Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Commun. 2012 Apr 24;62(3):454–472. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01643.x

Media Literacy Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review

Se-Hoon Jeong 1, Hyunyi Cho 2, Yoori Hwang 3
PMCID: PMC3377317  NIHMSID: NIHMS364075  PMID: 22736807

Abstract

Although numerous media literacy interventions have been developed and delivered over the past 3 decades, a comprehensive meta-analytic assessment of their effects has not been available. This study investigates the average effect size and moderators of 51 media literacy interventions. Media literacy interventions had positive effects (d=.37) on outcomes including media knowledge, criticism, perceived realism, influence, behavioral beliefs, attitudes, self-efficacy, and behavior. Moderator analyses indicated that interventions with more sessions were more effective, but those with more components were less effective. Intervention effects did not vary by the agent, target age, the setting, audience involvement, the topic, the country, or publication status.

Keywords: media literacy, intervention, meta-analysis, persuasion, resistance, critical, knowledge


Harmful effects of the media have been documented in a range of domains, including violence (e.g., Paik & Comstock, 1994), sexual behavior (e.g., Allen, D’Alessio, & Brezgel, 1995; Hestroni, 2007), and body image (e.g., Holmstrom, 2004). Media literacy interventions refer to education programs designed to reduce harmful effects of the media by informing the audience about one or more aspects of the media, thereby influencing media-related beliefs and attitudes, and ultimately preventing risky behaviors.

Several studies have reviewed the effects of media literacy interventions on topics such as violence (Cantor & Wilson, 2003), sexual behavior (Allen, D’Alessio, Emmers, & Gebhardt, 1996), and advertising (Livingstone & Helsper, 2006). A review by Bergsma and Carney (2008) examined the contexts and process of effective media literacy interventions focusing on health promotion. However, a comprehensive meta-analysis of media literacy interventions is not yet available. The present study assesses the average effect size of media literacy interventions and the conditions under which such interventions are more effective. We begin with a brief overview of the conceptual basis for media literacy interventions, followed by a review of outcomes and moderators of media literacy intervention effects.

Media Literacy Interventions

Conceptual Basis

Scholars have advanced divergent conceptualizations of media literacy and have failed to reach a broad consensus on the definition of media literacy.1 However, they generally agree that media literacy centers on specific knowledge and skills that can help critical understanding and usage of the media (e.g., Hobbs, 1998; McCannon, 2009; Martens, 2010). For example, Aufderheide (1993) has defined media literacy as “the ability to access, analyze, evaluate, and communicate messages in a variety of forms,” and has suggested that the fundamental objective of media literacy is to help audiences maintain “critical autonomy in relationship to all media.” Silverblatt (2001) defined media literacy as a “critical thinking skill that allows audiences to develop independent judgments about media content.” Critical thinking is relevant not only to receiving messages but also to producing meaning (Kellner & Share, 2005) and “learning to create one’s own messages” (Hobbs, 1998).

Some scholars have further differentiated the types of media literacy. For example, Meyrowitz (1998) classified media literacy into content, grammar, and medium literacy. Content literacy concerns knowledge of ideas and values represented in media messages. The knowledge that media content represents embedded values and points of view is central to the concept of media literacy. Grammar literacy focuses on knowledge of the techniques used in textual and visual messages, such as angles, cuts, zooms, and juxtaposition. Medium literacy concerns knowledge of different characteristics of the media. For example, some media are unisensory whereas others are multisensory. In addition, structure literacy concerns knowledge of the context of media production and consumption, such as the role of media institutions in the production of media messages (Lewis & Jhally, 1998). Knowledge of how commercial media differ from public media can be a form of structure literacy.2

We view that media literacy intervention aims to enhance criticism by increasing knowledge of the media, awareness of the influence of the media, and the ability to assess the realism of the media representation of reality. By doing so, media literacy interventions are expected to reduce the impact of the media on audiences’ beliefs, attitudes, norms, and behaviors. This meta-analysis examines the extent to which extant media literacy interventions have been able to influence these outcomes.

Theoretical Perspectives

Prior media literacy studies have examined various outcomes, some of which are relevant to understanding of the media and others are relevant to behaviors. A broad range of outcomes were examined in the studies because they were based on various theoretical frameworks. Inoculation theory (McGuire, 1964) has provided a rationale for media literacy education.3 The theory consists of threat and refutational preemption components that can protect the audience against subsequent attacks. Thus, by providing knowledge and skills to refute media messages, literacy interventions may help audiences to resist the influence of harmful media content.

Although inoculation theory was not developed specifically for media literacy education, some models were. For example, Potter’s (2004) cognitive processing model of media literacy suggests that knowledge structures and skills are fundamental to the acquisition of media literacy. Knowledge structures are sets of organized information in an individual and provide the contexts with which individuals make sense of media messages. Skills include those relevant to analysis, evaluation, grouping, induction, deduction, synthesis, and abstraction. The message interpretation process (MIP; Austin et al., 2002) model is another theoretical framework specific to media literacy. The MIP model suggests that messages guide decision making through logical and affective processes such as skepticism, realism, norms, and outcome expectancies. The MIP model includes affective aspects of media-based decisions, which distinguishes it from cognitively-focused frameworks such as inoculation theory and Potter’s model. In addition, Messaris’s theory of visual persuasion (1997) is relevant to media literacy. According to Messaris, visual persuasion may be more effective than verbal persuasion because of the indexical, iconic, and syntactic characteristics of visual signs. Thus, Messaris (1997) argued that learning the grammar of visual media may be useful for resisting the influence of media messages.

