Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Infant Child Dev. 2011 Jan 1;20(1):35–46. doi: 10.1002/icd.685

Sleep and Infant Learning

Amanda R Tarullo 1, Peter D Balsam 1,2, William P Fifer 1,3
PMCID: PMC3034475  NIHMSID: NIHMS212760  PMID: 21311602

Abstract

Human neonates spend the majority of their time sleeping. Despite the limited waking hours available for environmental exploration, the first few months of life are a time of rapid learning about the environment. The organization of neonate sleep differs qualitatively from adult sleep, and the unique characteristics of neonatal sleep may promote learning. Sleep contributes to infant learning in multiple ways. First, sleep facilitates neural maturation, thereby preparing infants to process and explore the environment in increasingly sophisticated ways. Second, sleep plays a role in memory consolidation of material presented while the infant was awake. Finally, emerging evidence indicates that infants process sensory stimuli and learn about contingencies in their environment even while asleep. As infants make the transition from reflexive to cortically mediated control, learned responses to physiological challenges during sleep may be critical adaptations to promote infant survival.

Keywords: Sleep, Infant, Classical Conditioning, Associative Learning, Sleep States, SIDS

Characteristics of Infant Sleep

Neonates sleep at least 16-18 hours per day, and their sleep patterns are markedly different from the sleep patterns of older infants and adults (Anders, Sadeh, & Appareddy, 1995; Gertner, Greenbaum, Sadeh, Dolfin, Sirota, & Ben-Nun, 2002). Neonatal behavioral states are not limited simply to wakefulness versus sleep: Within sleep epochs, distinct states also can be observed, which have characteristic patterns of respiration, heart rate, electroencephalographic (EEG) activity, eye movements, and muscle activity (Prechtl, 1977). In seminal work, Prechtl (1974) described behavioral states in the newborn. State 1 by Prechtl's designation, quiet sleep, is characterized by regular respiration, slow and regular heart rate, and the absence of eye movements and gross muscle movements, with the exception of transient movements and heart rate increases if the infant startles (Prechtl, 1974). Neonates in quiet sleep show an EEG pattern known as tracé alternant, defined by bursts of high amplitude slow wave activity interspersed with low voltage activity (Prechtl, 1974). While older infants also exhibit a quiet sleep state, the tracé alternant disappears by 46-48 weeks postconceptional age (PCA; Mirmiran, Maas, & Ariagno, 2003). Prechtl's State 2, active sleep, is characterized by variable respiration and heart rate and the presence of both slow and rapid eye movements, along with continuous EEG activity of mixed amplitude (Mirmiran et al., 2003; Prechtl, 1974). To meet criteria, a behavioral state must persist for several minutes without interruption (Prechtl & O'Brien, 1982). When sleeping infants do not show the sustained, specific pattern of physiological and behavioral activity characteristic of either quiet or active sleep, they are said to be in indeterminate sleep. Quiet and active sleep states can be distinguished by 30 weeks PCA in both fetuses and preterm infants, though indeterminate sleep predominates during this developmental period (Czikk, Sweeley, Homan, Milley, & Richardson, 2002; Mirmiran et al., 2003; Richardson, Caetano, Homan, & Carmichael, 1994). The emergence of quiet and active sleep states provides an early indication of neural maturation (Mirmiran et al., 2003). Compared to adults and older infants, newborns spend much more time in active sleep, amounting to over half of their total sleep time (Anders et al., 1995).

The first year of life is characterized by a number of developmental changes in sleep patterns, including an increase in quiet sleep (Fagioli & Salzarulo, 1982; Ficca, Fagioli & Salzarulo, 2000; Louis, Cannard, Bastuji, & Challamel, 1997; Mirmiran et al., 2003); increase in the number of sleep cycles and total sleep cycle length (Ficca, Fagioli, et al., 2000); decrease in active sleep (Coons & Guilleminault, 1982; Fagioli & Salzarulo, 1982; Louis et al., 1997; Mirmiran et al., 2003), decrease in indeterminate sleep (Fagioli & Salzarulo, 1982; Ficca, Fagioli, et al., 2000; Louis et al., 1997; Mirmiran et al., 2003), decrease in total sleep time (Louis et al., 1997); development of a clear circadian rhythm (Coons & Guilleminault, 1982; Louis et al., 1997; Mirmiran et al., 2003); and a changeover to the adult-like pattern of quiet sleep being more prevalent at the beginning of nighttime sleep (Fagioli & Salzarulo, 1982; Hoppenbrouwers, Hodgman, Harper, & Sterman, 1982). Sleep patterns are initially fragmented in human neonates, with frequent transitions not only among sleep states but also between sleeping and waking. In the first few months after birth, as the forebrain matures and exerts greater control over brainstem and cortical regions to organize sleep-wake rhythms, sleep episodes become longer and more continuous (Mirmiran et al., 2003). This developmental pattern of the consolidation of sleep episodes, first described by Coons and Guilleminault (1982), has also been observed in rats (Blumberg, Seelke, Lowen, & Karlsson, 2005). In humans, sleep spindle activity, which is thought to relate to the development of thalamo-cortical circuits, increases dramatically in early infancy and by six months of age comes to resemble adult sleep spindle activity (Louis, Zhang, Revol, Debilly, & Challamel, 1992). Sleep spindle activity is associated with hyperpolarization and synchronization of thalamo-cortical neurons (Dijk, 1995), and it has been suggested that the increase in sleep spindle activity in the first few months of life reflects the development of these thalamo-cortical circuits (Louis et al., 1992). These changes in sleep patterns from the fetal period through late infancy have been attributed to maturation of multiple neural networks (Scher, Steppe, Dahl, Asthana, & Guthrie, 1992).

