Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol. 2012 Jun;25(3):201–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2012.02.003

Depression during Pregnancy among Young Couples: The Effect of Personal and Partner Experiences of Stressors and the Buffering Effects of Social Relationships

Anna A Divney 1, Heather Sipsma 1, Derrick Gordon 2, Linda Niccolai 1, Urania Magriples 3, Trace Kershaw 1
PMCID: PMC3359647  NIHMSID: NIHMS369801  PMID: 22578481

Abstract

Study Objective

To assess the relationship between personal and romantic partner’s experiences of stressful life events and depression during pregnancy, and the social moderators of this relationship among 296 young couples with low incomes from urban areas.

Participants and Setting

We recruited couples who were expecting a baby from four OB/GYN and ultrasound clinics in lower CT; women were ages 14-21 and male partners were 14+.

Design and Outcome Measures

We analyzed self-reports of stressful events in the previous six months, depression in the past week and current interpersonal social supports. To determine the influence of personal and partner experiences of stressful events on depression, we used multilevel dyadic models and incorporated interaction terms. We also used this model to determine whether social support, family functioning and relationship satisfaction moderated the association between stressful events and depression.

Results

Experiences of stressful life events were common; 91.2% of couples had at least one member report an event. Money, employment problems and moving were the most common events. Personal experiences of stressful life events had the strongest association with depression among men and women; although partner experiences of stressful life events were also significantly associated with depression among women. Social support, family functioning and romantic relationship satisfaction significantly buffered the association between personal and partner stressful events and depression.

Conclusion

Interventions that improve relationships, support systems, and family functioning may reduce the negative impact of stressors, experienced both personally and by a romantic partner, on the emotional well-being of young expectant parents.

Keywords: Stressful Life Events, Interpersonal Relationships, Depression, Pregnancy, Adolescents

Introduction

Adolescents and young adults with low incomes experience more stressful life events compared to their higher income peers.1,2 These stressors include violence, death or illness of a relative or friend, financial issues, relationship separation, and disruptions to employment and education. The accumulation of life stressors can result in negative psychological and physical health outcomes.1,3 The disproportionate accumulation of life stressors among disadvantaged populations may help explain disparities in health across socioeconomic status.4,5

The effects of stressful life events are particularly important during pregnancy. Women’s experiences of stressful life events are associated with increased depression during pregnancy,6 which has been associated with poor birth outcomes7,8 and postpartum depression.9 If left untreated, postpartum depression among both parents can negatively affect parent-infant bonding and subsequent child development. 10-14 The influence of stressful life events on depression is of particular importance among expecting adolescents and young adults, who are still cognitively and socially developing, and may have not yet developed effective coping skills and resources.15 Expecting adolescents may, therefore, be more susceptible to depression as a result of experiences of stressful life events. Targeting factors that protect against the depressive symptoms that result from experiences of stressors may help improve the health and well-being of young mothers, fathers and their children.

Social relationships have been associated with depression in both negative and positive ways.16-21 Relationships with a partner, friend or family member may cause stress and increase depressive symptoms.19,22 On the other hand, social relationships may buffer the effects of stressful life events on depression by providing resources, support and strength.18,20 Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model, which describes multiple levels of influence (eg. community, family, dyadic), is useful in exploring the factors that contribute to and buffer against the risk for depression.23 Extensive research shows that high social support,24,25 positive family functioning,26,27 and positive romantic relationship functioning28 are associated with less depression among adolescents and young adults.

Despite increasing evidence demonstrating that social relationships affect health,29,30 few studies have assessed how a person’s or their romantic partner’s experiences of stressful life events influences depression. Given the importance of interpersonal relationships, experiences of stressful life events by a partner may indirectly influence a person’s mental well-being. Expecting fathers have been neglected in research on experiences during pregnancy. In particular, few studies have obtained direct report of stressors and depression from both members of an expecting couple. Moreover, the literature lacks in an ecological understanding of the influences of stressful life events on depression among young expecting parents.