Outcomes

The above conceptualizations and theories suggest a range of outcomes for media literacy interventions. Such outcomes may be classified into two broad categories: media-relevant outcomes and behavior-relevant outcomes. Austin et al.’s (2002), Messaris’s (1997), and Potter’s (2004) models suggest that interventions can influence media-relevant outcomes. One type of media-relevant outcome is knowledge, including knowledge of specific construction techniques used to persuade audiences (e.g., Harts, 1997; Hobbs & Frost, 2003) and knowledge about advertising (e.g., Buijzen, 2007). Another type is criticism, including concepts such as understanding of persuasive intent (Austin & Johnson, 1997a) and skepticism (Austin et al., 2005). The third type is influence, which refers to one’s awareness of the influence of the media on audiences. Finally, realism refers to the extent to which one believes that the portrayal of persons or events in the media corresponds with those in the real world. Previous studies have found that interventions can increase the audience’s media knowledge (Austin et al., 2005), media criticism (e.g., Austin et al., 2005), and awareness of media influence (e.g., Duran et al., 2008) while reducing the perceived realism of media messages (e.g., Austin et al., 2007).

The MIP model further suggests that interventions can influence behavior-relevant outcomes. On the basis of the theory of reasoned action (TRA; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) and the integrative model of behavior change (IM; Fishbein & Yzer, 2003), behavioral outcomes can be classified into behavioral beliefs, attitudes, norms, self-efficacy, and behaviors. Behavioral beliefs concern one’s perception about the outcomes of performing a behavior; attitudes refer to one’s overall evaluation of performing a behavior; norms refer to one’s perception about the behaviors and attitudes of a social reference group (e.g., friends or family); and self-efficacy refers to one’s perceived ability to perform a behavior. Previous studies have found that interventions can reduce the frequency of risky or antisocial behaviors (Austin et al., 2005) as well as behavioral intentions (Banerjee & Greene, 2007) by inducing negative attitudes (e.g., Banerjee & Greene, 2006) toward and negative behavioral beliefs (Austin et al., 2007; Gonzales et al., 2004) about such behaviors. In addition, media literacy interventions can reduce the likelihood of an individual engaging in the behaviors by reducing normative pressure (Austin et al., 2005) and increasing self-efficacy (Austin et al., 2005).

Based on the above discussion, we hypothesize that media literacy interventions will increase audiences’ knowledge, criticism, awareness of the influence of the media, while reducing their perceptions of realism. Further, we hypothesize that media literacy interventions will reduce risky or antisocial behaviors, increase negative behavioral beliefs about and negative attitudes toward such behaviors, reduce normative beliefs about such behaviors, and increase self-efficacy to avoid such behaviors.

Moderators

Moderating variables in media literacy studies generally fall into three classes: agent (or source), audience, and treatment (Potter and Byrne, 2007). The agent refers to the individual who delivers the intervention, and extant media literacy interventions have typically involved teachers, peer students, and researchers. An agent can be a teacher (e.g., Dysart, 2008; Wade et al., 2003) or peer students who are trained by the researcher (e.g., Austin et al., 2005, 2007). In many studies, the researcher served as an agent playing the role of a teacher or a parent (e.g., Banerjee & Greene, 2006; Huesmann et al., 1983). However, studies typically have not directly tested whether intervention effects vary by the type of agent.

Related to the role of agent noted above is the setting in which media literacy interventions are delivered. When the agent is the teacher, media literacy interventions are typically implemented in school settings. However, when the agent is the researcher, they are implemented in various settings such as schools (e.g., Dysart, 2008), communities (e.g., Comer et al., 2008), and labs (e.g., Divsalar, 2006). The setting in which an intervention is delivered (e.g., a more naturalistic or controlled setting) may play a role in determining its effects. In this regard, we examine whether the effects of media literacy interventions are a function of the setting.

Although children or adolescents have been the primary audience of media literacy interventions (Potter, 2004), some studies have included college students or adults (e.g., Irving & Berel, 2001; Ramasubramanian & Oliver, 2007). The age of the audience may be an important factor in the effects of media literacy interventions. For example, Piaget (1952) suggested that as children move from the concrete operational stage (ages 8–12) to the formal operational stage (age 13 and above), they start to think in abstract ways. Thus, they may develop the ability to better understand media literacy education (see also Livingstone & Helsper, 2006).

The extent to which an intervention involves its audience may determine its effectiveness. For example, Banerjee & Greene (2006) found that an intervention including the production of messages is more effective than that focusing only on the analysis of messages. This suggests that interventions with active audience involvement (e.g., discussion or production activities) may be more effective than those with passive audience involvement (e.g., lessons only). Banerjee & Green view that media literacy interventions with active audience involvement can be more effective because they elicit greater mental efforts and comprehension than interventions with passive audience involvement. Thus, media literacy programs with active involvement components may be more effective than those with passive components.

As discussed above, media literacy is a multidimensional construct. Consequently, intervention treatment includes multiple types of literacy, such as content, grammar, medium, and structure literacy (Meyrowitz, 1998; Lewis & Jhally, 1998). Some interventions focused on a single type of media literacy, such as content literacy (e.g., Abelman & Courright, 1983) whereas others addressed multiple types (e.g., Dysart, 2008). However, studies have not examined whether these factors work together to produce additive effects. Thus, the present meta-analysis tests whether combining these factors would produce stronger effects of media literacy education.

