Abstract
Differences in host behavior and resistance to disease can influence the outcome of host-pathogen interactions. We capitalized on the variation in aggregation behavior of Fowler's toads (Anaxyrus [ = Bufo] fowleri) and grey treefrogs (Hyla versicolor) tadpoles and tested for differences in transmission of Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd) and host-specific fitness consequences (i.e., life history traits that imply fitness) of infection in single-species amphibian mesocosms. On average, A. fowleri mesocosms supported higher Bd prevalences and infection intensities relative to H. versicolor mesocosms. Higher Bd prevalence in A. fowleri mesocosms may result, in part, from higher intraspecific transmission due to the aggregation of tadpoles raised in Bd treatments. We also found that, independent of species, tadpoles raised in the presence of Bd were smaller and less developed than tadpoles raised in disease-free conditions. Our results indicate that aggregation behavior might increase Bd prevalence and that A. fowleri tadpoles carry heavier infections relative to H. versicolor tadpoles. However, our results demonstrate that Bd appears to negatively impact larval growth and developmental rates of A. fowleri and H. versicolor similarly, even in the absence of high Bd prevalence.
Introduction
Traditionally, ecologists have focused on the effects of disturbance, competition, and predation when studying species interactions [1], [2]. However, recent studies underscore the impacts that parasites and pathogens (hereafter “pathogens”) have on communities [3], [4], [5]. For example, pathogens can directly affect host populations by causing mortality [6] or indirectly by altering life history traits, such as growth and developmental rates [7]. Additionally, sub-lethal infections can alter normal host behaviors such as feeding [8], antipredator [9], and thermoregulatory [10] behavior, each of which can shape the ecological interactions within a community. Indeed, certain behaviors by non-infected hosts can increase their chances of becoming infected. For example, hosts with promiscuous mating strategies may increase the probability of contact with an infected host [11]. Since pathogen transmission is one of the driving forces behind pathogen regulation of host populations [12], identifying how pathogen-induced changes in host behavior affect pathogen transmission is a central component of host–pathogen ecology.
Determining the role that pathogens play in amphibian populations is especially important given their worldwide declines [13], [14]. Chytridiomycosis, an infectious disease of amphibians, is caused by the pathogenic fungus Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd) and has been implicated as a causal agent in amphibian population declines over the past three decades [13], [15]. Bd is spread through aquatic environments by free-swimming zoospores [16], [17]. In tadpoles, Bd infections are restricted to within, or around, the keratinized labial teeth and jaw sheaths. Bd does not generally cause mortality in anuran tadpoles (but see [18]); however, Bd infections can reduce larval feeding efficiency [19], [20] and growth and developmental rates [21], [22], [23], [24].
Given the potential impacts that interspecific differences in host behavior and life history traits have on disease dynamics in host populations, understanding the effects of Bd on amphibians with different behaviors and life histories are a key component in understanding the ecology of this host-pathogen system. However, host variation in susceptibility and pathogen-induced life history responses are poorly understood (but see [18], [19], [23], [25]). For example, Bd infection reduced the foraging activity of A. fowleri tadpoles but not H. chrysoscelis tadpoles [19]. Additionally, few data exist on how far zoospores can travel and infect a susceptible host. If Bd transmission is limited by how far zoospores can travel, species-specific life history, or behavioral, strategies may differentially impact certain species. For example, species that aggregate may have an increased potential of disease transmission [26]. Indeed, recent evidence suggests that tadpoles of species that are highly social increase their aggregative behavior when infected with Bd [27].
In our study, we examined the effects of Bd on two species of anuran tadpoles—Fowler's toads (Anaxyrus [ = Bufo] fowleri) and grey treefrogs (Hyla versicolor)—in replicated, single-species, outdoor experimental mesocosms. These two species are ideal focal taxa because they are sympatric in ponds yet differ in their aggregation behavior and may have different patterns of pathogen transmission. Generally, A. fowleri tadpoles aggregate on the substrate of ponds [28], whereas H. versicolor tadpoles typically occur singly in the middle of the water column [29]. First, we measured the proportion of tadpoles aggregating and tested for species differences in tadpole aggregation when raised in the presence and absence of Bd. We then tested for species differences in Bd prevalence and infection intensity of tadpoles raised in the presence of Bd. We predicted that aggregation behavior in A. fowleri would facilitate higher Bd transmission and would result in higher Bd prevalence relative to H. versicolor mesocosms. Lastly, we tested whether raising tadpoles in the presence of Bd affected their rates of growth and development. We predicted that tadpoles raised in the presence of Bd would have reduced rates of growth and development relative to tadpoles raised in the absence of Bd.