To that end, this paper aims to fill the gap in the current literature by describing an integrative model of stress, social resources and depression. First, we describe the experiences of stressful events among young, low income, and mostly minority couples expecting a baby. Consistent with epidemiological studies of the distribution of stressful life events among low income minority populations, we predict that recent stressful life events are common among the young couples, and that men and women experience similar total amounts of stressful life events. Second, we aim to determine whether the frequency of the partner’s experiences of stressful life events correlates with the individual’s depression. We hypothesize that both individual and partner experiences of stressful life events are associated with depression; however, their own experiences will have a stronger influence. Third, we aim to identify social relationship factors that moderate the relationship between stressful life events and depression. From an ecological approach, we predict that greater social support, family functioning and relationship satisfaction would all buffer the effect of stressful life events on depression. We also hypothesize that the moderation effects will differ by gender.

Materials and Methods

Procedures

Data from this study come from baseline interviews of 296 expectant young couples (women ages 14-21 and their male partners ages 14+). Couples were recruited from obstetrics and gynecology clinics and an ultrasound clinic in four university-affiliated hospitals in urban areas in lower Connecticut. Between July 2007 and February 2011, young women attending a prenatal care visit were referred by a health care provider or approached directly by research staff. Potential participants were screened and, if eligible, research staff explained the study in detail and answered any questions. If their baby’s father was not present at the time of screening, research staff asked for permission to contact their partner to explain the study. If willing, research staff provided informational materials for their partner and asked them to talk to their partner about the study. Research staff called them and their partner to answer any questions and, if interested, scheduled an appointment for their baseline interview.

Inclusion criteria included: (a) pregnant or partner is pregnant at greater than 23 weeks gestation; (b) women age 14-21 and men age greater than or equal to 14 at time of the interview; (c) both report being in a romantic relationship with each other; (d) both report being the biological parents of the unborn baby; (e) both agree to participate in the study and (f) both are able to speak English or Spanish. Participants were interviewed in their third trimester of pregnancy (M = 29.1 weeks gestational age). Because this was a longitudinal study we used an initial run-in period as eligibility criteria. Participants were ineligible if they could not be re-contacted after screening and before their due date.

Of the 413 eligible couples, 296 couples enrolled in the study (72.2% participation). Participants were more likely to be two weeks further along in pregnancy at screening compared to those who refused (p<.03). Participation did not vary by any other pre-screened demographic characteristic (all p>.05). Two men were missing data for stressful life events and therefore two couples were excluded from the analysis resulting in a final sample of 294 couples (n=588).

At the baseline appointment a research assistant obtained informed consent. The couples completed interviews via separate audio computer assisted self interviews (audio-CASI). Audio-CASI allows respondents to listen to questions on headphones that are also displayed on the computer’s screen. This technology helps participants who have lower reading skills complete the survey31 and elicits valid and more accurate responses .32,33 Participation was voluntary, confidential, and did not influence the provision of health care or social services. All procedures were approved by the Yale University Human Investigation Committee and by Institutional Review Boards at study clinics. Participants were paid $25 for their time.

Measures

Stressful Life Events

Stressful life events in the participants’ recent history were evaluated by means of an 11-item, adapted version of the Life Events Scale.34 Participants responded yes-or-no to questions about whether they experienced each of 11 stressful life events in the previous 6 months, such as having a serious illness, having a death in the family, or having money problems (Table 1). Summing responses to all items formed the total stressful life events score. Higher values correspond to more stressful life events. The checklist approach to measuring stressful life events is the most common approach to measuring variation in experiences of stress.35 In this approach, the level of experience of stress is conceptualized as the cumulative amount of change or readjustment that results from experiencing negative life events.35-37 To avoid confounding of the stress exposure with the effects of stress, which can occur in stress inventories that allow subjective weighting of the impact of a stressor, this analysis independently assesses the participant’s stress exposure (stressful life events scale) and response (depression) as suggested by Turner and Wheaton.35 Items in this measure showed acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.69. A slightly lower internal consistency compared to other measures in this study is not concerning because it is an index. Indices often have lower reliability scores since a participant’s endorsement of one item should not necessarily be related to the endorsement of another item if the events are independent.38

Table 1.