The frequency of media literacy intervention sessions may lead to differences in the effects. Some media literacy interventions employed a single session (e.g., Irving et al., 1998), whereas others involved multiple sessions (e.g., Hennessey, 2008). Providing an appropriate dose of intervention sessions is considered crucial in achieving the desired outcome of an intervention (e.g., Hornik, 2002). In the present meta-analysis, we examine whether the effects of media literacy interventions vary as a function of the frequency of intervention sessions.

Media literacy interventions have addressed an increasingly wide range of topics, including alcohol, tobacco, violence, body image, sex, commercialism, and social issues. Thus, it is important to examine whether the effects of media literacy interventions vary according to the topic. Although a number of countries have adopted media literacy interventions, some countries such as the U.K., Canada, and Australia have a longer history of media literacy education than others due to social, policy, and regulatory factors (Kubey, 2003). Thus, this meta-analysis examines whether the effectiveness of media literacy interventions varies by the country (i.e., where the intervention is implemented). Finally, we will examine whether publication status (e.g., published or unpublished) determines the effects of media literacy interventions to test a possible publication bias. Thus, we examine whether the effects of media literacy interventions are moderated by the agent, the setting, audience age, audience involvement, intervention treatment, session frequency, the topic, the country, and publication status.

Method

Literature Search

Studies were searched using various databases, including Communication Abstracts, PsychInfo, PubMed, Proquest Dissertations and Theses Fulltext, and Google Scholar. The key-words used were “media literacy,” “media literacy intervention,” “media literacy curriculum,” and “media literacy program.” We also used combinations of key-words, such as “intervention,” “advertising,” and “skepticism,” to identify additional studies.4 We included all studies that were published before the cut-off point of December, 2009.

Of the 127 relevant studies, we selected 51 articles by excluding those that did not meet the following criteria. First, studies must have used quantitative methods. Although qualitative studies can provide an in-depth understanding of what students learn from media literacy programs, a meta-analysis should aggregate quantitative data.5 Second, studies must have tested the impact of a structured media literacy intervention on audiences.6 A typical intervention includes a program in which participants (a) learn about media production and its effects on audiences in lecture, print or video format, (b) are involved in various activities (e.g., discussions and homework) relevant to the program, or (c) produce their own media messages based on what they have learned from the program. Third, studies must have included one of the following outcomes of media literacy: knowledge, criticism, influence, realism, behavioral beliefs, attitudes, norms, self-efficacy, or behaviors.7 Fourth, studies must have reported statistical information required for a meta-analysis.8 Finally, studies had to be written in English. Fifty-one independent studies met all of the above criteria and were included in this meta-analysis.9

Outcomes

After selecting the list of studies that meet the above criteria, we identified a set of outcomes that were consistently included in the studies. The nine outcomes were as follows: knowledge, criticism, influence, realism, beliefs, attitudes, norms, self-efficacy, and behaviors. For knowledge, studies have typically used the term to refer to knowledge of specific construction techniques used to persuade audiences (e.g., Hobbs & Frost, 2003) as well as general knowledge of advertising (e.g., Buijzen, 2007).10 For criticism, studies have used terms such as understanding of persuasive intent (Austin & Johnson, 1997a) as well as skepticism (Austin et al., 2005). Beliefs about media myths (Pinkleton et al., 2008) and attitudes toward media messages or advertising (Austin et al., 2007) were also classified as criticism because these reflect critical perceptions of media content. For influence, studies have typically examined the extent to which respondents believed that the media can influence audiences (e.g., Duran et al., 2008). For realism, studies have typically used terms such as realism (Austin et al., 2007; Huesmann et al., 1983). For beliefs, studies used terms such as expectancy (Austin et al., 2007) and health consequences of tobacco use (Gonzales et al., 2004). For attitudes, studies have typically used the term attitudes (e.g., Banerjee & Greene, 2006), and for norms, studies have typically used the term norms (Austin et al., 2005). Most of the studies included in this meta-analysis have considered descriptive norms.11 For self-efficacy, studies have typically used the term efficacy (Austin et al., 2005). Finally, for behaviors, we grouped actual behaviors (Austin et al., 2005) as well as behavioral intentions (Banerjee & Greene, 2007) into a single category.

Moderators

We examined the following moderators: the agent, the setting, target age, audience involvement, intervention treatment, session frequency, the topic, publication status, and the country. Agents were coded as teacher, peer, researcher, or other. Related to the agent, settings were coded as school, community, both (school and community), or other (e.g., a lab). Target age was coded using the mean age of the respondents in a study. Audience involvement was classified into the following categories: (1) passive interventions involving only a lecture with print or audio-visual materials, (2) interactive interventions involving various activities such as in-class discussions, role playing, and computerized games, and (3) production interventions involving the actual production of messages. Intervention treatment was operationalized as the number of media literacy components. We examined each study to determine whether it included content, medium, grammar, and structure literacy components. Then, we counted the total number of components included in a study. For example, if a study included content and grammar literacy, then we coded that there were two treatment components in the study. The number of components included in each intervention could range from one to four. All studies included at least one literacy component and some included all four. For session frequency, the total number of sessions in the intervention was coded, and the number of sessions ranged from 1 to 40 sessions (mean = 3.62, median = 2). Topics were coded as alcohol, tobacco, drugs, body image and eating, violence, sex, advertising and marketing, social issues (e.g., racism, gender role), and general. Countries included the U.S., Australia, Canada, the Netherlands, and Tanzania. Because there were only eight studies conducted outside the U.S., all non-U.S. studies were merged into a single category. Publication status was coded as either published or unpublished.

