating Menopausal Vasomotor Symptoms. A supplement to OBG Management. 2006:8. [Google Scholar]
- Vaucher E, et al. Estrogen effects on object memory and cholinergic receptors in young and old female mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2002;23:87–95. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00250-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walf AA, Frye CA. Conjugated equine estrogen enhances rats' cognitive, anxiety, and social behavior. Neuroreport. 2008;19:789–792. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fe209c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteaker P, et al. Identification of a novel nicotinic binding site in mouse brain using [(125)I]-epibatidine. Br J Pharmacol. 2000;131:729–739. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0703616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteaker P, et al. Synthesis and characterization of 125I-alpha-conotoxin ArIB[V11L;V16A], a selective alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2008;325:910–919. doi: 10.1124/jpet.108.136895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson IA, et al. Cognitive aging and the hippocampus: how old rats represent new environments. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3870–3878. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5205-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolley CS. Acute effects of estrogen on neuronal physiology. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2007;47:657–680. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.47.120505.105219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zemlyak I, et al. Protection against gp120-induced neurotoxicity by an array of estrogenic steroids. Brain Res. 2002;958:272–276. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03558-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang F, et al. Synthesis and reactivity of the catechol metabolites from the equine estrogen, 8,9-dehydroestrone. Chem Res Toxicol. 2001;14:754–763. doi: 10.1021/tx010049y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao L, Brinton RD. Select estrogens within the complex formulation of conjugated equine estrogens (Premarin) are protective against neurodegenerative insults: implications for a composition of estrogen therapy to promote neuronal function and prevent Alzheimer's disease. BMC Neurosci. 2006;7:24. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zola-Morgan S, et al. Severity of memory impairment in monkeys as a function of locus and extent of damage within the medial temporal lobe memory system. Hippocampus. 1994;4:483–495. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zurkovsky L, et al. Estrogen modulates place learning through estrogen receptors in the hippocampus. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2006;86:336–343. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]