Frequencies of stressful life events among young men, women, and couples

Type of stressful life event Men Women p-value Couples
money problems 133 (45.2%) 132 (44.6%) 0.922 184 (62.6%)
became unemployed or seeking
work unsuccessfully
98 (33.1%) 77 (26.0%) 0.041 140 (47.6%)
moved 86 (29.1%) 106 (35.8%) 0.067 144 (48.6%)
family or friend die 77 (26.2%) 71 (24.0%) 0.637 130 (44.2%)
something valued was lost or stolen 66 (22.4%) 49 (16.6%) 0.089 102 (34.7%)
serious problem with a friend,
neighbor, or relative
48 (16.3%) 51 (17.2%) 0.818 87 (29.6%)
relative suffered a serious illness,
injury, or assault
49 (16.6%) 52 (17.6%) 0.813 86 (29.3%)
problems at work or school 36 (12.2%) 35 (13.4%) 0.801 69 (23.5%)
problems with the police 41 (13.9%) 14 (4.7%) <0.001 54 (18.4%)
suffered a serious illness, injury, or
assault
27 (9.2%) 14 (4.7%) 0.053 40 (13.6%)
broke off a steady relationship 21 (7.1%) 10 (3.4%) 0.108 30 (9.7%)
Total with at least one stressful life
event reported in the past 6 months
221 (75.2%) 226 (76.4%) .136 268 (91.2%)
Mean ± SD Mean ± SD p-value Mean ± SD
Total stressful life events 2.3 ± 2.24 2.1 ± 1.93 .132 3.6 ± 2.48

Depression

We measured Depression using 15 of the 20-items in the Center of Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D).39 The CES-D has been used extensively in community settings and among pregnant populations.6,39-41 The CES-D has also been shown to be reliable and valid among men and women, adolescents and racially and ethnically diverse populations.39,42,43 The five somatic manifestations of depression items were removed because they were potentially confounded by symptoms of pregnancy, including the items “did not feel like eating,” “trouble keeping mind on what I was doing,” “everything I did was an effort,” “my sleep was restless,” and “could not get going.”44 A recent study shows that removing these five somatic items does not affect the psychometric properties or predictive capacity of the scale among pregnant women.45 For each symptom of depression, participants indicated how often they felt or behaved in the previous week, ranging from 0 = “Less than 1 day a week” to 3 = “Most of the time (5-7 days a week).” Summing responses to all items formed the depression score, with higher scores indicating more depressive symptoms. Reliability for this measure was very good (α = 0.82).

Moderators

Social support was measured using an 9-item scale adapted from the MOS Social Support Survey.46 Participants indicated how often, on a 5-point scale, others were available to them for companionship, assistance, and other forms of support. Responses ranged from 0 = “None of the time” to 4 = “All of the time.” Summing responses to all items formed the social support score. Higher values correspond to more social support. Reliability for this measure was very good (α = 0.95).

Family functioning was assessed using a 12-item scale adapted from the Family Functioning Scale (FFS). Participants responded to statements concerning 5 general dimensions of family functioning: positive family affect, family communication family conflicts, family worries, and family rituals/supports. Participants indicated how accurate the statements were in describing their family on a 7-point scale and a total family functioning score was calculated by summing responses to all items.47 Higher scores correspond to greater family functioning. Reliability for this measure was very good (α = 0.81).

Relationship satisfaction was measured using the 32-item Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). Sample items include, “How often do you or your partner leave the house after a fight,” and “Do you kiss your partner?” A total relationship satisfaction score was computed by summing responses to all items and higher scores indicate greater relationship satisfaction.48 Reliability for this measure was very good (α = 0.92).

Analysis

We generated descriptive statistics using frequencies and means to describe demographic characteristics of our sample population and the main outcome variable, depression. We then examined bivariate correlations of the demographic characteristics with total stressful life events and depression. Next, we described the stress experiences of men, women and couples by generating frequencies for each stressful life event and generating the means for our composite score. McNemar’s test was used to compare the frequencies of each life event between men and women and a paired t-test was used to examine differences for our overall composite score.