Effect Size Calculation

To compute the meta-analytic effect size, the results of all studies were converted into a common effect-size metric (d) by using the following equation:

d=12Spooled(Spooled=s12(n11)+s22(n21)n1+n22)

In other words, the effect size represents the difference between the treatment group (i.e., the media literacy intervention group) and the control group. When there was more than one treatment group in a study, the overall mean across treatment groups was compared with that of the control group. In addition, we included the statistics (e.g., means and standard deviations) from immediate posttests but not the statistics from the delayed posttests. This is because the time lag for the immediate posttest was comparable across studies included in this meta-analysis, whereas it was not for the delayed posttest. That is, immediate posttests were typically administered on the day of the intervention, whereas delayed posttests were conducted days to a year after the intervention. A statistical software package Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2 was used for calculating effect sizes and for conducting meta-analyses.

Analysis Methods

First, the average effect size was computed across all outcome measures, including knowledge, criticism, realism, influence, beliefs, attitudes, norms, self-efficacy, and behaviors. Then the average effect size was computed for each outcome. Of the 51 independent studies, the numbers of studies reporting the effects of media literacy interventions on each outcome were as follows: 10 for knowledge, 24 for criticism, 14 for realism, 3 for influence, 7 for beliefs, 22 for attitudes, 7 for norms, 3 for self-efficacy, and 25 for behavior. The analyses were conducted using random-effects analyses based on Hedges & Olkin’s (1985) procedure.12 Finally, analyses were conducted to test the effects of potential moderators. The moderator analyses were based on the average effect size of each study, combining all outcomes. The categorical moderators (i.e., the agent, the setting, audience involvement, the topic, publication status, and the country) were analyzed using mixed-effects categorical analyses, whereas the continuous moderators (i.e., target age, intervention treatment, and session frequency) were analyzed using meta-regression.

Results

The Average Effect Size of Media Literacy Interventions

The mean effect size of media literacy interventions, weighted by sample size, was .37 (p < .001), with a 95% confidence interval ranging from .27 to .47. This suggests that, overall, the effects of media literacy interventions were significant in the positive direction (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of Moderator Analyses

Variable K d 95% CI QB p
Total set 51 0.37*** 0.27, 0.47
Agent 7.08 0.07
    Teacher 11 0.51** 0.22, 0.81
    Peer 4 0.31* 0.05, 0.56
    Researcher 24 0.42*** 0.30, 0.54
    Other or not specified 12 0.17* 0.004, 0.34
Setting 2.62 0.45
    School 37 0.36*** 0.24, 0.48
    Community 3 0.54*** 0.28, 0.79
    Both 2 0.21 −0.11, 0.54
    Other or not specified 9 0.41** 0.17, 0.65
Audience Involvement 1.79 0.62
    Passive 15 0.50*** 0.24, 0.75
    Interactive 22 0.33*** 0.24, 0.41
    Production 11 0.33*** 0.15, 0.52
    Other or not specified 3 0.21 −0.29, 0.71
Topic 7.67 0.47
    Advertising 8 0.36*** 0.18, 0.55
    Alcohol 3 0.42*** 0.28, 0.56
    Body image & Eating 16 0.28*** 0.17, 0.39
    Drug 1 0.27** 0.08, 0.47
    General literacy 4 0.56+ −0.03, 1.14
    Sex 1 0.38*** 0.21, 0.55
    Social 3 0.24 −0.15, 0.63
    Tobacco 6 0.20* 0.04, 0.36
    Violence 9 0.50** 0.16, 0.84
Country 0.80 0.37
    U.S. 43 0.34*** 0.24, 0.43
    Non U.S. 8 0.52* 0.13, 0.92
Publication status 0.76 0.38
    Published 42 0.39*** 0.27, 0.50
    Unpublished 9 0.29** 0.11, 0.47

Note.

+

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

K refers to the number of independent effect sizes considered for each group.

The Average Effect Size by Outcomes

Media literacy interventions had positive effects on almost all outcome measures: knowledge (d=1.12, p < .001, 95% CI: .77 to 1.47), criticism (d=.29, p < .001, 95% CI: .20 to .38), realism (d=.54, p < .001, 95% CI: .24 to .84), influence (d=.60, p < .05, 95% CI: .09 to 1.11), beliefs (d=.23, p < .001, 95% CI: .12 to .35), attitudes (d=.28, p < .001, 95% CI: .17 to .39), self-efficacy (d=.34, p < .001, 95% CI: .18 to .50), and behaviors (d=.23, p < .001, 95% CI: .15 to .31). Thus, our hypothesis that media literacy interventions increase audiences’ knowledge of the media, criticism of the media, awareness of the influence of the media, while reducing media realism was supported. In addition, the hypothesis that media literacy interventions reduce risky or antisocial behaviors, increase negative behavioral beliefs about and negative attitudes toward such behaviors, and increase self-efficacy to avoid such behaviors was supported. However, the effect on norms (d=.18, p = .08, 95% CI: −.02 to .39) was not supported.

Moderators

For the categorical moderators, Table 1 provides a list of effect sizes for each categorical group. The effects of media literacy interventions did not vary according to the agent, the setting, audience involvement, the topic, the country, or publication status.