Sleep promotes brain development

Sleep also may play a dynamic role in brain development. Active sleep may facilitate neural processes such as synapse formation and pruning by providing endogenous stimulation to the brain (Denenberg & Thoman, 1981; Mirmiran & Ariagno, 2003; Roffwarg, Muzio, & Dement, 1966). The high prevalence of active sleep in early infancy, a period of rapid brain development, seems consistent with this hypothesis. Evidence that active sleep is important for brain development comes from the animal literature, in which rats deprived of REM sleep during the neonatal period had reduced cerebral cortex and brainstem volume and alterations in neurotransmitter sensitivity compared to typically developing rats (Mirmiran, Feenstra, Dijcks, Bos, & Van Haaren, 1988; Mirmiran, Uylings, & Corner, 1983). Comparative phylogenetic research indicates that across mammalian species, REM sleep duration was associated with brain volume relative to body mass, which can be taken as a very rough indicator of cognitive complexity (Lesku, Roth, Amlaner, & Lima, 2006). Extrapolating from these findings at the species level, it is possible that REM sleep duration also could contribute to neurodevelopmental differences between individuals of the same species.

REM sleep in infancy appears to set the stage for later learning. Rats deprived of REM sleep during the neonatal period showed no neural plasticity when placed in an enriched environment as adults (Mirmiran et al., 1983). In humans, this early influence of sleep patterns on brain development may predict individual differences in later cognitive function. For example, infants who had more mature patterns of sleep in the neonatal period, including higher nighttime activity level and lower total sleep time, obtained higher scores on the Bayley Mental Development Index when tested at 6 months of age (Gertner et al., 2002).

Role of Sleep in Plasticity, Learning, and Memory Consolidation

Sleep deprivation studies provide evidence that both REM and NREM sleep influence experience-dependent neural plasticity mechanisms. REM sleep deprivation prolongs the critical period of synaptic plasticity in the rat visual cortex and delays the development of synaptic plasticity in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the hypothalamus (Shaffery, Lopez, Bissette, & Roffwarg, 2006; Hogan, Howard, Roffwarg, & Shaffery, 2001). Thus, REM sleep not only is involved in neural maturation but also influences synaptic plasticity in response to visual input. While this phenomenon has been studied most thoroughly in the developing visual system, REM sleep has excitatory inputs throughout the brain, so it may influence plasticity in other brain regions as well (Peirano & Algarin, 2007). NREM sleep also is thought to play a role in synaptic remodeling, specifically through reactivation of neural activity patterns that occurred during wakefulness (Bear & Malenka, 1994; Kavanau, 1994; Kudrimoti, Barnes, & McNaughton, 1999; Peirano & Algarin, 2007). Synaptic remodeling of the visual cortex in response to monocular deprivation was enhanced by NREM sleep (Frank, Issa, Stryker, & Keck, 2001).