To examine the relationships between stressful life events and depression, we used multilevel modeling to account for the correlated nature of the data. To assess the influence of both the person (actor) and the partner (partner effect), the Actor-Partner Independence Model was used. Actor effects refer to whether a person’s score on a predictor variable influences the person’s own outcome (e.g. woman’s stressful life experiences relates to her own depression). Partner effects refer to whether a partner’s score on the predictor variable influences another person’s outcome (e.g., male partner’s stressful life events influences woman’s depression). The Actor-Partner Independence Model incorporates responses from both members of a dyad into a single analysis. Multilevel modeling (or hierarchical linear modeling) to estimate Actor-Partner Independence Model treats the members of a dyad as nested scores within the same group.49 A detailed description on how to conduct APIM analyses using multilevel modeling programs has been previously outlined (e.g., Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006) and served as the guide for our analysis plan. To assess whether gender moderated the influence of stressful life events on depression, gender by stressful life events interaction terms were tested. All modeling controlled for age, education, income and race/ethnicity. Modeling also controlled for whether the participant reported s/he was trying to get pregnant (yes or no) for this pregnancy, as pregnancy intentions have been related to depression among expecting adolescents and young adults.6,50 As suggested by Turner and Wheaton, the analyses also controlled for role occupancy as defined in this analysis as whether they have other children and whether they were working or going to school.35 Participants with more roles may be more at risk for some stressful life events in the inventory (ie. working participants are more likely to have experienced problems at work). Participants with more roles may have also developed better coping skills or social competence to allow them to function in an expanded number of roles, which, unaccounted for, would mute the association between stressful life events and depression.35

To determine whether the relationship between stressful life events and depression was moderated by social factors, we included 3-way interaction terms with stressful life events, gender, and our proposed moderators (social support, family functioning and relationship satisfaction) plus their lower order terms in our multilevel models. A complete case analysis was conducted due to the small number of missing data. PASW Statistics 20 was used for all analyses.

Results

Demographics

Demographic characteristics of the sample and mean depression scores for men and women are displayed in Table 2. This table also shows the bivariate correlations of the demographic variables with total stressful life events and depression.

Table 2.

Demographic characteristics and bivariate correlations with total stressful life events and depression.

Female Male P-value Total Stressful
Life Events
Depression

N (%) N (%) r r
Gendera 296 (50.0%) 296 (50.0%) --- 0.058 −0.118**
Race/Ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic black 117 (39.5%) 144 (48.6%) 0.069 0.014
 Hispanic 117 (39.5%) 108 (36.5%) 0.001 −0.115** 0.003
 Non-Hispanic white 50 (16.9%) 31 (10.5%) REF REF
 Other 12 (4.1%) 13 (4.4%) −0.004 0.043
First child 233 (79.0%) 221 (75.7%) 0.308 −0.024 −0.060
Trying to get pregnant 97 (32.8%) 113 (38.8%) <0.001 −0.019 −0.017
Currently employed 84 (28.4%) 178 (60.5%) <0.001 −0.164*** −0.083*
Currently in school 117 (39.5%) 79 (26.9%) <0.001 0.074 0.045

Mean ± SD Mean ± SD r r

Age (years) 18.7 ± 1.63 21.3 ± 4.06 <0.001 0.001 −0.024
Years in school 11.7 ± 1.82 11.8 11.7 ± 1.89 0.456 0.005 0.002
Personal income $5,846 ± $7,458 $10,927 ± $11,878 <0.001 −0.069 −0.086*
Relationship Length (months) 26.5 ± 19.5 27.2 ± 20.0 0.070 −0.024 −0.015
Depression 10.55 ± 7.39 8.88 ±-6.62 0.002 0.249*** ---
*

P<.05

**

P<.01

***

P<.001

a

Gender coded as female=0, male=1

Prevalence of stressful life events among men, women and couples

Stressful life events in the previous six months were common with 75.2% of women, 76.4% of men and 91.2% of couples experiencing at least one stressful life event in the previous 6 months (Table 1). The two most common stressful life events were related to money problems and employment. Men were more likely to report employment problems (p=.041) and were more likely to report problems with the police (p<.001).

Personal and partner effects of stressful life events on depression (see Table 3)

Table 3.

Regression model examining relationship between stressful life events (own and partner’s) and Depression.