For the continuous moderators, the meta-regression results indicate that session frequency (b = 0.008, SE b = 0.002, p < .001) and intervention treatment (b = −0.05, SE b = 0.02, p < .05) were significant moderators. For session frequency, studies with more intervention sessions reported larger effect sizes. For intervention treatment, however, the direction was somewhat unexpected. Studies with more intervention treatment components reported smaller effects. Target age (b = 0.001, SE b = 0.01, p = .86) was not a significant predictor.

Discussion

This meta-analysis contributes to the media literacy literature by a) providing a summary of the general effects of media literacy interventions, b) examining the effects of media literacy interventions on different types of outcomes (i.e., media-relevant vs. behavior-relevant), and c) specifying the moderators that influence the effect size of media literacy interventions.

First, media literacy interventions were generally effective (d=.37). Media literacy interventions had positive effects on most of the outcomes considered in this meta-analysis, although the effect on norms was marginally significant. The results suggest that it is possible to employ media literacy interventions to address the harmful effects of mass media. The effects of the media on risky behaviors (e.g., violence, smoking, and underage drinking) have long been criticized. The results of this study suggest that media literacy interventions may be an effective approach for reducing potentially harmful effects of media messages. Intervention effects were found across divergent topics for diverse audiences, for a broad range of media-related (e.g., knowledge) and behavior-related (e.g., attitudes and behaviors) outcomes. The results that intervention effects did not vary according to target age, the setting, audience involvement, and the topic suggest that interventions can be equally effective across a spectrum of settings (e.g., school, community, or lab), age groups, levels of audience involvement, and topics (e.g., alcohol, violence, and sex). Media literacy interventions is particularly important with the development of social media because the quality of information circulated through social media (e.g., Twitter) is not guaranteed, and thus, audiences’ literacy has become more important than ever.

Second, the present study clarified a list of outcomes that can be influenced by media literacy interventions based on a review of a number of theoretical frameworks relevant to media literacy. Although this meta-analysis was not able to conduct a direct test of differences in the effect size by the type of outcome, the results indicate that media literacy interventions have differential effects on the two types of outcomes: media-relevant outcomes and behavior-relevant outcomes. That is, media literacy interventions may have greater effects on media-relevant outcomes (e.g., knowledge and realism) than on behavior-relevant outcomes (e.g., attitudes and behaviors). This may be because media literacy interventions focus more on media-related content than on behavior-related content. Because most media literacy interventions have focused on enhancing critical thinking rather than on inducing behavior change, media literacy interventions may have greater impact on media-related outcomes. Further, this may be explained by the idea that media-relevant outcomes are more proximal or immediate outcomes of media literacy interventions, whereas behavior-relevant outcomes are more distal outcomes. Proximal outcomes are those that are more immediately influenced by media literacy interventions, whereas distal outcomes are those that are more remotely influenced by interventions.13 Thus it is not surprising that media literacy interventions will have stronger effects on media-related outcomes than on behavior-relevant outcomes. Future media literacy interventions may benefit by actively combining and integrating media-related educational components and behavior-related educational components (see Primack, Fine, Yang, Wickett, & Zickmund, 2009).

Finally, the present meta-analysis identified the moderators that influence the effect size of media literacy interventions. The results of the moderator analyses indicated that the impact of media literacy interventions increased as a function of the number of sessions, suggesting that media literacy interventions are more likely to be successful when the program is reinforced through multiple sessions. This result is consistent with Hornik’s (2002) argument regarding campaign effects based on the dose-response relationship.

The findings of this study suggest areas of future research. In terms of intervention treatment, the results are somewhat unexpected. In general, interventions with fewer components were more effective than those with more. There can be at least two possible explanations for this finding. One explanation is that interventions with more components are likely to contain too much information, resulting in information overload and information loss. This can be explained by the limited capacity model of information processing (Lang, 2000). Another possible explanation is that interventions with more components are more likely to be confusing, particularly to young audiences who are less likely to be cognitively sophisticated. If so, a simple and more focused education program may enhance the effects of a media literacy intervention. Thus, future research should examine the effects of media literacy interventions by varying the number of treatment components and considering the audience’s age.

The agent was not a significant moderator. In other words, the effect size of the intervention did not vary by the agent. Existing evidence on the role of agents in intervention effects is not entirely consistent. Some studies have found that experts are more effective than nonexperts (Durantini, Albarracin, Mitchell, Earl, & Gillette, 2006), while others suggest that peers are more effective than nonpeers (Webel, Okonsky, Trompeta, & Holzemer, 2010). Expert-led interventions may be more effective because of their knowledge, experience, and authority, whereas nonexpert-led interventions may be more effective because of perceived similarity and identification. The effect of agents in media literacy interventions may be clarified when future interventions directly compare the effects delivered by experts and peers.

This meta-analysis has some limitations. First, some unpublished studies, such as conference papers, were not included in the analysis. However, the average effect size for published studies was not larger than that for unpublished ones, indicating no publication bias. In addition, Rosenthal’s fail-safe N for this study was 4,688, which indicates the number of missing studies required for making the results of this meta-analysis nonsignificant. This suggests that it is unlikely that our findings are due to publication bias.14 Second, some studies were excluded from this meta-analysis because of the lack of necessary statistical information.

Overall, this study suggests that media literacy interventions are effective. Positive effects of media literacy interventions were observed across diverse agents, target age groups, settings, topics, and countries. Future research could further examine the specific types of intervention treatments that can enhance media-relevant and behavior-relevant outcomes.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for each study

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Charlie Armstrong, Soojung Kim, and Sunghwan Kim for coding the moderators. This study was supported in part by grant RO3CA128438 from the National Cancer Institute. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Cancer Institute or the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

1

Media literacy education has reflected diverse perspectives ranging from critical/cultural to social scientific. The present meta-analysis focuses on the social scientific approach which emphasizes protection.