Sleep plays an important role in memory consolidation processes, both in developing animals (e.g. Deregnaucourt, Mitra, Feher, Pytte, & Tchernichovski, 2005) and in adult humans (Fenn, Nusbaum, & Margoliash, 2003; Stickgold & Walker, 2005; Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004; Walker, Brakefield, Hobson, & Stickgold 2003). Adults tested after an intervening period of sleep had enhanced ability to abstract a rule from previously learned materials, leading Wagner et al. (2004) to suggest that neural restructuring that occurs during sleep could facilitate insight. In adults, the effective consolidation of different types of memory may be sleep state dependent. Plihal & Born (1999) found that REM sleep and slow-wave sleep were associated with the consolidation of non-declarative and declarative memory, respectively. Visual memory consolidation appears to be dependent both on REM sleep (Karni, Tanne, Rubenstein, Askenasy, & Sagi, 1994) and on slow-wave sleep (Gais, Plihal, Wagner, & Born, 2000). Stage 2 NREM sleep has been specifically identified as important in the consolidation of motor memory (Smith & MacNeill, 1994; Walker, Brakefield, Morgan, Hobson, & Stickgold, 2002). Sleep spindle activity originating in the hippocampus during slow-wave sleep may serve the function of memory consolidation by transmitting information from the hippocampus to the neocortex (Buzsaki, 1996; Gais, Molle, Helms, & Born, 2002; Sirota, Csicsvari, Buhl, & Buzsaki, 2003). Capellini et al. (2009) further posit that following the transfer of information from the hippocampus and amygdala to the neocortex during NREM sleep, this information then is integrated into neocortical networks during REM sleep. Relatedly, in adults, consolidation of declarative memory was related to duration of NREM-REM sleep cycles (Mazzoni et al., 1999) and was impaired when sleep cycles were interrupted (Ficca, Lombardo, Rossi, & Salzarulo, 2000). While conclusions about infant sleep cannot be extrapolated from adult studies, it would be an interesting research question to examine whether the normative developmental increase during infancy in the number and duration of sleep cycles (Ficca, Fagioli, et al., 2000) corresponds to improved consolidation of different types of memories during sleep.

The role of sleep in infant memory consolidation has yet to be comprehensively examined. A recent study exposed 15-month-old infants to an artificial language and manipulating whether or not they napped between the learning phase and the test phase (Gomez, Bootzin, & Nadel, 2006). Infants in both groups demonstrated memory for word pairs identical to those presented in the learning phase, but only infants who napped showed that they had learned the grammatical rules of the language and could recognize those rules in novel word strings. Sleep therefore appeared to facilitate abstracting knowledge about environmental contingencies from stimuli presented during wakefulness (Gomez et al., 2006).

Learning, Memory, and Neonatal Sleep Patterns

Newborn infants spend up to 70% their time asleep (So, Adamson, & Horne, 2007) and are able to maintain wakefulness only for brief intervals (Anders & Roffwarg, 1973; Peirano & Algarin, 2007). One consequence of this fact is that most awake experiences will be followed shortly thereafter by a period of sleep, so any learning that takes place while awake is likely to benefit from the memory facilitation produced by sleep. Perhaps even more intriguing is the possibility that infants have the capacity to learn new information while asleep. Notably, in adults, there have been no conclusive demonstrations of an ability to learn during sleep. Two studies with small samples of adults have reported some indication of classical conditioning during sleep (Beh & Barratt, 1965; Ikeda & Morotoni, 1996). However, research on adult learning during sleep is scant, and other researchers have contended that adults are not capable of learning during sleep (Aarons, 1976). In any event, it is problematic to compare “learning during sleep” for neonates versus adults because of the qualitative differences between neonate and adult sleep. The greater plasticity of the neonate brain may enable learning in circumstances that would not suffice for adult learning (Hensch, 2004).

Even while asleep, infants are surrounded by environmental contingencies, often spanning multiple sensory modalities. For example, a decrease in warmth reliably follows the removal of a swaddling blanket. Might neonates be able to learn about the environmental contingencies they experience during sleep, in addition to those experienced during their fleeting periods of wakefulness? This question is of great interest to SIDS researchers because of its implications for infant survival. Some of the situations infants encounter during sleep, such as respiratory occlusion and thermal challenges, require a response on the part of the infant, and infants already at risk for SIDS who have difficulty learning to respond adaptively may be especially vulnerable. Before exploring whether infants are capable of learning an association between two sensory stimuli during sleep, it is necessary to establish 1) that newborns are capable of learning and 2) that they are capable of processing new information during sleep.

Neonatal Learning

From the first days of life, awake infants are capable of classical conditioning and operant learning (Lipsitt, 1998; Rovee-Collier & Lipsitt 1982; Sullivan et al., 1991; Watson & Rayner 1920). It is possible to condition both positive approach behaviors (e.g. rooting, sucking, or smiling) and avoidance/escape behaviors by selectively reinforcing existing reflexes in waking neonates (Lipsitt, 1998). For example, awake human neonates can learn to alter sucking behavior to obtain a variety of reinforcers including milk (Papousek, 1961), a sweet tasting solution (Siqueland & Lipsitt, 1966), and the sound of their mothers' voice (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). In another example of cross-sensory associative learning in awake neonates, Sullivan et al. (1991) paired a citrus odor with tactile stimulation. The following day, the neonates produced the conditioned response – a head turn – in response to the citrus odor. This learned response was observed regardless of whether the neonates were awake or asleep at the time of testing (Sullivan et al., 1991). This study demonstrates a number of impressive competencies in the human neonate: to learn an association between two stimuli across sensory modalities, remember that association over a 24 hour period, and generalize the learned response to occur during sleep as well as wakefulness. It is easy to see how such competencies might be adaptive – such as learning to recognize and orient toward the mother's odor to facilitate suckling.