Depression

B SE t p
Total stressful life events (Actor) .765 .135 5.683 <.001

Total stressful life events (Partner) .334 .135 2.475 .014

Gender
 Female −2.362 .593 −3.982 <.001
 Male REF REF REF

Age .122 .102 1.190 .235

Years in school .071 .162 .438 .661

Personal income −.039 .029 −1.306 .192

Race/ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic black 1.245 .930 1.339 .181
 Hispanic 1.960 .937 2.092 .037
 Non-Hispanic white REF REF REF REF
 Other 3.419 1.559 2.193 .029

First Child −.573 .683 −.838 .402

Trying to get pregnant .031 .588 .052 .958

Currently employed .652 .621 1.050 .294

Currently in school −.201 .640 −.313 .754

In a multilevel regression model adjusted for age, education, income, race/ethnicity, whether this was their first child, whether they were in school or working and whether they were trying to get pregnant, there was a significant relationship between personal total stressful life events and depression, with more total stressful life events predicting more depression (B=.769, SE=.136, t=5.673, p<.001). There was also a significant relationship between partner stressful life events and depression (B=.336, SE=.136, t=2.484, p=.013), with more total stressful life events of the partner relating to more depression for the person.

Gender as a moderator

Gender did not interact with the relationship between personal total stressful life events and depression (t=-1.374, p=.170). However, gender did interact with the relationship between partner’s total stressful life events and depression (t=1.972, p=.049). Partner’s increased stressful life events related to increased depression among women (B=.600, SE=.189, t=3.169, p=.002), however, this relationship did not exist among men (B=0.060, SE=.195, t=0.309 p=.758).

Social support as a moderator

Gender did not have a 3-way interaction with the relationship between own stressful life events, social support and depression (t=.292, p=.770), nor did it have a 3-way interaction with the relationship between partner’s stressful life events, own social support and own depression (t=0.581, p=.561). However, in the entire sample, social support moderated the relationship between own stressful life events and depression (t=-2.686, p=.007). To explore the nature of this interaction we estimated the simple effects by examining the equations at the mean and one standard deviation above and below the mean. Among the highest tertile of social support, stressful life events was not associated with depression (B=.346, SE=.209, t=1.654, p=.099). As social support decreased, the relationship between stressful life events and depression became stronger. Among the middle tertile of social support, more total stressful life events was associated with more depression (B=.701, SE=.130, t=5.377, p<.001). Among the lowest tertile of social support, more total stressful life events were even more strongly associated with more depression (B=1.056, SE=.159, t=6.651, p<.001). In the entire sample, social support did not moderate the relationship between partner’s stressful life events and depression (t=−0.739, p=.460).

Family functioning as a moderator

Gender did not have a 3-way interaction with the relationship between partner’s stressful life events, own family functioning and own depression (t=.164, p=.869). In the entire sample, family functioning did not moderate the relationship between partner’s stressful life events and depression (t=−.097, p=.923). Gender did have a 3-way interaction with the relationship between family functioning, own stressful life events and depression (t=2.067, p=.039). To explore the nature of this interaction we estimated the simple effects among men and women separately by examining the equations at the mean and one standard deviation above and below the mean. Among women, as family functioning increased, the relationship between stressful life events and depression weakened. Among women in the highest tertile of family functioning, stressful life events were not associated with depression (B=.030, SE=.345, t=0.088, p=.930). Among women in the middle tertile of family functioning, more stressful life events was associated with more depression (B=.498, SE=.232, t=2.142, p=.033) and the strongest relationship between stressful life events and depression was found among women in the lowest tertile of family functioning (B=.965, SE=.255, t=3.783, p<.001). Among men in all groups, as family functioning increased, the relationship between stressful life events and depression slightly increased, but did not vary as significantly across levels of family functioning compared to women. Men with the highest tertile of family functioning had the strongest relationship with stressful life events and depression (B=.581, SE=.281, t=2.068, p=.040). This relationship between stressful life events and depression was slightly weaker among men in the middle tertile of family functioning (B=.490, SE=.177, t=2.767, p=.006) and was weakest for men among the lowest tertile of family functioning (B=.400, SE=.198, t=2.018, p=.045).