2

Although interventions did not have to include the content, medium, grammar, or structure literacy components, studies typically had at least one of the four components.

3

Some inoculation studies were relevant to media literacy, whereas others were not. The list of inoculation studies that were excluded from this meta-analysis is available upon request.

4

To include the studies that did not use the term “media literacy,” we used search terms (e.g., “intervention,” “advertising,” “skepticism”) to generate a list of studies and reviewed each study to determine its appropriateness for this meta-analysis. To be qualified as a study of media literacy (i.e., not a study of reading, writing, or technological literacy), the study must have included some aspect of critical literacy as indicated by a number of definitions of media literacy (pp. 2–3). We conducted this additional search to provide a comprehensive review of media literacy interventions.

5

As a result, five studies using qualitative research methods such as in-depth or focus group interviews were excluded.

6

The list of 42 studies that were excluded by these criteria is available upon request.

7

A meta-analysis requires a sufficient number of studies focusing on a specific outcome. This is because a meta-analysis provides a summary of the effect of an independent variable on an outcome across multiple studies. Thus, some outcomes that were considered by only one study were excluded from this meta-analysis. As a result, three studies were excluded because they focused on outcomes that were not considered by other media literacy studies.

8

Although we contacted the authors to obtain missing statistical information, 26 studies were eventually excluded.

9

Studies based on the same sample, such as Buijzen & Mens (2007) and Buijzen (2007), were counted as one study, and the effect sizes for these two studies were averaged. On the other hand, Experiments 1, 2, and 3 in Sagarin et al.’s (2002) study were treated as three independent studies because they were based on different samples.

10

Some researchers view knowledge as an outcome, whereas others view it a manipulation check. In this meta-analysis, we considered it an outcome, not a manipulation check.

11

Specifically, seven studies measured descriptive norms, whereas only one (Chernin, 2007) measured subjective norms. Although the two types of norms are different (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990; Rimal & Real, 2003), we grouped them into a single category because there was only one study focusing on subjective norms.

12

The statistical software package used in this study, Comprehensive Meta-Analysis, was programmed based on Hedges & Olkin’s (1985) approach. An alternative analysis could be conducted using Hunter & Schmidt’s (1990) approach.

13

The distinction between proximal and distal outcomes may not be ideal. For example, beliefs may be considered more distal than knowledge but more proximal than behaviors.

14

Publication bias refers to the tendency of researchers to publish more studies that present significant results than those that present nonsignificant results. Thus, meta-analyses often include unpublished studies to examine whether effect sizes differ by publication status.

