Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Mar 18.
Published in final edited form as: Front Biosci (Schol Ed). 2011 Jan 1;3:298–330. doi: 10.2741/s153

Table 2.

Influence of bioactive lipids on cognition: Human studies

Study Study aims and design Sample Variables Results
M. A.
Beydoun
et al.
(111)
Association between plasma n-3
FAs and cognitive decline in older
adults.

Prospective cohort design.
2251 individuals (aged 50 –
65 yrs) residing in
Minneapolis, MN during
1990-1992 and 1996-1998.
DV:
(1) Cognition assessed at 2 time points
(3-yr interval):
(a) verbal learning and memory
(b) psychomotor
(c) verbal fluency
(d) global cognitive decline (composite
score based on change scores for tests a-
c)

(2) Plasma FAs at study entry (saturated,
monounsaturated, linoleic acid, α-
linolenic acid, n-3 and n-6 FAs)
Association between FAs and
cognitive decline from
baseline to 3 years:
(1) ↑ global cognitive
decline associated with:
↑ palmitic acid
↑ AA
↓ linoleic acid

(2) ↓ decline in verbal
fluency associated with:
↑ DHA + EPA
J. Colombo
et al.
(108)
Association between levels of
phospholipid DHA in mothers and
infants at delivery, and the
development of attention during
infancy and toddlerhood.
Longitudinal (prospective) follow-
up design.
70 infants recruited for
follow-up from among 350
infants and mothers enrolled
in study of effects of DHA
supplementation on
pregnancy outcomes
(randomized, double-blind,
controlled clinical trial).
IV:
(1) Supplemented mothers’ diet during
last trimester of pregnancy by providing
eggs with high- vs. standard-levels of
DHA (135 mg vs. 35 mg DHA/egg,
respectively).
(2) Group membership (high- vs. low-
DHA) determined by levels of DHA in
RBC for mother and infant at delivery.

DV:
Laboratory measures (gaze duration,
heart rate) of attention in children during:
(a) Visual habituation task in infants at
ages 4, 6, and 8 mos.
(b) Single-object exploration and
distractibility tasks in toddlers at ages 12
and 18 mos.
Compared to children of
mothers with low-DHA levels
at delivery:

(1) Infants of mothers with
high-DHA levels at delivery
showed accelerated
development for visual
attention at ages 4 and 6 mos.
(shorter gaze duration during
visual habituation task).

(2) Toddlers of mothers with
high-DHA levels at delivery
showed more mature
development of attention
(longer single-object
exploration; reduced
distractibility).
E.
Cyhlarova
et al.
(123)
Association between literacy skills
and n-3 and n-6 FAs in dyslexia.
32 dyslexic adults, 20 non-
dyslexic controls (mean ages
= 32 and 34 yrs, respectively)
DV:
RBC levels of n-3 and n-6 FAs (venous
blood draw after overnight fast)

Literacy skills and intelligence:
WRAT word reading, spelling;
WAIS subtests (similarities, vocabulary,
block design, picture arrangement, digit
span, digit symbol)
A. Dyslexic individuals and
non-dyslexic controls did not
differ for mean levels of RBC
FAs.

B. Both dyslexic and control
groups:
Word reading and total n-3
FAs positively correlated:
↑ total n-3 FAs
↑ reading score

C. Dyslexic individuals only:
(1) Word reading positively
correlated with n-3 FAs:
↑ α-linolenic acid
↑ reading score
(2) Word reading negatively
correlated with n-6 FAs:
↑ adrenic acid
↓ reading score

D. Control individuals only:
Word reading positively
correlated with n-3 FAs:
↑ DHA
↑ reading score
C.
Dullemeijer
et al.
(112)
Association between n-3 PUFAs
and change in cognition over 3
years in older adults.

Parent protocol: Randomized,
placebo-controlled trial to
determine effect of folic acid on
cognitive performance, carotid
intima-media thickness, hearing.

Baseline assessments occurred
during 2000 and 2001.
819 male and female adults at
baseline (ages 50-70 yrs)
IV: Random assignment to folic acid
supplementation (n=406) vs. placebo
(n=413)

DV: Data reported for individuals in
placebo-arm only (n=404):
(1) Change in performance from baseline
to 3 yrs in 5 domains:
(a) Memory (Word Learning Test)
(b) Sensorimotor speed
(Stroop Color-Word-Subtest I; Concept
Shifting-Subtests O, A, B)
(c) Complex speed (Stroop Color-Word-
III; Concept Shifting- C)
(d) Information-processing speed (Letter
Digit Substitution)
(e) Word fluency (Verbal Fluency Test)

(2) Plasma n-3 PUFAs at baseline
(venous blood after overnight fast):
EPA, docosapentaeoic acid, DHA
A. Cognitive performance
declined from baseline to 3
yrs for speed-related
responding: sensorimotor
speed, complex speed,
information-processing
speed.

B. n-3 PUFA concentrations
at baseline were associated
with decline in speed-related
responding at 3 yrs:
↑ n-3 PUFAs at baseline
↓ decline over 3 yrs for
sensorimotor speed and
complex speed.
J. A.
Dunstan et
al. (109)
Effects of antenatal n-3 PUFA
supplementation on cognitive
development in children at age 2.5
yrs.

Randomized, double-blind
placebo-controlled trial conducted
between January 2000 and
September 2001.
72 infants of 98 women
enrolled during pregnancy
IV: From 20 weeks gestation to delivery,
pregnant Caucasian women with allergic
disease randomly assigned to receive:
(1) n-3 FA supplementation (n=52; 4
g/day fish oil with 2.2 g/day DHA and
1.1 g/day EPA)
vs.
(2) placebo (n=46; 4 g/day olive oil with
2.7 g/day n-9 oleic acid).
Both n-3 FA supplement and placebo
included 3-4 mg/g oil α-tocopherol.

DV:
(1) n-3 and n-6 PUFAs in phospholipids
of erythrocyte membranes in umbilical
cord blood
(2) Assessments of children at age 2.5 yrs
on measures of infant growth and
development (GMDS), receptive
language (PPVT), and behavior (CBCL)
A. Effect of antenatal
supplementation with n-3
PUFA on FA composition of
erythrocyte membranes in
umbilical cord blood of
neonates:
↑ n-3 PUFAs
↓ n-6 PUFAs

B. Effects of antenatal
supplementation with n-3
PUFA on child’s cognitive
development at age 2.5 yrs:
↑ eye–hand coordination
C. Association between
cognition at age 2.5 yrs and
PUFA levels in cord–blood
erythrocytes:
(1) Eye–hand coordination
positively correlated with n-3
PUFA levels (EPA, DHA):
↑ n-3 PUFA levels
↑ eye–hand coordination.

(2) Eye–hand coordination
and performance scores
negatively correlated with n-6
PUFA levels:
↑ AA levels
↓ eye–hand coordination and
performance.
Y. Ishikura
et al.
(124)
Effect of dietary supplementation
with AA on auditory P300 in
healthy older men.

Double-blind crossover design
25 healthy men (ages 57-68
yrs)
IV: 3-month crossover protocol (1
mo/phase):
Group A (n=13):
AA, washout, placebo
Group B (n=12):
placebo, washout, AA

AA supplement:
600 mg/day of AA-enriched triglyceride
(240 mg AA);
Placebo:
olive oil (600 mg/day)

DV:
(1) EEG recorded from midline parietal
electrode (Pz) to evaluate amplitude and
latency of auditory P300 component of
event-related potential elicited using an
oddball paradigm.

(2) Serum phospholipids measured before
and after supplement and placebo
periods.
A. Significant effect of AA-
supplementation on P300
response:
↓ P300 latency (mean Δ
= −10.1 ms ± 15.9)
↑ P300 amplitude (mean Δ =
0.8 μV ± 1.7).

B. Significant effect of AA-
supplementation on serum
phospholipids:
↑ serum AA (mean Δ = 37.7
μg/ml ± 26.2)
↓ serum EPA (mean Δ =
−13.0 μg/ml ± 20.3).
J. L.
Jacobson
et al.
(110)
Relationship of cord-plasma DHA
concentration to physical growth
and cognitive development at ages
6 and 11 mos for Inuit infants
living in Arctic Quebec.
Developmental outcomes
associated with PUFA intake from
breast feeding were also
evaluated.

Prospective longitudinal cohort
study, November 1995 to March
2001
109 Inuit infants and their
mothers
DV:
(1) Biological samples:
(a) Blood collected from severed
umbilical cord and from mothers after
delivery. Milk collected from breast-
feeding mothers at 1-mo postnatal
interview.
(b) FA compositions of plasma
phospholipids and total maternal milk
lipids.

(2) Perceptual, cognitive, and motor
development assessed at ages 6 mos
(Teller; FTII) and 11 mos (Teller; FTII;
BSID-II)
A. ↑ DHA concentration in
umbilical cord positively
associated with development:
↑ gestation length
↑ visual acuity (6 mos)
↑ novelty preference (6 mos)
↑ Mental development score
(11 mos)
↑ Psychomotor development
score (11 mos)

B. Post-natal DHA intake
from breast-feeding was not
correlated with
developmental outcome.
O. van de
Rest et al.
(114)
Association between dietary
intake of fatty fish and change in
cognition over 6 years in elderly
men.

Prospective cohort study, the
Veterans Affairs Normative Aging
Study. Recruitment initiated in
1963 in Boston, MA area.
451 men for whom complete
set of measures available
(mean age at baseline
cognitive assessment = 68
yrs).
DV:
(1) Estimates of n-3 PUFA intake from
dietary questionnaire (g of n-3 FAs per
portion/day): dark-meat fish (1.37 g);
canned tuna (0.69 g); shellfish (0.46 g);
other fish (0.17 g)

(2) Cognitive domains assessed at
baseline and over 6 yrs followup:
language, speed, attention, memory, and
spatial copying
A. Cross-sectional (baseline)
analyses:
Cognitive performance was
not associated with dietary
fatty fish or n-3 PUFA intake.

B. Longitudinal analyses:
Cognitive change over 6 yrs
was not associated with
dietary fatty fish or n-3 PUFA
intake.
L. J.
Whalley et
al. (120)
Influence of erythrocyte n-3
PUFA concentration on age-
related cognitive decline in the
presence or absence of the APOE
ε4 allele.

Prospective study of population-
based sample (1936 birth cohort).
Recruitment from November 1999
to February 2002.
Original sample = 478 healthy
volunteers (ages 63-66 yrs);
Subsample = 120 individuals
for whom cognitive
assessments and FA data were
available.
DV:
(1) Intelligence score at 11 yrs available
from national database

(2) At baseline, venous blood collected
for (a) APOE genotyping and (b) FA
composition of erythrocyte membranes
(total n-3 PUFAs, EPA, DHA, n-6:n-3
ratio)
(3) Cognition evaluated at ages 64
(baseline), 66, and 68 yrs: nonverbal
reasoning (RPM), verbal declarative
memory (AVLT), executive function
(Uses of Common Objects), information
processing speed and psychomotor
performance (WAIS-R Digit Symbol),
constructional ability (WAIS Block
Design)
A. Relationship between
PUFA concentrations and
intelligence:

(1) For APOE ε4 noncarriers
(absent), total n-3 PUFA at
baseline (≈ 63 yrs) was
positively correlated with
general intelligence at ages
11 yrs and 63-65 yrs:
↑ n-3 PUFA at 63 yrs
↑ Intelligence score at ages
11 and 63 yrs.

(2) Relationship between
PUFA concentrations and
intelligence was not
significant for APOE ε4
carriers (present).

B. Erythrocyte membrane
PUFA content did not differ
between groups based on
APOE ε4 status.

C. Verbal declarative
memory (AVLT) score
differed significantly between
APOE ε4 allele groups at all
time points: APOE ε4 allele
noncarriers > carriers.

Abbreviations for Table 2: ↑, increase; ↓, decrease; Δ, change; AA, Arachidonic acid; AVLT, Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test; BSID-II, Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd edition; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; CGI, Clinical Global Impression scale; DHA, Docosahexaenoic acid; DV, Dependent variable; EEG, Electroencephalogram; EPA, Eicosapentaenoic acid; FA, Fatty Acid; FTII, Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence; GMDS, Griffiths Mental Development Scales; IV, Independent variable; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; PUFA, Polyunsaturated fatty acid; RBC, Red Blood Cell; RPM, Raven’s Progressive Matrices; Teller, Teller Visual Acuity Card Test; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–revised.