Relationship Satisfaction as a moderator

Gender did not have a 3-way interaction with the relationship between own stressful life events, relationship satisfaction and depression (t=.857, p=.392) nor partner stressful life events, relationship satisfaction and depression (t=−.132 p=.895). In the entire sample, relationship satisfaction did not moderate the relationship between own stressful life events and depression (t=1.098, p=.273). There was a trend toward significant moderation of the relationship between partner’s stressful life events and depression by relationship satisfaction (t=-1.906, p=.057). To explore the nature of this interaction we estimated the simple effects by examining the equations at the mean and one standard deviation above and below the mean. Among the highest tertile of relationship satisfaction, partner stressful life events were not associated with depression (B=.091, SE=.172, t=0.530, p=.597). The association between partner stressful life events and depression increased as relationship satisfaction decreased. Among those in the middle tertile of relationship satisfaction, more partner stressful life events was associated with more depression (B=.283, SE=.113, t=2.515, p=.012) and the strongest relationship between partner’s stressful life events and depression was found among those in the lowest tertile of relationship satisfaction (B=.477, SE=.163, t=2.923, p=.004).

Discussion

Consistent with previous research,1,2 stressful life events were very common among young men and women and even more common when considered as a couple. Over 90% of couples had a member report a stressful life event in the previous 6 months. The distribution of individual stressful life events varied by gender; men had more problems with unemployment and police. Because women were younger and pregnant, they may have been less likely to work or seek work and less likely to engage in activities that may involve the police. Money problems were the leading stressful life events reported overall. Couples may be grappling with how they will financially support their new baby in an already economically strained situation.

More experiences of stressful life events in the previous 6 months related to increased depression among both men and women. Among women, her partner’s experiences of stressful life events related to her increased depression in addition to her own experiences of stressors. Understanding the contribution of one’s own and partner’s experiences of stressors to the health of pregnant women is of great importance as depression is associated with maternal and child health. 10,12

This study shows that social relationships can positively affect the health of young expecting couples. Among both young men and women, the relationship between stressful life events and depression disappeared among those with the highest amount of social support, while those with the least social support reported the strongest association between stressors and depression. Family functioning also buffered the association between stressful life events and depression among young women. Unexpectedly, family functioning did not buffer the association between stressful life events and depression among young men. For men with the most family functioning, stressful life events had the strongest association with increased depression, although the trend across levels of family functioning was slight and less dramatic compared to women. This finding suggests that family functioning may operate differently among young men compared to women. A better understanding of masculine identity and independence among young expecting men may help explain this finding.

Relationship satisfaction also moderated the association between partner’s stressful life events and depression among both men and women, although the trend was not significant. Among those with the greatest relationship satisfaction, partner stressful life events did not influence depression. Among those with the lowest relationship satisfaction, partner stressful life events had a strong significant effect on depression. Evidence from this study shows that romantic partners can have both positive and negative effects on the health of young expecting adults. Men are often a forgotten group in research and programs in reproductive and child health; however, this study demonstrates that men’s experiences and support influences the health of pregnant women.

While this study advances our understanding of the influence of stressful life events on depression and the social buffers of this influence, it does have limitations. First, because this analysis is cross-sectional, we are unable to determine causality between stressful life events and depression. The analysis is temporally feasible, however, as stressors were reported for the previous 6 months while depression was ascertained related to feelings in the previous week. Future longitudinal studies focused on the accumulation of stressors from pregnancy to postpartum may help us better understand the direction of the pathways between stress and health across generations. In addition, because stressful life events and depression were self-reported in this study, the measures are subject to recall-bias, which may lead to misestimation of the associations. ACASI, however, has been shown to allow for more accurate reporting of sensitive topics32 and has been useful in obtaining psychosocial risk information during pregnancy.33 Stress is also a complex construct and its relationship with depression may vary along different dimensions, including the type and timing of the stressful life event, which are not captured in this analysis. It is possible that the effects of the individual events may differ across individuals; however, we did not weight events by a subjective assessment of the event. Subjective weighting of the impact of a stressor may largely be a function of an individual’s coping skills and resources,35 which may be related to depression. This analysis independently assessed the individual’s exposure (events) and response (depression). Future studies that identify the differential effects of stressors across type and timing will continue to clarify the relationship between stressors and depression during pregnancy. Last, the stressful life events scale could not have captured all of the potential stressors these young couples experienced during pregnancy, such as feelings of stigma or shame associated with being pregnant, which may also be related to emotional or psychological distress. 51,52

Many of the stressful life experiences the young couples reported likely stem from their social context, including problems related to money, unemployment and theft. In the absence of structural interventions that improve the social and environmental living conditions for low income populations, understanding the factors that buffer the effects of stressful life events on depression is important, particularly during pregnancy and early parenthood. This study demonstrates the importance of romantic partners during pregnancy in both contributing to and buffering against mental health risks. Incorporating discussions of relationships, social supports and stress into prenatal care may help identify women in need of resources. Interventions that improve romantic relationships and social support systems may significantly reduce the negative impact of social stressors on the mental health of young expecting parents.