References

  • *.Abelman R, Courtright J. Television literacy: Amplifying the cognitive level effects of television’s prosocial fare through curriculum intervention. Journal of Research and Development in Education. 1983;17(1):46–57. [Google Scholar]
  • Allen M, D’Alessio D, Brezgel K. A meta-analysis summarizing the effects of pornography II: Aggression after exposure. Human Communication Research. 1995;22:258–283. [Google Scholar]
  • Allen M, D’Alessio D, Emmers T, Gebhardt L. The role of educational briefings in mitigating effects of experimental exposure to violent sexually explicit material: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sex Research. 1996;33:135–141. [Google Scholar]
  • Aufderheide P. Media literacy: A report of the national leadership conference on media literacy. Aspen, CO: Aspen Institute; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • Austin EW, Miller AC-R, Silva J, Guerra P, Geisler N, Gamboa L, Phakakayai O, Kuechle B. The effects of increased cognitive involvement on college students’ interpretations of magazine advertisements for alcohol. Communication Research. 2002;29:155–179. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Austin EW, Chen YY, Pinkleton BE, Johnson JQ. Benefits and costs of Channel One in a middle school setting and the role of media-literacy training. Pediatrics. 2006;117:e423–e433. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Austin EW, Johnson KK. Immediate and delayed effects of media literacy training on third graders’ decision making for alcohol. Health Communication. 1997a;9:323–349. doi: 10.1080/108107397127897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Austin EW, Johnson KK. Effects of general and alcohol-specific media literacy training on children’s decision making about alcohol. Journal of Health Communication. 1997b;2:17–42. doi: 10.1080/108107397127897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Austin EW, Pinkleton BE, Funabiki RP. The desirability paradox in the effects of media literacy training. Communication Research. 2007;34:483–506. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Austin EW, Pinkleton BE, Hust SJ, Cohen M. Evaluation of an American Legacy Foundation/Washington State Department of health media literacy pilot study. Health Communication. 2005;18:75–95. doi: 10.1207/s15327027hc1801_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Banerjee S, Greene K. Analysis versus production: Adolescent cognitive and attitudinal responses to antismoking interventions. Journal of Communication. 2006;56:773–794. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Banerjee S, Greene K. Antismoking initiatives: Effects of analysis versus production media literacy interventions on smoking-related attitude, norm, and behavioral intention. Health Communication. 2007;22:37–48. doi: 10.1080/10410230701310281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Beltramini R, Bridge P. Relationship between tobacco advertising and youth smoking: Assessing the effectiveness of a school-based, antismoking intervention program. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2001;35:263–277. [Google Scholar]
  • Bergsma LJ, Carney ME. Effectiveness of health-promoting media literacy literature education: A systematic review. Health Education Research. 2008;23:522–542. doi: 10.1093/her/cym084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Buijzen M. Reducing children’s susceptibility to commercials: Mechanisms of factual and evaluative advertising interventions. Media Psychology. 2007;9:411–430. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Buijzen M, Mens C. Adult mediation of television advertising effects: a comparison of factual, evaluative, and combined strategies. Journal of Children and Media. 2007;1:177–191. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Byrne S. Media literacy interventions: What makes them boom or boomerang? Communication Education. 2009;58(1):1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • Cantor J, Wilson BJ. Media and violence: Intervention strategies for reducing aggression. Media Psychology. 2003;5:363–403. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Chernin A. The relationship between children’s knowledge of persuasive intent and persuasion. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania; 2007. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58:1015–1026. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Comer JS, Furr JM, Beidas RS, Weiner CL, Kendall PC. Children and terrorism-related news: Training parents in coping and media literacy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:568–578. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.4.568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Compton J, Pfau M. Use of inoculation to foster resistance to credit card marketing targeting college students. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2004;32:343–364. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Coughlin JW, Kalodner C. Media literacy as a prevention intervention for college women at low-or high-risk for eating disorders. Body Image. 2006;3:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Divsalar D. Body image dissatisfaction: An intervention study with college women. San Francisco Bay, CA: Alliant International University; 2006. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Duran RL, Yousman B, Walsh KM, Longshore MA. Holistic media education: An assessment of the effectiveness of a college course in media literacy. Communication Quarterly. 2008;56(1):49–68. [Google Scholar]
  • Durantini MR, Albarracin D, Mitchell AL, Earl AN, Gillette JC. Conceptualizing the influence of social agents of behavior change: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of HIV-prevention interventionists for different groups. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:212–248. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Dysart MM. The effectiveness of media literacy and eating disorder prevention in schools. Raleigh, NC: North Carolina State University; 2008. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Eisen M. Intermediate outcomes from a life skills education program with a media literacy component. In: Crano WD, Burgoon M, editors. Mass media and drug prevention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2002. pp. 187–214. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Fealk ER. Evaluation of aducation: Are one-shot media literacy interventions effective? Boston, MA: Brandeis University; 1998. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Fiissel DL. Using health promotion principles to prevent eating disorders in 8 to 10 year old girls. Windsor, Canada: University of Windsor; 2005. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • Fishbein M, Yzer MC. Using theory to design effective health behavior interventions. Communication Theory. 2003;13:164–183. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Goldberg ME, Niedermeier KE, Bechtel LJ, Gorn GJ. Heightening adolescent vigilance toward alcohol advertising to forestall alcohol use. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing. 2006;25:147–159. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Gonzales R, Glik D, Davoudi M, Ang A. Media literacy and public health: Integrating theory, research, and practice for tobacco control. American Behavioral Scientist. 2004;48(2):189–201. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Harts ML. Media literacy and video technology: Educational and motivational tools to empower African-American males in special education. United States -- New York: Columbia University Teachers College; 1997. Unpublished Ed.D. Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Hennessey M. Body dissatisfaction, eating disorders and a media literacy intervention among Tanzanian females. Boston, MA: Boston University; 2008. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Hestroni A. Three decades of sexual content on prime-time network programming: A longitudinal meta-analytic review. Journal of Communication. 2007;57:318–348. [Google Scholar]
  • Hobbs R. The seven great debates in the media literacy movement. Journal of Communication. 1998;48(1):16–32. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Hobbs R, Frost R. Measuring the acquisition of media-literacy skills. Reading Research Quarterly. 2003;38:330–355. [Google Scholar]
  • Holmstrom AJ. The effects of the media on body image: A meta-analysis. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media. 2004;48:196–217. [Google Scholar]
  • Hornik RC. Public health communication: Evidence for behavior change. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Huesmann L, Eron L, Klein R, Brice P, Fischer P. Mitigating the imitation of aggressive behaviors by changing children's attitudes about media violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;44:899–910. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.44.5.899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error and bias in research findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Irving LM, Berel SR. Comparison of media-literacy programs to strengthen college women's resistance to media images. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2001;25:103–111. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Irving LM, DuPen J, Berel S. A media literacy program for high school females. Eating Disorders. 1998;6:119–131. [Google Scholar]