Materials and Methods
Animal Collection and Husbandry
All tadpoles used in our experiments were derived from field-collected egg masses of A. fowleri and H. versicolor from within the Meeman Shelby State Park in Tennessee, USA (Shelby County, TN, USA) in 2009. For each species, all of the egg masses appeared to be oviposited the night before collection. Immediately after collection, eggs were transported to the laboratory at The University of Memphis. Upon hatching, tadpoles were maintained in 37.85 L glass aquaria (filled with approximately 20 L of aged tap water that was continually aerated) containers until they had reached the free-swimming stage (stage 25 [30]). We haphazardly selected a subset of tadpoles from all the clutches and combined the tadpoles from the different clutches to evenly distribute potential genetic effects on the larval traits we measured. We then randomly selected a subset out of the remaining stock of tadpoles to expose to Bd, which served as “pathogen source” tadpoles (see below). We kept the remainder of the tadpoles in the laboratory for 7 days before placing them in the mesocosms. Tadpoles were maintained on a 12 h light∶12 h dark photoperiod at 19°C (±1°C) and were fed a 50∶50 mixture of ground rabbit chow and Sera Micron® (a powered commercial algal-based food containing Spirulina and sea algae meal) ad libitum daily.
Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis inoculation
Bd was grown in the laboratory on tryptone-gelatin hydrolysate-lactose (TGhL) agar in 9 cm Petri dishes according to standard protocol [16]. We harvested Bd zoospores (FMB 001, isolated in 2008 from an infected anuran in Shelby County, TN, USA) by adding 10.0-mL of sterile water to the cultures and collected the zoospores that emerged from the zoosporangia after 30 minutes. We exposed a subset of the laboratory born tadpoles (N = 38 per species) to an infectious dose of Bd by placing individual tadpoles in 50 mL water baths containing infectious concentrations of fungal zoospores (120,000 zoospores/mL) for 48 hours. Our design simulated transmission by water, one of the possible modes of Bd transmission in natural environments [17]. After the exposure period, the majority of the tadpoles (pathogen-source tadpoles; see below) were held by species in 37.85 L glass aquaria (filled with approximately 20 L of aged tap water that was continually aerated) in the laboratory. The pathogen source tadpoles (N = 24 per species) were held in the laboratory for 7 days before placing them in outdoor experimental tanks to allow Bd infections to develop (see below). The remaining tadpoles (N = 14 per species) were held individually in the laboratory for 7 days to confirm our infection protocol.
Experimental Design
We manipulated the presence of Bd in replicated experimental mesocosms. We reared tadpoles in 32 polyethylene tanks (1.83 m diameter) positioned in an array at the University of Memphis Edward J. Meeman Biological Field Station (Shelby County, Tenn., United States; 35°22′N, 90°1′W). We prepared tanks prior to the breeding season of anurans in early April 2009. Each tank was filled with tap water to a depth of 30.5 cm (∼613 L) and 1.0 kg of air dried leaf litter collected from a nearby deciduous forest was added. One 500-ml aliquot of concentrated plankton suspension was collected from three unused experimental tanks that were set up during August 2008. We placed a 65.58×121.92 cm piece of fiberglass composite in each tank at a 30° angle which simulated the margin of a pond. This is an important component of our design because larval A. fowleri do not have lungs and are negatively buoyant [31]; thus, it allowed tadpoles to position themselves in the water column without expending extra energy to respirate and provided a location for A. fowleri tadpoles to aggregate as observed in natural ponds. We then securely fashioned fiberglass mesh screens (1-mm mesh) as lids to each tank to prevent colonization of feral predators and competitors and to provide shading and allowed tanks to condition for 14 days before adding tadpoles. All tank preparations followed approved IACUC protocols (The University of Memphis #0650) and no further permits were necessary.
We used a 2×2×2 fully factorial design consisting of tadpoles of each species (A. fowleri and H. versicolor) reared in two pathogen treatments (Bd and control) for two trial durations (10 and 15 days). The resulting 8 treatment combinations were replicated 4 times and assigned randomly to the 32 experimental tanks. On 0800 on 04 May 2009 (Day 0), we brought the pathogen source and non-exposed tadpoles from the laboratory and allowed them to acclimate to the ambient temperature for 4 hours prior to placing them in the experimental tanks. For the control treatment, we placed 30 conspecific tadpoles in each experimental tank (N = 8 per species). For the Bd treatment, we first placed 27 non-exposed tadpoles of each species in the remaining experimental tanks (N = 8 per species). We then placed 3 Bd exposed tadpoles of each species in the tanks of the Bd treatment, equalizing the density of the two pathogen treatments.
Throughout the experiment, we placed all laboratory materials in a containment tank with bleach (6% sodium hypochlorite) to yield a 10% solution, which kills Bd [32]. At the completion of the experiment, we also added bleach to each tank to yield a 10% solution. The lids were then securely fashioned on the tanks and we allowed the bleach solution sit for 30 days prior to emptying the water from each tank.
Bd transmission, prevalence and life history traits
On Day 10, we destructively sampled 16 experimental tanks by individually removing every larva from each tank with a small dipnet. To prevent accidental contamination of samples, we first collected all tadpoles from the control tanks. In the Bd tanks, we collected each larva individually, rinsed it with aged tap water, and placed it in an individual screw top vial containing MS222. Before using the dipnet again, the dipnet was thoroughly rinsed with aged tap water and briefly soaked in the mesocosm to remove any potential zoospores the previous larva deposited on the net. These methods prevented accidental transfer of Bd between tadpoles in, and between, each experimental tank. On Day 15, we destructively sampled the remaining 16 experimental tanks as described previously. All tadpoles were stored in 100% EtOH and brought to the laboratory where we measured the size (total length; TL) and determined the developmental stage [30] of each larva. After collecting data on size and development, we dissected the oral apparatus for quantitative PCR analysis to confirm Bd infection.
We used real-time quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR) [33] to confirm the infection status of all tadpoles from Bd mesocosms and three randomly chosen control mesocosms. We also used qPCR on laboratory held tadpoles (N = 14 per species) to confirm our Bd exposure method. In brief, DNA was extracted from the tissue of the entire oral apparatus, which was dissected from all tadpoles immediately after collection, and stored in 100% EtOH until qPCR analyses. Each sample was run in triplicate against a Bd standard titration (from 105 to 101 zoospores) using relative qPCR on an ABI 7300 real-time PCR machine, and the pathogen treatment (Bd exposed or control) was unknown to the experimenter. We considered an animal as “infected” if the genome equivalent was greater than 0.1 [34].
Behavioral Observations
During routine visits to our mesocosm array (days 5 and 10 of the experiment), we monitored our mesocosm array from 0800–1000 and made a series of observations (N = 2 per visit) on the number of tadpoles aggregating on the artificial fiberglass pond margin. Here, we define an aggregation as any number of tadpoles (≥2) that co-occurred within one-half of the width (∼30 cm) of the artificial pond margin. Thus, we did not consider tadpoles aggregating if they were co-occurred on opposite ends of the artificial pond margin. We focused on tadpole aggregations on the fiberglass pond margins because A. fowleri tadpoles are unlikely to aggregate on the floor of the tank because they avoid deep water (however, tadpoles sometimes positioned themselves along small ridges on the wall of the mesocosm). Because tadpoles generally aggregated on the upper one-third of the fiberglass, we considered this an effective measure of aggregation behavior because of the close proximity of tadpoles to one another on a standardized area within the mesocosm.
Statistical Analyses
To obtain Bd prevalence within each mesocosm, we calculated the proportion of tadpoles infected with Bd at the termination of the experiment. Additionally, we obtained the zoospore equivalents of Bd-infected tadpoles as a measure of infection intensity. For this measure, we considered mean values per tank as the unit of analysis because measurements from infected individuals within tanks were not independent. We used analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test the main effects of species (A. fowleri and H. versicolor) and experimental duration (10 and 15 days), and their interaction, on the dependent variables proportion of tadpoles infected and their corresponding zoospore equivalents. We used square root and log transformations to normalize the prevalence and intensity data (respectively).
We measured size (TL) and stage (Gosner) per day as metrics of larval performance. We considered mean values per tank as the unit of analysis because measurements from individuals within tanks were not independent. We used multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to test for the effects of independent variables species (A. fowleri and H. versicolor), pathogen treatment (Bd and control), and experimental duration (10 and 15 days) and their interactions on the dependent variables size and stage. Because of significant correlations between size and stage, we then used reciprocal univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on those dependent variables to control for effects of the one variable on the other.