Acknowledgements of Financial Support

We would like to thank The National Institute of Mental Health (Grant #5R01MH075685) for funding this research.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflicts of Interest: The authors of this paper have no conflicts of interest to report.

References

  • 1.Hatch SL, Dohrenwend BP. Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: a review of the research. Am J Community Psychol. 2007;40(3-4):313–332. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gallo LC, Matthews KA. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: do negative emotions play a role? Psychol Bull. 2003 Jan;129(1):10–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barreto S, McManus M. Casting the net for “depression” among ethnic minority children from the high-risk urban communities. Clin Psychol Rev. 1997;17(8):823–845. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00054-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Matthews KA, Gallo LC. Psychological perspectives on pathways linking socioeconomic status and physical health. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:501–530. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gallo LC, Bogart LM, Vranceanu AM, Matthews KA. Socioeconomic status, resources, psychological experiences, and emotional responses: a test of the reserve capacity model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005 Feb;88(2):386–399. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lancaster CA, Gold KJ, Flynn HA, Yoo H, Marcus SM, Davis MM. Risk factors for depressive symptoms during pregnancy: a systematic review. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2010;202(1):5–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2009.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Deave T, Heron J, Evans J, Emond A. The impact of maternal depression in pregnancy on early child development. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology. 2008;115(8):1043–1051. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2008.01752.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, Melville JL, Iyengar S, Katon WJ. A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(10):1012–1024. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Paulson JF, Bazemore SD. Prenatal and postpartum depression in fathers and its association with maternal depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2010 May 19;303(19):1961–1969. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cogill SR, Caplan HL, Alexandra H, Robson KM, Kumar R. Impact of maternal postnatal depression on cognitive development of young children. British Medical Journal (Clinical research ed.) 1986 May 3;292(6529):1165–1167. doi: 10.1136/bmj.292.6529.1165. 1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kim P, Swain JE. Sad dads: paternal postpartum depression. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 2007 Feb;4(2):35–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stein A, Gath D, Bucher J, Bond A, Day A, Cooper P. The relationship between post-natal depression and mother-child interaction. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1991 Jan 1;158(1):46–52. doi: 10.1192/bjp.158.1.46. 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Field T. Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting, and safety practices: a review. Infant behavior & development. 2010 Feb;33(1):1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wilson S, Durbin CE. Effects of paternal depression on fathers’ parenting behaviors: a metaanalytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010 Mar;30(2):167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Compas BE. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence. Psychol Bull. 1987 May;101(3):393–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Boelen PA, Reijntjes A. Negative cognitions in emotional problems following romantic relationship break-ups. Stress and Health. 2009;25(1):11–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brendgen M, Lamarche V, Wanner B, Vitaro F. Links between friendship relations and early adolescents’ trajectories of depressed mood. Dev Psychol. 2010 Mar;46(2):491–501. doi: 10.1037/a0017413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hoffman S, Hatch MC. Stress, social support and pregnancy outcome: a reassessment based on recent research. Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology. 1996 Oct;10(4):380–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.1996.tb00063.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Taylor RD, Seaton E, Dominguez A. Kinship Support, Family Relations, and Psychological Adjustment Among Low-Income African American Mothers and Adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2008;18(1):1–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006 Aug;29(4):377–387. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Westman M. Stress and Strain Crossover. Human Relations. 2001 Jun 1;54(6):717–751. 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Adam EK, Chyu L, Hoyt LT, et al. Adverse adolescent relationship histories and young adult health: cumulative effects of loneliness, low parental support, relationship instability, intimate partner violence, and loss. The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 2011 Sep;49(3):278–286. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological systems theory. In: Vasta R, editor. Annals of child development. Vol 6. JAI Press; Greenwich, Conn: 1989. pp. 187–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kawachi I, Berkman L. Social ties and mental health. Journal of Urban Health. 2001;78(3):458–467. doi: 10.1093/jurban/78.3.458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Koniak-Griffin D, Walker DS, de Traversay J. Predictors of depression symptoms in pregnant adolescents. J Perinatol. 