Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol Rev. 2011 Nov 4;32(1):34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.10.003

Table 1.

Summary of Studies Examining the CaR-FA-X Model of Mechanisms Underlying Overgeneral Autobiographical Memory (OGM)

Authors (year) Participants (N) Measure of CaR-FA-X Mechanism(s) Assessment of OGMa Overall Findings Consistent with the CaR-FA-X Model
Capture and Rumination
Bessell et al. (2008) Adults with acquired brain injury (N = 58) Rumination vs. distraction induction AMT Y: consistent with rumination mechanism
Crane, Barnhofer, Visser et al. (2007) Adults with past MDD (N =34) Analytical/abstract vs. concrete/experiential self-focus induction AMT Y: consistent with rumination mechanism
Debeer et al. (2009) Undergraduate students (N = 314) RRS measure of rumination, including brooding and reflection subscales Standard and Minimal Instructions AMTs Y: consistent with rumination mechanism
Raes, Hermans, Williams, Geypen et al. (2006) Secondary school students (N = 112) Extent to which participants unscrambled sentences in ruminative vs. non-ruminative ways; RSS measure of rumination Overgeneral vs. specific retrieval style induction Findings suggest rumination OGM relationship is bidirectional
Raes et al. (2008) Undergraduate students (N = 195) Analytical/abstract vs. concrete/experiential self-focus induction SCEPT and forced-choice SCEPT Y: consistent with rumination mechanism
Sutherland & Bryant (2007) High (n = 26 and 48 for Studies 1 and 2) and low (n = 28 and 50 for Studies 1 and 2) dysphoric undergraduate students Rumination vs. distraction induction (Study 1); rumination induction on positive or negative content (Study 2 AMT Y: consistent with rumination mechanism
Functional Avoidance
Aglan et al. (2010) Adult women with (n = 46) and without (n = 57) a history of MDD History of CSA AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Boelen et al. (2010) Bereaved adults (N = 109) Extent to which specific memories were related vs. unrelated to the loss Standard and trait AMTs N: inconsistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Crane & Duggan (2009) Adults with recurrent suicidal behavior (N = 49) History of CSA AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Geraerts et al. (2010) Undergraduate students (N = 87) Thought suppression vs. control conditions for negative vs. neutral memories AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Golden et al. (2007) Bereaved adults with (n = 16) and without (n = 13) CG Extent to which memories were related to the self, the deceased, or a living significant other AMT, BMT for the deceased, and BMT for a living significant other N: inconsistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Hauer et al. (2008) Adults with (n = 70) and without (n = 63) CSA History of CSA Standard AMT and direct retrieval AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Hermans, de Decker et al. (2008) Undergraduate students (N = 39) Failure of an exam (unexpected aversive experience AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Johnson et al. (2005) Adolescents in a longitudinal study on family violence (N = 134) Exposure to family violence and sexual abuse between 6–12 and 12–18 years Asked to retrieve as many specific childhood memories to cues within 3min; coded for specificity N: inconsistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Lemogne et al. (2009) Healthy adults (N = 38) Avoidance symptoms on the IES-R Interview assessing several aspects of episodic memory (e.g., specificity) for positive and negative events Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Maccallum & Bryant (2010) Bereaved adults with (n = 24) and without (n = 21) CG Extent to which memories were related or unrelated to the loss AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
McNally et al. (2006) Adults with (n = 139) and without (n = 25) CSA Continuous memory, recovered memory, repressed memory, or no memory of CSA AMT for memories from before and after age 13 Mixed support for functional avoidance mechanism
Moradi et al. (2008) Adult refugees (N = 37; Study 1) Avoidance symptoms on the PDS AMT N: inconsistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Raes, Hermans, Williams, & Eelen (2006) Low- (n = 45) and high- (n = 45) specific undergraduate students (Study 1); low-specific female undergraduate students (n = 48; Study 2) Low- and high-specific tendencies; frustrating vs. neutral puzzle task (aversive experience vs. control) AMT Mixed support for functional avoidance mechanism
