Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Econ J (London). 2012 Apr 12;122(560):418–448. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2012.02499.x

Table 5.

2SLS Estimates of the Effect of Education on Cognitive Abilities.

Males Females

Age < 16 h = 1 h = 2 h = 3 h = 1 h = 2 h = 3
Memory .597 *
(.346)
.511 **
(.227)
.434 **
(.187)
.512
(.341)
.521 *
(.274)
.352 *
(.193)
Exec. func. .635 *
(.357)
.547 **
(.223)
.371 **
(.185)
−.100
(.389)
.020
(.300)
.093
(.210)
N 431 928 1400 491 968 1435

Age < 19

Memory .504
(.872)
.267
(.299)
.174
(.309)
.359 *
(.214)
.213
(.190)
.220
(.147)
Exec. func. 1.085
(1.470)
.512
(.327)
.236
(.309)
−.052
(.229)
−.047
(.207)
.008
(.160)
N 636 1258 1860 718 1424 2163

Notes. Table reports 2SLS estimates of the effect of schooling on cognitive test scores. Columns denote the bandwidth selection h from 1 to 3 years. Rows indicate two different sample selection: (a) conditional on leaving before the age of 19; (b) conditional on leaving before the age of 16. All regressions include: a linear function of month of birth and its interaction with the reform dummy; controls for adult height and for survey year. The standard error in parenthesis are robust to heteroskedasticity and clustered at individual level. Significance levels:

(*)

p-values between 10 and 5 percent;

(**)

p-values between 5 and 1 percent;

(***)

p-values less than 1 percent.