A preliminary analysis revealed that the number of tadpoles aggregating did not differ within or between observation dates. Thus, for each pathogen treatment, we averaged the proportion of tadpoles that we observed aggregating (number of tadpoles aggregating/total number of tadpoles per mesocosm) across both observation dates. We used two-way ANOVA to test for the effects of the independent variables species (A. fowleri and H. versicolor), pathogen treatment (Bd and control), and their interaction on the dependent variable proportion of tadpoles aggregated. We also used regression analysis to examine the relationship between the proportion of tadpoles aggregating and the average Bd zoospores in each mesocosm. We used arcsine transformation for proportion data and log transformations on Bd infection loads to normalize our data.
All statistical analyses were performed in SPSS. Our data met the assumptions of the statistical tests used.
Results
Infection prevalence of the subset of A. fowleri and H. versicolor tadpoles (N = 14 per species) that were held in the laboratory was 35% and 64%, respectively. This confirms our infection protocol was capable of infecting susceptible hosts. We used a conservative estimate and considered our starting Bd prevalence to be 10% (i.e., 3/30) and that any prevalence per tank above 10% was evidence for transmission.
One experimental mesocosm (H. versicolor, Bd, 15 day duration) developed an algal bloom that killed all the tadpoles, and was subsequently excluded from analyses. With the exception of the discarded replicate, we did not observe any appreciable mortality (<1% across all treatments). No tadpoles (N = 90) from the 3 randomly selected control mesocosms tested positive for Bd.
Bd transmission, prevalence, and infection intensity
Although Bd transmission was low, we detected transmission in 4 A. fowleri (N = 3, Day 10; N = 1, Day 15) and 1 H. versicolor (Day 10) mesocosms. In addition, we found species differences in Bd prevalence and the average infection intensity per individual. Bd prevalence significantly differed between species (F1,15 = 15.23, p = 0.0025), where A. fowleri mesocosms had higher prevalence relative to H. versicolor mesocosms (11.7%±1.54 and 4.8%±1.60, respectively). Irrespective of species, tadpoles exposed to Bd for 10 days had marginally higher Bd prevalence relative to 15 days (F1,15 = 4.54, p = 0.0566); however, there was no species×duration interaction.
In terms of Bd infection intensity, A. fowleri tadpoles had more intense average infections relative to H. versicolor tadpoles (F1,15 = 13.47, p = 0.004; Figure 1). Additionally, we found a significant species×duration interaction, where at the 15 day trial duration, A. fowleri tadpoles had more intense average infections relative to H. versicolor tadpoles (F1,15 = 5.74, p = 0.035).
Behavioral Observations
Since tadpole aggregations did not differ between the two observation dates (p>0.100), we used the mean proportion of tadpoles aggregating across both dates in our analysis. Neither species (F1,32 = 2.63, p = 0.116) nor pathogen (F = 0.149, p = 0.702) significantly affected the proportion of tadpoles aggregating on the artificial fiberglass pond margin. However, we found a significant species×pathogen interaction (F1,32 = 13.16, p = 0.001) on the proportion of tadpoles aggregating. Holm-Sidak post hoc analyses revealed that A. fowleri tadpoles aggregated significantly more (t1,15 = 2.84, p = 0.008) in Bd mesocosms relative to control mesocosms whereas H. versicolor tadpoles aggregated significantly less (t1,15 = 2.29, p = 0.030) in Bd mesocosms relative to control mesocosms. In addition, we found that the proportion of tadpoles aggregating was a significant predictor of Bd infection intensity. The total number of Bd zoospores detected (calculated as a sum of zoospores among infected individuals in the same replicate) increased linearly as a function of proportion of tadpoles aggregating (F1,15 = 4.74, p = 0.0485; Figure 2).
Life History Traits
There were significant MANOVA effects of species, pathogen treatment, duration, and species×duration interaction on the combined larval responses (Table 1). ANCOVA revealed significant effects of Bd on the size and stage of tadpoles developing in pathogen tanks. Under disease conditions, tadpoles of both species were smaller and less developed than tadpoles reared in disease-free conditions, irrespective of the experiment duration (Table 1, Figure 3, Figure 4). Additionally, at the 15 day trial duration, tadpoles of both species reared in disease conditions were significantly smaller than tadpoles in disease-free conditions for the same duration of time (Figure 4).