  • Kellner D, Share J. Toward critical media literacy: Core concepts, debates, organizations, and policy. Discourse: Studies in the cultural politics of education. 2005;26:369–386. [Google Scholar]
  • Kubey R. Why U.S. media education lags behind the rest of the English-speaking world. Television New Media. 2003;4:351–370. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Kusel AB. Primary prevention of eating disorders through media literacy training of girls. San Diego, CA: California School of Professional Psychology; 1999. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Lang A. The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication. 2000;50(1):46–70. [Google Scholar]
  • Lang A. The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication. 2000;50(1):46–70. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Lew AM, Mann T, Myers H, Taylor S, Bower J. Thin-ideal media and women’s body dissatisfaction: Prevention using downward social comparisons on non-appearance dimensions. Sex Roles. 2007;57:543–556. [Google Scholar]
  • Lewis J, Jhally S. The struggle over media literacy. Journal of Communication. 1998;48(1):109–120. [Google Scholar]
  • Livingstone S, Helsper EJ. Does advertising literacy mediate the effects of advertising on children? A critical examination of two linked research literatures in relation to obesity and food choice. Journal of Communication. 2006;56:560–584. [Google Scholar]
  • Martens H. Evaluating media literacy education: Concepts, theories and future directions. Journal of Media Literacy Education. 2010;2:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  • McCannon R. Media literacy/media education: Solution to big media? In: Strasburger VC, Wilson BJ, Jordan AB, editors. Children, adolescents, and the media. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2009. pp. 519–569. [Google Scholar]
  • McGuire WJ. Inducing resistance to persuasion: Some contemporary approaches. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 1. New York: Academic; 1964. pp. 191–229. [Google Scholar]
  • *.McVey GL, Davis R. A program to promote positive body image: A 1-year follow-up evaluation. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2002;22(1):96–108. [Google Scholar]
  • Messaris P. Visual persuasion. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • Meyrowitz J. Multiple media literacies. Journal of Communication. 1998;48(1):96–108. [Google Scholar]
  • Paik H, Comstock G. The effects of television violence on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Communication Research. 1994;21(4):516–546. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Pfau M, Van Bockern S, Kang JG. Use of inoculation to promote resistance to smoking initiation among adolescents. Communication Monographs. 1992;59:213–230. [Google Scholar]
  • Piaget J. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press; 1952. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Pinkleton BE, Austin EW, Cohen M, Chen Y-C, Fitzgerald E. Effects of a peer-led media literacy curriculum on adolescents' knowledge and attitudes toward sexual behavior and media portrayals of sex. Health Communication. 2008;23:462–472. doi: 10.1080/10410230802342135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Posavac HD, Posavac SS, Weigel RG. Reducing the impact of media images on women at risk for body image disturbance: Three targeted interventions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2001;20:324–340. [Google Scholar]
  • Potter WJ. Media literacy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • Potter WJ, Byrne S. What are the effects of media literacy? In: Mazzerella SR, editor. Twenty Questions about youth and the media. New York, NY: Peter Lang; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • Primack BA, Fine D, Yang CK, Wickett D, Zickmund S. Adolescents' impressions of antismoking media literacy education. Health Education Research. 2009;24:608–621. doi: 10.1093/her/cyn062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Rabak-Wagener J, Eickhoff-Shemek J, Kelly-Vance L. The effect of media analysis on attitudes and behaviors regarding body image among college students. Journal of American College Health. 1998;47(1):29–35. doi: 10.1080/07448489809595616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Ramasubramanian S, Oliver MB. Activating and suppressing hostile and benevolent racism: Evidence for comparative media stereotyping. Media Psychology. 2007;9:623–646. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Richardson SM, Paxton SJ, Thomson JS. Is BodyThink an efficacious body image and self-esteem program? A controlled evaluation with adolescents. Body Image. 2009;6:75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rimal RN, Real K. Understanding the influence of perceived norms on behaviors. Communication Theory. 2003;13:184–203. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Robinson T, Wilde M, Navracruz L, Haydel K, Varady A. Effects of reducing children's television and video game use on aggressive behavior: A randomized controlled trial. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2001;155:17–23. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.155.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Rosenkoetter LI, Rosenkoetter SE, Acock AC. Television violence: An intervention to reduce its impact on children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2009;30:381–397. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Rosenkoetter LI, Rosenkoetter SE, Ozretich RA, Acock AC. Mitigating the harmful effects of violent television. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2004;25:25–47. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Sagarin BJ, Cialdini RB, Rice WE, Serna SB. Dispelling the illusion of invulnerability: The motivations and mechanisms of resistance to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83:526–541. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Scharrer E. “I noticed more violence:” The effects of a media literacy program on critical attitudes toward media violence. Journal of Mass Media Ethics. 2006;21(1):69–86. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Silver BE. A media skills intervention for adolescents on gender attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors. Kingston, RI: University of Rhode Island; 1999. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  • Silverblatt A. Media literacy: Keys to interpreting media messages. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Steiner-Adair C, Sjostrom L, Franko DL, Pai S, Tucker R, Becker AE, Herzog DB. Primary prevention of risk factors for eating disorders in adolescent girls: Learning from practice. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2002;32:401–411. doi: 10.1002/eat.10089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Vooijs MW, van der Voort THA. Learning about television violence: The impact of a critical viewing curriculum on children's attitudinal judgments of crime series. Journal of Research and Development in Education. 1993a;26(3):133–142. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Vooijs MW, van der Voort THA. Teaching children to evaluate television violence critically: The impact of a Dutch schools television project. Journal of Educational Television. 1993b;19(3):139–152. [Google Scholar]
  • *.Wade TD, Davidson S, O'Dea JA. A preliminary controlled evaluation of a school-based media literacy program and self-esteem program for reducing eating disorder risk factors. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2003;33:371–383. doi: 10.1002/eat.10136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Webb T, Martin K, Afifi AA, Kraus J. Media literacy as a violence-prevention strategy: A pilot evaluation. Health Promotion Practice. 2009 doi: 10.1177/1524839908328998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Webel AR, Okonsky J, Trompeta J, Holzemer WL. A systematic review of the effectiveness of peer-based interventions on health-related behaviors in adults. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100:247–253. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.149419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *.Wilksch SM, Wade TD. Reduction of shape and weight concern in young adolescents: A 30-month controlled evaluation of a media literacy program. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:652–661. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181a1f559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yamamiya Y, Cash TF, Melnyk SE, Posavac HD, Posavac SS. Women’s exposure to thin-and-beautiful media images: Body image effects of media-ideal internalization and impact-reduction interventions. Body Image. 2005;2:74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.11.001. * References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

RESOURCES