Neonates process information while asleep

Newborn infants are in fact capable of processing information during sleep. For example, they actively process auditory stimuli while asleep (Cheour et al., 2002; deRegnier et al., 2000; Sambeth et al., 2008). Neonate event related potentials (ERPs) differed in response to their mother's voice versus a stranger's voice, with the ERP to the stranger's voice showing the negative slow wave that is a characteristic response to novelty (deRegnier et al., 2000). Thus, the infants were able to recognize a familiar stimulus, their mother's voice, and differentiate it from a novel stimulus. Neonates also are able to learn to recognize an auditory stimulus during sleep in a mismatched negativity paradigm, in which a deviant tone is occasionally interspersed with a repeated standard tone (Cheour et al., 2002). Their ERPs to the deviant tone resemble the response observed in waking adults, indicating that they have learned during sleep to habituate to the standard tone and to distinguish it from the deviant tone. Thus, even during sleep, neonates are processing sensory inputs and representing specific aspects of their environments.

Nenonates can learn during sleep

A recent study demonstrated learning in neonates when information was presented only during sleep (Fifer et al., 2009). Sleeping neonates were presented with a delay eyeblink conditioning paradigm pairing a tone with an air puff, and responses were assessed using electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings. The neonates increased their rate of conditioned eye movements in response to the tone alone, providing evidence of their capacity to learn associations while asleep. Moreover, in frontal brain regions, there was a change in evoked response potentials (ERPs) to the tone over the course of the conditioning session, with an increasingly positive slow wave response, suggesting an adaptation in cognitive processing of the conditioned stimulus.

Sleep State May Influence Learning

Several studies of classical conditioning in sleeping rats suggest that the efficacy of associative learning paradigms may vary according to sleep state. Rats that underwent a classical conditioning paradigm during REM sleep were able to learn a conditioned response, as assessed by neural responding in the hippocampus and amygdala to the conditioned stimulus (Hennevin, Hars, Maho, & Block, 1995). They also transferred the conditioned response to the awake state, engaging in avoidance behaviors in response to the conditioned stimulus. Interestingly, this learning was state dependent – while associative learning occurred in both awake and REM sleep states and generalized between these two states, no new associative learning occurred during NREM sleep, nor did previously acquired conditioned responses transfer to the NREM sleep state (Hennevin et al., 1995; Maho & Bloch, 1992). The findings from the animal literature raise the possibility that associative learning during sleep could be state dependent for human infants as well.

It will be important for future studies of infant associative learning during sleep to classify sleep states and the levels within them more thoroughly using cardio-respiratory, electroencephalographic, and myographic measures together. Neonates often alternate between sleep states fairly frequently, and a significant portion of their sleep is still of indeterminate state (not classifiable as either quiet or active sleep). Thus, it is likely that in one conditioning session a given infant might spend time in quiet, active, and indeterminate sleep. Nonetheless, identifying the predominant sleep state may help to account for individual differences in learning among infants of the same PCA. If human neonates are found to learn associations more readily in active as opposed to quiet sleep state, that would suggest an additional evolutionary function of the predominance of active sleep in early infancy. Spending more time in active sleep not only provides sources of endogenous stimulation to facilitate neural maturation, but also might benefit neonates by offering more opportunities to master environmental contingencies and to develop adaptive responses to those contingencies.