Table 1. Summary of the MANOVA and univariate ANCOVAs for larval stage (Gosner) and size (total length) for single species mesocosms of Anaxyrus fowleri and Hyla versicolor tadpoles reared in the presence of absence of Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis infected tadpoles at two trial durations.
MANOVA: stage, size | |||
Source | Wilks' λ | F | p |
Species | 0.032 | 330.57 | <0.001 |
Pathogen | 0.629 | 6.490 | 0.006 |
Duration | 0.294 | 26.47 | <0.001 |
Species×pathogen | 0.987 | 0.149 | 0.863 |
Species×duration | 0.472 | 12.30 | <0.001 |
Pathogen×duration | 0.775 | 3.248 | 0.058 |
Species×pathogen×duration | 0.939 | 0.720 | 0.498 |
Both independent variables were used as reciprocal covariates in ANCOVAs. Significance levels for univariate tests were interpreted at 0.05.
Discussion
Quantifying patterns and outcomes of host-pathogen interactions are essential for understanding not only the ecological implications of pathogens on their hosts, but also on ecosystem processes such as food web dynamics [35] and community interactions [36]. Determining host specific rates of transmission and infection are key challenges of host-pathogen ecology because differential transmission can significantly impact disease dynamics. Although transmission was low in our experiment, we observed intraspecific transmission (as measured through increases in prevalence above the baseline starting prevalence) in 4/8 A. fowleri mesocosms and 1/7 H. versicolor mesocosms. We also found that Bd prevalence marginally increased relative to the starting prevalence in A. fowleri mesocosms, whereas Bd prevalence in H. versicolor mesocosms decreased by the end of the experiment. When assessing transmission in our experiment, we assumed that all of the 3 pathogen source tadpoles used in each mesocosm were infected and used a conservative estimate of 10% as the starting prevalence. However, Bd prevalence among the subset of A. fowleri and H. versicolor tadpoles held in the laboratory to confirm our infection protocol was 35% and 64%, respectively. This suggests that not all of the 3 pathogen source tadpoles were actually Bd+ and that our starting prevalence might have been <10% in some of the mesocosms. If so, the transmission and prevalence that we report here might be an underestimate of actual transmission in our mesocosms.
In addition, infected A. fowleri tadpoles carried approximately 4× heavier Bd loads relative to H. versicolor tadpoles. Given that our data on Bd transmission and prevalence within A. fowleri mesocosms were relatively low, one plausible explanation is that our data reflect that A. fowleri tadpoles are generally more susceptibility to Bd. While this claim has some support in that other bufonid tadpoles appear more susceptible to Bd infection and exposure (e.g. mortality, disorientation, and lethargy; [18]), our field and laboratory data do not fully support this explanation. If A. fowleri are more susceptible to Bd relative to H. versicolor, we would have expected to observe a similar pattern of Bd prevalence in the A. fowleri tadpoles housed individually in the laboratory to confirm our infection protocol; however, of this subset, approximately 2× more H. versicolor tadpoles tested Bd + than A. fowleri tadpoles. While A. fowleri tadpoles might be more susceptible to Bd, it appears that increased Bd transmission and prevalence might also be influenced by other factors, such as increased social behaviors.
One possible alternative is that species differences in aggregation behavior may have increased Bd transmission and prevalence. Compared to disease-free conditions, we observed significantly more A. fowleri tadpoles aggregating when raised in the presence of Bd but observed the opposite effect among H. versicolor tadpoles. These results support our data on increased Bd transmission and prevalence among A. fowleri tadpoles and suggest that pathogen-induced behavioral changes may lead to higher pathogen transmission (e.g. [26]). A recent study found that Bd + A. boreas tadpoles (another highly social species of bufonid) associated with Bd + conspecifics significantly more than towards Bd − conspecifics [27], which suggests that Bd − A. fowleri tadpoles do not avoid Bd + tadpoles and that aggregations of A. fowleri tadpoles would increase intraspecific transmission. We also found that irrespective of species, mesocosms the degree to which tadpoles aggregated significantly predicted Bd infection intensity, further supporting the idea that aggregations can increase transmission and disease risk. Thus, we suggest that (a) social behaviors, such as aggregation, observed in many species of bufonid tadpoles might increase their chances of encountering aquatic pathogens and that (b) low pathogen resistance increases their infection intensity. Without an understanding of other components of the tadpole response to Bd (e.g., physiology and immunology), we cannot claim that differences in aggregation behavior are the exclusive factor that influenced differences in Bd transmission and prevalence because differences in the physiological/immunological makeup of A. fowleri and H. versicolor tadpoles may have contributed to the species differences we observed.
The duration of exposure to a pathogen can increase the probability of infection (e.g., [37]) and/or increase the duration the hosts' immune response, each of which can reduce host growth rates by reallocating energy originally devoted to somatic maintenance and development to immune function [38]. Our results also demonstrate that tadpoles reared in the presence of Bd have values of life history traits that imply reduced fitness. Overall, A. fowleri and H. versicolor tadpoles reared under disease conditions were less developed than tadpoles within disease-free conditions. Specifically, we found an interactive effect of trial duration and pathogen treatment on tadpole size. Compared to 10 day trial duration, tadpoles reared in disease conditions for 15 days were significantly smaller than tadpoles reared in the absence of disease. These results suggest two alternative hypotheses regarding the effects of Bd. First, zoospore density at the individual and mesocosm levels was not high enough to affect growth rates until after 10 days, resulting in increased pathogen effects at the longer trial duration. Alternatively, Bd was transmitted quickly between pathogen source and susceptible tadpoles, increasing the duration of time that tadpoles were exposed to Bd, thereby increasing the effects of Bd. Because developmental rates become fixed during late stages in ontogeny [39], [40], undersized individuals may not be able to “catch-up” in their developmental trajectory [41] after early Bd exposure.
Given the strong effects of pathogen treatments on larval life history traits that we measured, the low prevalence of among tadpoles of Bd-exposed mesocosms was unexpected. Thus, our data suggest that exposure to Bd is sufficient to cause reductions in growth and developmental rates, which could be due to hosts preventing, or clearing, Bd infections—both of which will require tradeoffs between Bd resistance and stage/size when tadpoles exposed to low doses of Bd. First, low intensity Bd-infections may be successfully cleared by the host shortly after infection. Although no immunopathologies have been reported for Bd, recent experimental evidence suggests that tadpoles exposed to a low dose of Bd exhibited reduced size although only 40% of the tadpoles were infected with Bd [24]. Second, Bd-infection is prevented at low doses but energy is reallocated from growth and development to prevention. Although the exact mode of Bd infection of anuran tadpoles is unknown, other chytridiomycete fungi attach to host specific cells prior to entering the host [42], [43]. Specific host responses, such as the activation of lymphocytes and antibodies of the larval immune system [44] may prevent Bd infection. Whether infection is cleared or prevented, host responses to Bd are likely traded off against larval growth and development. Our data, along with [24] emphasize the necessity of testing for effects of Bd exposure in the absence of infection.
Interestingly, we found reduced growth and developmental rates of tadpoles reared in disease conditions in the absence of severe mouthpart deformations. Two recent experiments have proposed that Bd-induced damage to the keratinized mouthparts of tadpoles reduce their growth and developmental rates by altering the feeding kinematics [20] and decreases their feeding efficiency [19]. During feeding, the keratinized rows of labial teeth and jaw sheaths of tadpoles and are essential for effective feeding [45]. We examined the mouthparts of each tadpole reared in disease conditions and found a low incidence of mouthpart deformation (<5%; unpublished data). The low incidence of Bd-induced mouthpart deformation is not surprising, given the length of our experiments. Larval Rana muscosa infected with Bd begin to lose keratin 49 days post-infection and have no keratinized jaw sheaths 147 days post-infection [46], which is much longer than maximum duration of time tadpoles from our longest trial were infected with Bd (i.e., 15 days). In the absence of severe mouthpart deformations, we observed strong pathogen effects on larval life history traits, suggesting that Bd-induced structural damage is not the only mechanism behind reduced growth and development in tadpoles.
Acknowledgments
We thank R. Wassersug for comments on mesocosm design and L. Venesky for assistance collecting and processing tadpoles. J. Rohr provided statistical advice. Reviews by M. Rensel, M. Takahashi, and T. Wilcoxen improved the clarity of the manuscript. Collection permits from Tennessee (Tennessee Department of Environment and Conservation Science Collection Permit #2009-002 and Tennessee Wildlife Resources Agency Scientific Collection Permit #3450) were obtained prior to collecting the animals used in these experiments and all experimental procedures were approved by the University of Memphis IACUC (#0650). The experiments comply with the current laws of the USA.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This publication was developed, in part, under a GRO Research Assistance Agreement No. MA-916980 awarded by the United States Environmental Protection Agency to M.D. Venesky. Bd qPCR analysis was performed on instrumentation provided by NSF (MRI-0923419) to J. Kerby. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. No additional external funding received for this study.
References
- 1.Morin PJ. Predation, Competition, and the Composition of Larval Anuran Guilds. Ecological Monographs. 1983;53:119–138. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Menge BA, Sutherland JP. Community Regulation - Variation in Disturbance, Competition, and Predation in Relation to Environmental-Stress and Recruitment. American Naturalist. 1987;130:730–757. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hatcher MJ, Dick JTA, Dunn AM. A keystone effect for parasites in intraguild predation? Biology Letters. 2008;4:534–537. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lafferty KD, Allesina S, Arim M, Briggs CJ, De Leo G, et al. Parasites in food webs: the ultimate missing links. Ecology Letters. 2008;11:533–546. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01174.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Johnson PTJ, Ives AR, Lathrop RC, Carpenter SR. Long-term disease dynamics in lakes: causes and consequences of chytrid infections in Daphnia populations. Ecology. 2009;90:132–144. doi: 10.1890/07-2071.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brunner JL, Schock DM, Davidson EW, Collins JP. Intraspecific reservoirs: complex life history and the persistence of a lethal ranavirus. Ecology. 2004;85:560–566. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Goater CP. Growth and survival of postmetamorphic toads: interactions among larval history, density, and parasitism. Ecology. 1994;75:2264–2274. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Otterstatter MC, Gegear RJ, Colla SR, Thomson JD. Effects of parasitic mites and protozoa on the flower constancy and foraging rate of bumble bees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2005;58:383–389. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Thiemann GW, Wassersug RJ. Patterns and consequences of behavioural responses to predators and parasites in Rana tadpoles. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 2000;71:513–528. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Elliot SL, Blanford S, Thomas MB. Host-pathogen interactions in a varying environment: temperature, behavioural fever and fitness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2002;269:1599–1607. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Altizer S, Nunn CL, Thrall PH, Gittleman JL, Antonovics J, et al. Social organization and parasite risk in mammals: Integrating theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics. 2003;34:517–547. [Google Scholar]
- 12.de Castro F, Bolker B. Mechanisms of disease-induced extinction. Ecology Letters. 2005;8:117–126. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Daszak P, Cunningham A, Hyatt AD. Infectious disease and amphibian population declines. Diversity and Distributions. 2003;9:141–150. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lips KR, Brem F, Brenes R, Reeve JD, Alford RA, et al. Emerging infectious disease and the loss of biodiversity in a Neotropical amphibian community. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:3165–3170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506889103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Berger L, Speare R, Daszak P, Green DE, Cunningham A, et al. Chytridiomycosis causes amphibian mortality associated with population declines in the rain forests of Australia and Central America. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1998;95:9031–9036. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.15.9031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Longcore JE, Pessier AP, Nichols DK. Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis gen et sp nov, a chytrid pathogenic to amphibians. Mycologia. 1999;91:219–227. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pessier AP, Nichols DK, Longcore JE, Fuller MS. Cutaneous chytridiomycosis in poison dart frogs (Dendrobates spp.) and White's tree frogs (Litoria caerulea). Journal of Veterinary Diagnostic Investigation. 1999;11:194–199. doi: 10.1177/104063879901100219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Blaustein AR, Romansic JM, Scheesseele EA, Han BA, Pessier AP, et al. Interspecific variation in susceptibility of frog tadpoles to the pathogenic fungus Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis. Conservation Biology. 2005;19:1460–1468. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Venesky MD, Parris MJ, Storfer A. Impacts of Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis Infection on Tadpole Foraging Performance. EcoHealth. 2009;6:565–575. doi: 10.1007/s10393-009-0272-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Venesky MD, Wassersug RJ, Parris MJ. Fungal pathogen changes the feeding kinematics of larval anurans. Journal of Parasitology. 2010;96:552–557. doi: 10.1645/GE-2353.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Parris MJ. Hybrid response to pathogen infection in interspecific crosses between two amphibian species (Anura: Ranidae). Evolutionary Ecology Research. 2004;6:457–471. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Parris MJ, Baud DR. Interactive effects of a heavy metal and chytridiomycosis on gray treefrog larvae (Hyla chrysoscelis). Copeia. 2004;2004:344–350. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Parris MJ, Cornelius TO. Fungal pathogen causes competitive and developmental stress in larval amphibian communities. Ecology. 2004;85:3385–3395. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Garner TWJ, Walker S, Bosch J, Leech S, Rowcliffe JM, et al. Life history tradeoffs influence mortality associated with the amphibian pathogen Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis. Oikos 2009 [Google Scholar]