1996;16(1):69–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Keitner G, Miller I. Family functioning and major depression: an overview. Am J Psychiatry. 1990 Sep 1;147(9):1128–1137. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.9.1128. 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sarmiento IA, Cardemil EV. Family Functioning and Depression in Low-Income Latino Couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 2009;35(4):432–445. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2009.00139.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Murry VM, Harrell AW, Brody GH, et al. Long-Term Effects of Stressors on Relationship Well-Being and Parenting Among Rural African American Women. Family Relations. 2008;57(2):117–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2008.00488.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The spread of obesity in large social networks over 32 years. New England Journal of Medicine. 2007;357:370–379. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa066082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Umberson D, Montez JK. Social relationships and health: a flashpoint for health policy. J Health Soc Behav. 2010;51(Suppl):S54–66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Des Jarlais DC, Turner CV. Audio-CASI versus interviewer obtained survey from participants in syringe exchange programs. Paper presented at: North American Syringe Exchange Convention; Baltimore, Maryland. 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Latkin C, Vlahov D, Anthony J. Socially Desirable Responding and Self-Reported HIV Infection Risk Behaviors Among Intravenous Drug Users. Addiction. 1993;38:517–526. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02058.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.C’de Baca J, Lapham SC, Skipper BJ, Watkins ML. Use of computer interview data to test associations between risk factors and pregnancy outcomes. Computers and Biomedical Research. 1997;30:232–243. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1997.1449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brugha TS, Cragg D. The List of Threatening Experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1990 Jul;82(1):77–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Turner R, Wheaton B. Checklist Measurement of Stressful Life Events. In: Cohen S, Kessler R, Gordon L, editors. Measuring Stress. Oxford University Press; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. Journal of psychosomatic research. 1967 Aug;11(2):213–218. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Paykel ES, Prusoff BA, Uhlenhuth EH. Scaling of life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1971 Oct;25(4):340–347. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750160052010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. The effects of test reliability on relationships between measures of life events and depression. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1986;21(2):53–62. doi: 10.1007/BF00578743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Phillips GS, Wise LA, Rich-Edwards JW, Stampfer MJ, Rosenberg L. Prepregnancy depressive symptoms and preterm birth in the Black Women’s Health Study. Annals of epidemiology. 2010 Jan;20(1):8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sharp LK, Lipsky MS. Screening for depression across the lifespan: a review of measures for use in primary care settings. American family physician. 2002 Sep 15;66(6):1001–1008. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Radloff LS. The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1991;20(2):149–166. doi: 10.1007/BF01537606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Roberts RE. Reliability of the CES-D Scale in different ethnic contexts. Psychiatry research. 1980 May;2(2):125–134. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(80)90069-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Allen MJYW. Introduction to measurement theory. Waveland Press; Prospect Heights (IL): 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kabir K, Sheeder J, Stevens-Simon C. Depression, weight gain, and low birth weight adolescent delivery: do somatic symptoms strengthen or weaken the relationship? Journal of pediatric and adolescent gynecology. 2008 Dec;21(6):335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2008.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Soc Sci Med. 1991;32(6):705–714. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90150-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Tavitian ML, Lubiner JL, Green L, Grebstein LC, Velicer WF. Dimensions of Family Functioning. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality. 1987;2:191–204. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Spanier GB. Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scale for assessing the quality of marriage and similiar dyads. Journal of Marriage & the Family. 1976;38:15–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Cook WL. Dyadic Data Analysis. The Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gipson JD, Koenig MA, Hindin MJ. The effects of unintended pregnancy on infant, child, and parental health: a review of the literature. Studies in family planning. 2008 Mar;39(1):18–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4465.2008.00148.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Whitehead E. Teenage pregnancy: on the road to social death. International journal of nursing studies. 2001 Aug;38(4):437–446. doi: 10.1016/s0020-7489(00)00086-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wiemann CM, Rickert VI, Berenson AB, Volk RJ. Are pregnant adolescents stigmatized by pregnancy? The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 2005 Apr;36(4):352, e351–358. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES