Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 May 25.
Published in final edited form as: J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010 Jun 1;40(6):1325–1356. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00620.x

Table 4.

Effects of Source Credibility as a Function of Attitude Formation Versus Change and Message Repetition

Attitude formation
Attitude change
Variable Immediate
impact (d+)
Delayed
impact (d+)
Impact
change (Δ+)
Immediate
impact (d+)
Delayed
impact (d+)
Impact
change (Δ+)
Message repetition
No repetition 0.78 0.27 −0.51 0.27 0.17 −0.11
(0.58 to 0.98) (0.09 to 0.45) (−0.64 to −0.37) (0.21 to 0.34) (0.10 to 0.24) (−0.16 to −0.06)
Qw = 9.26, ns Qw = 6.08, ns Qw = 27.59** Qw = 61.90*** Qw = 41.72* Qw = 77.43***
k=10 k= 29
Message repeated 0.53 0.37 −0.16
(0.37 to 0.69) (0.21 to 0.54) (−0.28 to −0.04)
Qw = 12.55, ns Qw = 28.88** Qw = 34.66***
k=14
Between-group
differences (QB)
3.61 0.73, ns 16.78***

Note. The immediate and delayed impact of source credibility was calculated by subtracting mean persuasion for low-credibility sources from mean persuasion for high-credibility sources. Positive effect sizes indicate that highly credible sources induced greater persuasion than did less credible sources. The effect sizes in the impact change columns were computed by subtracting the immediate impact of source credibility from the delayed impact. These effect sizes are all negative because the impact of source credibility decayed over time, in general. The Qw statistic is an index of homogeneity of effect sizes within a given cell. Significant Qw values indicate violations of the homogeneity assumption. The between-classes effect (Qb) is a sum of squares analogous to a main effect in a fixed-effects ANOVA test (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). k= number of studies; d+ and Δ+ = weighted average effect size; CI = confidence interval around d+ or Δ+.

p < .06.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.