- 25.Woodhams DC, Alford RA. Ecology of chytridiomycosis in rainforest stream frog assemblages of tropical Queensland. Conservation Biology. 2005;19:1449–1459. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Blaustein AR, Bancroft BA. Amphibian population declines: Evolutionary considerations. Bioscience. 2007;57:437–444. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Han BA, Bradley PW, Blaustein AR. Ancient behaviors of larval amphibians in response to an emerging fungal pathogen, Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2008;63:241–250. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Beiswenger RE. Diel Patterns of Aggregative Behavior in Tadpoles of Bufo-Americanus, in Relation to Light and Temperature. Ecology. 1977;58:98–108. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wilbur HM, Alford RA. Priority Effects in Experimental Pond Communities - Responses of Hyla to Bufo and Rana. Ecology. 1985;66:1106–1114. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gosner KL. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica. 1994;16:183–190. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wassersug RJ, Feder ME. The Effects of Aquatic Oxygen Concentration, Body Size and Respiratory Behavior on the Stamina of Obligate Aquatic (Bufo-Americanus) and Facultative Air-Breathing (Xenopus-Laevis and Rana-Berlandieri) Anuran Larvae. Journal of Experimental Biology. 1983;105:173–190. doi: 10.1242/jeb.105.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnson ML, Speare R. Survival of Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis in water: quarantine and disease control implications. Emerging Infectious Diseases. 2003;9:922–925. doi: 10.3201/eid0908.030145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Boyle DG, Boyle DB, Olsen V, Morgan JAT, Hyatt AD. Rapid quantitative detection of chytridiomycosis (Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis) in amphibian samples using real-time Taqman PCR assay. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms. 2004;60:141–148. doi: 10.3354/dao060141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Searle CL, Belden LK, Bancroft BA, Han BA, Biga LM, et al. Experimental examination of the effects of ultraviolet-B radiation in combination with other stressors on frog larvae. Oecologia. 2010;162:237–245. doi: 10.1007/s00442-009-1440-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lafferty KD, Dobson AP, Kuris AM. Parasites dominate food web links. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103:11211–11216. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604755103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kohler SL, Wiley MJ. Pathogen outbreaks reveal large-scale effects of competition in stream communities. Ecology. 1997;78:2164–2176. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hajek AE. Larval behavior in Lymantria dispar increases risk of fungal infection. Oecologia. 2001;126:285–291. doi: 10.1007/s004420000509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sheldon BC, Verhulst S. Ecological immunology: costly parasite defenses and trade-offs in evolutionary ecology. TREE. 1996;11:317–321. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10039-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hensley FR. Ontogenetic loss of phenotypic plasticity of age at metamorphosis in tadpoles. Ecology. 1993;74:2405–2412. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Leips J, Travis J. Metamorphic responses to changing food levels in two species of hylid frogs. Ecology. 1994;75:1345–1356. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Radder RS, Warner DA, Shine R. Compensating for a bad start: Catch-up growth in juvenile lizards (Amphibolurus muricatus, Agamidae). Journal of Experimental Zoology Part a-Ecological Genetics and Physiology. 2007;307A:500–508. doi: 10.1002/jez.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Deacon JW, Saxena G. Orientated zoospore attachment and cyst germination in Catenaria anguillulae, a facultative endoparasite of nematodes. Mycological Research. 1997;101:513–522. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ibelings BW, De Bruin A, Kagami M, Rijkeboer M, Brehm M, et al. Host parasite interactions between freshwater phytoplankton and chytrid fungi (Chytridiomycota). Journal of Phycology. 2004;40:437–453. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rollins-Smith LA. Metamorphosis and the amphibian immune system. Immunological Reviews. 1998;166:221–230. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-065x.1998.tb01265.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Venesky MD, Wassersug RJ, Parris MJ. How Does a Change in Labial Tooth Row Number Affect Feeding Kinematics and Foraging Performance of a Ranid Tadpole (Lithobates sphenocephalus)? Biological Bulletin. 2010;218:160–168. doi: 10.1086/BBLv218n2p160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rachowicz LJ, Vredenburg VT. Transmission of Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis within and between amphibian life stages. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms. 2004;61:75–83. doi: 10.3354/dao061075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]