Learning during Sleep: Importance for Survival

Regardless of underlying mechanisms, there is clear adaptive value to learning during sleep. Infant survival is threatened during sleep, and the ability to learn escape behaviors or postural adjustments rapidly in response to cardio-respiratory challenges may be critically important (Mitchell, Williams, & Taylor, 1999). Neonates necessarily are inexperienced with prone sleep, as there is no opportunity for this behavior in the womb. Reliable contingencies are present in the sleep environment: certain sleep positions and tactile cues can co-occur with respiratory and thermal challenges. Relief from these conditions can be associated with adaptive postural adjustments or arousal (Lipsitt, 1982; Paluzynska, Harris, & Thach, 2004). For example, the neonate is aided by an innate, brainstem-mediated sleep startle reflex of head lifting in reaction to respiratory occlusion (McGraw, 1963; Paluszynska et al., 2004). Gunther (1955) characterized the neonate response to respiratory occlusion as a fixed action pattern. The reflexive head movements would lead to increased air flow, so they would be reinforced, thereby increasing the probability that this behavior would be repeated promptly in the future, even in response to milder respiratory occlusion (Gunther, 1955, Lipsitt, 1998). Infants born with a weaker or deficient respiratory occlusion reflex, or those with deficits in the neural pathways required for associative learning of an adaptive arousal response, may be particularly susceptible during this already vulnerable period (Burns & Lipsitt, 1991; Lipsitt, 1998). Recent SIDS research suggests that respiratory-related challenges are only one of many requiring physiological responses during infant sleep (Sahni, Schulze, Kashyap, Ohira-Kist, Fifer, & Myers, 2005). Changes in temperature, blood pressure, and heart rate constantly require adjustments during sleep and transitions to different states.

Consistent with the view that learning plays a key role in adjustment to challenges, developing rat pups exposed to brief episodes of hypoxia during quiet sleep had increased latency to arousal over repeated trials, indicating habituation (Darnall, Williams, & Schneider, 2008). When arousal was rewarded with increased air flow, simulating the result of an adaptive response, habituation to hypoxia was no longer evident. Thus, the association of an infant response with escape from hypoxia not only reinforces that response but also prevents habituation to hypoxia. Neonatal rats exposed to repeated episodes of hypoxia during sleep exhibit a persistent increase in the rate of sleep apneas, indicating an experience-dependent effect on respiratory control mechanisms during sleep (Thomas, Friedman, MacKenzie, and Strohl, 1995). Human infants who, like Darnell et al.'s rat pups, previously have experienced intermittent hypoxia and have failed to mount an effective response may be more vulnerable to arousal failure.

Although there are myriad risk factors associated with the incidence of SIDS, age dependency is the hallmark of the syndrome. Converging data from a number of related fields support the characterization of the high risk period as one of marked homeostatic instability (Kinney, Filiano, Sleeper, Mandell, Valdes-Dapena, & White, 1995). Complex sleep dependent developmental changes are evident in a wide range of systems including control of respiration (Haddad & Donnelly, 1988; Hoppenbrouwers, Hodgman, Harper, & Sterman, 1980), periodic breathing (Kelly, Carley, & Shannon, 1988), and thermal regulation (Azaz et al., 1992). During this same period of vulnerability, infants have difficulty mounting a robust response to postural challenge, showing no increase in heart rate and only a weak electrocortical response to a head-up tilt while asleep (Myers, Gomez-Gribben, Smith, Tseng, & Fifer, 2006). SIDS can result from a deficit in autonomic control, in which cerebellar and vestibular structures fail to mount compensatory somatomotor and cardio-respiratory responses to a drop in blood pressure (Harper, 2001). During this time there is also an abrupt shift in the pattern of sleep-wake states with a marked decline in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep as well as an attenuation of ability to respond to hypoxia (Kemp & Thach, 1993; Trinder et al., 1990). In addition, circadian rhythms become more firmly established (Lodemore, Peterson, & Wailoo, 1992). Interestingly, through this time period, EEG activity and behavioral arousal during quiet sleep are diminished, whereas in active sleep arousability is increased (Trinder et al., 1990).

Conclusions

The unique characteristics of infant sleep, including a preponderance of active sleep and frequent transitions between states, may be particularly well suited for timely learning about the extrauterine environment. Optimal sleep prepares the infant to learn when awake, and after learning has occurred during wakefulness, critical memory consolidation processes follow during sleep. Emerging evidence suggests that neonates also can process sensory input and learn while sleeping. A more thorough understanding of sleep state processes during stimulation is needed to address possible evolutionary implications of infant learning in different states, as well as developmental changes in learning capacities. The ability to learn adaptive responses to physiological challenges in the sleeping environment has crucial implications for infant survival. Future research also should examine individual differences in infants who are in the developmental window of greatest SIDS vulnerability in order to better understand the role of learning during sleep in promoting infant survival.

Acknowledgments

The writing of this manuscript was supported by the Sackler Institute of Developmental Psychobiology at Columbia University and by National Institute of Health grants R37 HD032774 to WPF, R01MH068073 to PDB, and T32 MH18264-21 to ART.

