Table 1.
First author (year) | NC | NE | Age | Sample | Training domain | Effect Sizes | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Category | Stat | Source | Type | Effect | Size | ||||||
Rueda (2005) | 25a | 24 | 4 | Typically developing | Attention | IG p-p | d | C | N | small ↑ frontal ERP amplitude | — |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ reasoning | 0.82d | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ IQ | 0.63d | ||||||
Rueda (2005) | 12 | 12 | 6 | Typically developing | Attention | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ frontoparietal ERP amplitude | — |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ vocabulary | — | ||||||
Thorell (2009) | 30a | 17 | 4–5 | Typically developing | Visuospatial WM | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ spatial WM | 0.41 |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ verbal WM | 1.40 | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ attention | 0.65 | ||||||
Karbach (2009) | 42 | 14 | 8–10 | Typically developing | Task-switching | RM | d | R | N | ↓ switch costs | ~1.2 |
IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ inhibitory control | 0.44 | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ working memory | 0.60b | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ reasoning | 0.49 | ||||||
Stevens (2008) | 13 | 9 | 6–8 | Typically developing | Language via attention | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ receptive language | 1.43 |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ attentional ERP amplitude | 1.76 | ||||||
Stevens (2008) | 13 | 7 | 6–8 | Specific language impairment | Language via attention | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ receptive language | 3.91 |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ attentional ERP amplitude | 6.73 | ||||||
Holmes (2009) | 20 | 22 | 8–11 | Low WM | Verbal and visuospatial WM | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ verbal WM | 1.06b,c |
IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ visuospatial WM | 1.20b,c | ||||||
RM | d | R | F | ↑ math at follow-up | 0.49b,e | ||||||
Mackey (in press) | 11 | 17 | 7–9 | Low SES | Fluid reasoning | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ reasoning | 1.32 |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ WM | 0.88 | ||||||
Kerns (1999) | 7 | 7 | 7–11 | ADHD | Attention | RM | d | C | N | ↑ attention | 2.64b |
RM | d | C | N | ↓ interference | 3.06 | ||||||
RM | d | C | F | ↑ math | 1.94 | ||||||
Klingberg (2002) | 7 | 7 | 7–15 | ADHD | Verbal and visuospatial WM | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ visuospatial WM | 4.66b |
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ reasoning | 1.89 | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↓ ADHD | 1.44 | ||||||
Klingberg (2005) | 24 | 20 | 7–12 | ADHD | Verbal and visuospatial WM | IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ verbal WM | 0.68c |
IG p-p | d | C | N | ↑ visuospatial WM | 0.66c | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | N | ↓ interference | 0.25 | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↑ reasoning | 0.18 | ||||||
IG p-p | d | C | F | ↓ ADHD | 0.90c | ||||||
Shalev (2007) | 16 | 20 | 6–13 | ADHD | Attention | IG p-p | η2 | R | F | ↑ academic achievement | 0.15b |
IG p-p | η2 | R | F | ↓ ADHD | 0.29b |
Note. Age is given in years. See Supplementary Material for definitions and procedures for determining the effect sizes (Stat) based on the parameters given in the original studies.
NC = sample size of the control group; NE = sample size of the experimental/training group; WM = working memory; IG p-p = independent groups, pre-post; RM = repeated measures, change within group; Stat = the effect size statistic given; d = Cohen’s d; η2 = Eta-squared; C = effect size value calculated; R = effect size value as reported in the original study; N = near (improvement on trained tasks or nontrained, structurally similar tasks); F = far (improvement on structurally dissimilar tasks).
Multiple control groups used, combined N given.
Average effect size calculated from multiple dependent variables measuring the same construct.
Effect stable at follow-up test.
See note in Supplemental Material for calculation of these effect sizes.
Effect reported from follow-up test.