Raymaekers, Smeets, Peters, & Merckelbach (2010) Adults with (n = 86) and without (n = 26) CSA Continuous memory, recovered memory, or no memory of CSA AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Schönfeld & Ehlers (2006) Adult trauma survivors with (n = 29) and without (n = 26) PTSD Measures of cognitive strategies linked to avoidance Standard and pictorial AMTs Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Schönfeld et al. (2007) Adult assault survivors with (n = 14) and without (n = 28) PTSD Measures of cognitive strategies linked to avoidance AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Sinclair et al. (2007) Adults with a history of deliberate self-harm (n = 68) Reported history of CPA and CSA AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Valentino et al. (2009) Maltreated (n = 77) and non-maltreated (n = 115) children History of abuse (CSA or CPA) or neglect from Department of Human Services reports AMT Y: consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Impaired Executive Control
Birch & Davidson (2007) Dysphoric (n = 17) and non-dysphoric (n = 17) older adults WMS III working memory index subtests AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Dalgleish et al. (2007) 7 studies, one with adults with eating disorders, and others with adult community volunteersb Various measures of executive control; AMT manipulations that vary the role of executive control in OGM associated with depression AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Heeren et al. (2009) Healthy adults who completed a mindfulness training (n = 18) and matched controls (n = 18) Measures of cognitive and motor inhibition, and cognitive and behavioral flexibility AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Neshat-Doost et al. (2008) Adult community volunteers (N = 50) Color Stroop task vs. control tasks prior to the AMT AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Piolino et al. (2010) Younger (n = 50) and older (n = 50) healthy adults Measures of updating, inhibition, shifting, and feature binding in working memory VAF task; TEMPau task Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Raes et al. (2010) Primary school children (N = 135) Inhibitory Control subscale of the EATQ-R AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Ros et al. (2010) Younger (n = 50) and older (n = 46) healthy adults Measures of working memory AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Rutherford (2010) Undergraduate students (N = 80) Digit recall task during the AMT vs. control AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Yanes et al. (2008) Undergraduate students (N = 134) Assessed memory for task instructions AMT Y: consistent with executive control mechanism
Multiple Mechanisms
Barnhofer et al. (2007) Adults with (n = 16) and without (n = 19) past MDD Extent to which cues on the AMT were self-relevant; dual vs. single task conditions (AMT with and without random generation task) AMT Interaction between capture phenomenon and executive control mechanism
Dalgleish et al. (2007) Adult community volunteers with depressed mood (N = 32; Study 8) Performance on AMT-R can reflect functional avoidance or impaired executive control AMT-R Consistent with executive control mechanism
Dalgleish et al. (2008) Adult community volunteers with a trauma history (N = 36) Performance on AMT-R can reflect functional avoidance or impaired executive control AMT-R Consistent with functional avoidance mechanism
Raes, Hermans, Williams, Brunfaut et al. (2006) Adults with current MDD(N = 28) RRS measure of rumination; history of trauma on the TEC AMT Interaction between rumination and functional avoidance mech.
Raes, Hermans, Williams Demyttenaere et al. (2006) Adults with current MDD (N = 26) RSS measure of rumination; measure of working memory (letter-number sequencing) AMT Main effects of rumination and executive control mechanisms
Sumner, Griffith, & Mineka (2011) Undergraduate students (N = 109) RRS measure of rumination; extent to which cues on the AMT were self-relevant; measures of verbal fluency and inhibition AMT Main effects of capture and rumination, and executive control mechanisms
a

OGM reflects either fewer specific memories or more overgeneral memories.

b

See Multiple Mechanisms section for Study 8. AMT = Autobiographical Memory Test; MDD = major depressive disorder; RRS = Ruminative Responses Scale; RSS = Rumination on Sadness Scale; SCEPT = Sentence Completion for Events from the Past Test; CSA = childhood sexual abuse; CG = complicated grief; BMT = Biographical Memory Test; IES-R = Impact of Events Scale – Revised; PDS = Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale; PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; CPA = childhood physical abuse; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; VAF task = Verbal Autobiographical Fluency task; TEMPau task = Test Episodique de Mémoire du Passé autobiographique; EATQ-R = Revised Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire; AMT-R = Reversed Autobiographical Memory Test; TEC = Traumatic Experiences Checklist.