References

  1. Aarons L. Sleep-assisted instruction. Psychological Bulletin. 1976;83:1–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anders TF, Roffwarg HP. The effects of selective interruption and deprivation of sleep in the human newborn. Developmental Psychobiology. 1973;6:79–91. doi: 10.1002/dev.420060110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anders T, Sadeh A, Appareddy V. Normal sleep in neonates and children. In: Ferber R, Kryger MH, editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine in the child. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1995. pp. 7–18. [Google Scholar]
  4. Azaz Y, Fleming PJ, Levine M, McCabe R, Stewart A, Johnson P. The relationship between environmental temperature, metabolic rate, sleep state, and evaporative water loss in infants from birth to three months. Pediatric Research. 1992;32:417–423. doi: 10.1203/00006450-199210000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bear MF, Malenka RC. Synaptic plasticity: LTP and LTD. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1994;4:389–399. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90101-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beh HC, Barratt PEH. Discrimination and conditioning during sleep as indicated by the electroencephalogram. Science. 1965;147:1470–1471. doi: 10.1126/science.147.3664.1470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blumberg MS, Seelke AMH, Lowen SB, Karlsson K. Dynamics of sleep-wake cyclicity in developing rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102:14860–14864. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506340102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burns B, Lipsitt LP. Behavioral factors in crib death: Toward an understanding of the sudden infant death syndrome. Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1991;12:159–184. [Google Scholar]
  9. Buzsaki G. The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6:81–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Capellini I, McNamara P, Preston BT, Nunn CL, Barton RA. Does sleep play a role in memory consolidation? A comparative test. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:1–4. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cheour M, Martynova O, Naatanen R, Erkkola R, Sillanpaa M, Kero P, Raz A, Kaipio ML, Hiltunen J, Aoltnonen O, Savela J, Hamalainen H. Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature. 2002;415:599–600. doi: 10.1038/415599b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Coons S, Guilleminault C. Development of sleep wake patterns and non-rapid eye movement sleep stages during the first six months of life in normal infants. Pediatrics. 1982;69:793–798. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Czikk MJ, Sweeley JC, Homan JH, Milley JR, Richardson BS. Cerebral leucine uptake and protein synthesis in the near-term ovine fetus: relation to fetal behavioral state. American Journal of Physiology – Regulatory, Integrative, & Comparative Physiology. 2002;284:R200–R207. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00190.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Darnall RA, Williams SB, Schneider RW. Arousal habituation in response to intermittent hypoxia and its reversal: Effects of age and sex. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology; Washington, D. C.. 2008. Nov, [Google Scholar]
  15. DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP. Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science. 1980;208:1175–1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Denenberg VH, Thoman EB. Evidence for a functional role for active (REM) sleep in infancy. Sleep. 1981;4:185–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Deregnaucourt S, Mitra PP, Feher O, Pytte C, Tchernichovski O. How sleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature. 2005;433:710–716. doi: 10.1038/nature03275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. deRegnier RA, Nelson CA, Thomas KM, Wewerka S, Georgieff MK. Neurophysiologic evaluation of auditory recognition memory in healthy newborn infants and infants of diabetic mothers. Journal of Pediatrics. 2000;137:777–784. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2000.109149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dijk DJ. EEG slow waves and sleep spindles: Windows on the sleeping brain. Behavioural Brain Research. 1995;69:109–116. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00007-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fagioli I, Salzarulo P. Sleep states development in the first year of life assessed through 24-h recording. Early Human Development. 1982;6:215–228. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(82)90109-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fenn KM, Nusbaum HC, Margoliash D. Consolidation during sleep of perceptual learning of spoken language. Nature. 2003;425:614–616. doi: 10.1038/nature01951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ficca G, Fagioli I, Salzarulo P. Sleep organization in the first year of life: Developmental trends in the quiet sleep – paradoxical sleep cycle. Journal of Sleep Research. 2000;9:1–4. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00172.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ficca G, Lombardo P, Rossi L, Salzarulo P. Morning recall of verbal material depends on prior sleep organization. Behavioural Brain Research. 2000;112:159–163. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00177-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fifer WP, Byrd DL, Kaku M, Eigsti I, Isler JR, Grose-Fifer J, Balsam PD. Newborn infants learn during sleep. 2009 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005061107. Manuscript under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frank M, Issa N, Stryker M, Keck W. Sleep enhances plasticity of the developing visual cortex. Neuron. 2001;30:275–287. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00279-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gais S, Molle M, Helms K, Born J. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:6830–6834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06830.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gais S, Plihal W, Wagner U, Born J. Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1335–1339. doi: 10.1038/81881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gertner S, Greenbaum CW, Sadeh A, Dolfin Z, Sirota L, Ben-Nun Y. Sleep-wake patterns in preterm infants and 6 month's home environment: implciations for early cognitive development. Early Human Development. 2002;68:93–102. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(02)00018-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gomez RL, Bootzin RR, Nadel L. Naps promote abstraction in language-learning infants. Psychological Science. 2006;17:670–674. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01764.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gunther M. Instinct and the nursing couple. Lancet. 1955;265:575–578. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(55)91631-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Haddad GG, Donnelly DF. The interaction of chemoreceptors and baroreceptors with the central nervous system. A critical role in early life. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1988;533:221–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb37251.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Harper RM. Autonomic control during sleep and risk for sudden death in infancy. Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 2001;139:185–194. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hennevin E, Hars B, Maho C, Bloch V. Processing of learned information in paradoxical sleep: Relevance for memory. Behavioural Brain Research. 1995;69:125–135. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00013-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hensch TK. Critical period regulation. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:549–579. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hogan D, Howard P, Roffwarg HP, Shaffery J. The effects of 1 week REM sleep deprivation on parvalbumin and calbindin immunoreactive neurons in central visual pathways of kittens. Journal of Sleep Research. 2001;10:285–296. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00270.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoppenbrouwers T, Hodgman J, Harper R, Sterman M. Respiration during the first six months of life in normal infants: IV. Gender differences. Early Human Development. 1980;4(2):167–177. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(80)90020-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hoppenbrouwers T, Hodgman J, Harper R, Sterman M. Temporal distribution of sleep states, somatic activity, and autonomic activity during the first half year of life. Sleep. 1982;5:131–144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ikeda K, Morotomi T. Classical conditioning during human NREM sleep and response transfer to wakefulness. Sleep. 1996;19:72–74. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJ, Sagi D. Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science. 1994;265:679–682. doi: 10.1126/science.8036518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kavanau JL. Sleep and dynamic stabilization of neural circuitry: A review and synthesis. Behavior Brain Research. 1994;63:111–126. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90082-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kelly DH, Carley DW, Shannon DC. Periodic breathing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1988;533:301–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb37259.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kemp JS, Thach BT. A sleep position-dependent mechanism for infant death on sheepskins. American Journal of Disorders of Childhood. 1993;147(6):642–646. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kinney HC, Filiano JJ, Sleeper LA, Mandell F, Valdes-Dapena M, White WF. Decreased muscarinic receptor binding in the arcuate nucleus in sudden infant death syndrome. Science. 1995;269(5229):1446–1450. doi: 10.1126/science.7660131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effect of behavioral state, experience and EEG dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:4090–4101. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04090.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lesku JA, Roth TC, Amlaner CJ, Lima SL. A phylogenetic analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: The integration of anatomy, physiology, and ecology. The American Naturalist. 2006;168:441–453. doi: 10.1086/506973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lipsitt L. Infant learning. In: Quay HC, Troll L, Finlay CE, editors. Review of Infant Development. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1982. pp. 62–78. [Google Scholar]
  47. Lipsitt LP. Learning and emotion in infants. Pediatrics. 1998;102:1262–1267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lodemore MR, Petersen SA, Waillo MP. Factors affecting the development of night time temperature rhythms. Archives of Disorders of Childhood. 1992;67(10):1259–1261. doi: 10.1136/adc.67.10.1259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Louis J, Cannard C, Bastuji H, Challamel MJ. Sleep ontogenesis revisited. Sleep. 1997;20:323–333. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.5.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Louis J, Zhang JX, Revol M, Debilly G, Challamel MJ. Ontogenesis of nocturnal organization of sleep spindles: A longitudinal study during the first 6 months of life. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology. 1992;83:289–296. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90088-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Maho C, Bloch V. Responses of hippocampal cells can be conditioned during paradoxical sleep. Brain Research. 1992;581:115–122. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90350-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mazzoni G, Gori S, Formicola G, Gneri C, Massetani R, Murri L, Salzarulo P. Word recall correlates with sleep cycles in elderly subjects. Journal of Sleep Research. 1999;8:185–188. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1999.00154.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. McGraw MB. The Neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York, NY: Columbia University Press; 1963. [Google Scholar]
  54. Mirmiran M, Ariagno RL. Role of REM sleep in brain development and plasticity. In: Maquet P, Smith C, Stickgold R, editors. Sleep and brain plasticity. Oxford: University Press; 2003. pp. 181–187. [Google Scholar]
  55. Mirmiran M, Feenstra MG, Dijcks FA, Bos NP, Van Haaren F. Functional deprivation of noradrenaline neurotransmission: Effects of clonidine on brain development. Progress in Brain Research. 1988;73:159–172. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60503-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mirmiran M, Uylings HB, Corner MA. Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat. Developmental Brain Research. 1983;7:277–286. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(83)90184-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mirmiran M, Maas YGH, Ariagno RL. Development of fetal and neonatal sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2003;7:321–334. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mirmiran M, Uylings HB, Corner MA. Pharmacological suppression of REM sleep prior to weaning counteracts the effectiveness of subsequent environmental enrichment on cortical growth in rats. Brain Research. 1983;283:102–105. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(83)90086-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mitchell EA, Williams SM, Taylor BJ. Use of duvets and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1999;81:117–119. doi: 10.1136/adc.81.2.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Myers MM, Gomez-Gribben E, Smith KS, Tseng A, Fifer WP. Developmental changes in infant heart rate responses to head-up tilting. Acta Paediatrica. 2006;95:77–81. doi: 10.1080/08035250500325074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Paluzynska DA, Harris KA, Thach BT. Influence of sleep position experience on ability of prone-sleeping infants to escape from asphyxiating microenvironments by changing head position. Pediatrics. 2004;114:1634–1639. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Papousek H. Conditioned head rotation reflexes in infants in the first months of life. Acta Paediatrica. 1961;50:565–576. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1961.tb08047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Peirano PD, Algarin CR. Sleep in brain development. Biological Research. 2007;40:471–478. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:571–582. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Prechtl HF. The behavioral states of the newborn infant (a review) Brain Research. 1974;76:185–212. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90454-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Prechtl HF. Assessment and significance of behavioural states. In: Berenberg SR, editor. Brain, fetal, and infant. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff Medical Division; 1977. pp. 79–90. [Google Scholar]
  67. Prechtl HF, O'Brien MJ. Behavioral states of the full-term newborn: The emergence of a concept. In: Stratton P, editor. Psychobiology of the human newborn. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1982. pp. 53–73. [Google Scholar]
  68. Richardson BS, Caetano H, Homan J, Carmichael L. Regional brain blood flow in the ovine fetus during transition to the low-voltage electrocortical state. Developmental Brain Research. 1994;81:10–16. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(94)90063-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC. Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science. 1966;152:604–619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rovee-Collier C, Lipsitt LP. Learning, adaptation, and memory in the newborn. In: Stratton P, editor. Psychobiology of the human newborn. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sahni R, Schulze KF, Kashyap S, Ohira-Kist K, Fifer WP, Myers MM. Sleeping position and electrocortical activity in low birthweight infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2005;90:F311–315. doi: 10.1136/adc.2004.055327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sambeth A, Ruohio K, Alku P, Fellman V, Huotilainen M. Sleeping newborns extract prosody from continuous speech. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2008;119:332–341. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.09.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Scher MS, Steppe DA, Dahl RE, Asthana S, Guthrie RD. Comparison of EEG sleep measures in healthy full-term and preterm infants at matched conceptional ages. Sleep. 1992;15:442–448. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.5.442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shaffery JP, Lopez J, Bissette G, Roffwarg HP. Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation revives a form of developmentally regulated synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex of post-critical period rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2006;391:96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.08.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Siqueland ER, Lipsitt LP. Conditioned head-turning in human newborns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1966;3:356–376. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(66)90080-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsaki G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2003;100:2065–2069. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437938100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Smith C, MacNeill C. Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students. Journal of Sleep Research. 1994;3:206–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00133.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. So K, Adamson TM, Horne RS. The use of actigraphy for the assessment of the development of sleep/wake patterns in infants during the first 12 months of life. Journal of Sleep Research. 2007;16:181–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00582.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Stickgold R, Walker M. Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: What is the role of sleep. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sullivan RM, Taborsky-Barba S, Mendoza R, Itano A, Leon M, Cotman CW, Payne TF, Lott I. Olfactory classical conditioning in neonates. Pediatrics. 1991;87:511–518. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Thomas AJ, Friedman L, MacKenzie CN, Strohl KP. Modification of conditioned apneas in rats: evidence for cortical involvement. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1995;78:1215–1218. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1995.78.4.1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Trinder J, Newman NM, Le Grande M, Whitworth F, Kay A, Pirkis J, Jordan K. Behavioural and EEG responses to auditory stimuli during sleep in newborn infants and in infants aged 3 months. Biological Psychology. 1990;31(3):213–227. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(90)90035-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, Verleger R, Born J. Sleep inspires insight. Nature. 2004;427:353–355. doi: 10.1038/nature02223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Walker M, Brakefield T, Hobson J, Stickgold R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature. 2003;425:616–620. doi: 10.1038/nature01930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Walker M, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: Sleep dependent motor skill learning. Neuron. 2002;35:205–211. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00746-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Watson JB, Rayner R. Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1920;3:1–14. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES