Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2012 Feb 22;137(1-3):20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.032

Cortical Gyrification in Velo-Cardio-Facial (22q11.2 Deletion) Syndrome: A Longitudinal Study

Arun Kunwar a, Seethalakshmi Ramanathan a, Joshua Nelson a, Kevin M Antshel a, Wanda Fremont a, Anne Marie Higgins b, Robert J Shprintzen, Wendy R Kates a
PMCID: PMC3414250  NIHMSID: NIHMS361670  PMID: 22365148

Abstract

Introduction

Velocardiofacial syndrome (VCFS) has been identified as an important risk factor for psychoses, with upto 32% of individuals with VCFS developing a psychotic illness. Individuals with VCFS thus form a unique group to identify and explore early symptoms and biological correlates of psychosis. In this study, we examined if cortical gyrification pattern, i.e. gyrification index (GI) can be a potential neurobiological marker for psychosis.

Method

GIs of 91 individuals with VCFS were compared with 29 siblings and 54 controls. Further, 58 participants with VCFS, 21 siblings and 18 normal controls were followed up after 3 years and longitudinal changes in GI were compared. Additionally, we also correlated longitudinal changes in GI in individuals with VCFS with prodromal symptoms of psychosis on the Scale of Prodromal Symptoms (SOPS).

Result

Individuals with VCFS had significantly lower GIs as compared to their siblings and normal controls. Longitudinal examination of GI did not reveal any significant group-time interactions between the three groups. Further, longitudinal change in GI scores in the VCFS group was negatively correlated with positive prodromal symptoms, with the left occipital region reaching statistical significance.

Conclusion

The study confirms previous reports that individuals with VCFS have reduced cortical folding as compared to normal controls. However over a period of three years, there is no difference in the rate of change of GI among both individuals with VCFS and normal controls. Finally, our results suggest that neuroanatomical alterations in areas underlying visual processing may be an early marker for psychosis.

Keywords: VCFS, Gyrification index, prodromal symptoms, occipital cortex

1. Introduction

Velocardiofacial syndrome (VCFS) results from a microdeletion spanning over 40 genes (Carlson et al, 1997) at 22q11.2, with an estimated prevalence of 1 in 2000–4000 births (Shprintzen, 2005). Physical features include a characteristic facial appearance, and cardiac and pharyngeal abnormalities. Additionally, VCFS is associated with behavioral abnormalities including disinhibition, impulsivity, schizoid features, social withdrawal, and flat affect (Feinstein et al, 2002; Swillen et al, 1999). Significantly, it has been reported that up to 32% individuals with VCFS develop a psychotic illness in adulthood (Arnold et al, 2001; Bassett and Chow, 1999; Green et al, 2009; Murphy et al, 1999; Sphrintzen et al, 1992). The high rate of psychosis in VCFS suggests that, with the exception of being the offspring of a dual mating or the monozygotic co-twin of an affected individual, the specific microdeletion of chromosome 22q11 associated with VCFS represents the highest known risk factor for the development of schizophrenia. This strong association between schizophrenia and VCFS implies that a gene or genes mapping to chromosome 22q11 may play a role in the etiology of schizophrenia.

It is therefore not surprising that several investigators have explored this unique relationship between VCFS and schizophrenia. Qualitative (Chow et al, 1999; Lynch et al, 1995; Mitnick et al, 1994; van Amelswoort et al, 2001; Vataja and Elomaa, 1998) and quantitative (Eliez et al, 2000; 2001; Kates et al, 2001; 2006; Simon et al, 2005; Tan et al, 2009) neuroimaging studies have identified neuroanatomic overlap between individuals with VCFS and schizophrenia, including enlarged ventricles, midline brain abnormalities (e.g. cavum septum pellucidum), and volumetric reductions in prefrontal, parietal, temporal and occipital GM and WM, and hippocampal volumes. Similarly, studies of individuals with schizophrenia (Cahn et al, 2002; Corsona et al, 1999; Davatzikos et al, 2005; Hazlett et al, 1999; Pearlson and Marsh, 1999; Pol et al, 2002; Shenton et al, 2001; Wright et al, 2000) have reported reductions in the volumes of total and prefrontal GM, temporal and parietal lobes, caudate nucleus, thalamus, hippocampus and amygdala.

Overlapping anomalies in cortical complexity have also been reported in studies of individuals with schizophrenia and those with VCFS. The complexity of cortical folding has been represented in most studies with the gyrification index (GI). Gyrification abnormalities in schizophrenia have been reported in the left hemisphere (Sallet et al, 2003), left frontal and right temporal regions (Harris et al, 2004a). Pre-existing differences in gyral folding have also been identified as a potential trait marker for individuals who subsequently develop schizophrenia. Harris et al (2004b) reported that right prefrontal lobe GI was significantly increased in individuals who subsequently developed schizophrenia.

In individuals with VCFS, findings are variable. Whereas Bearden and colleagues (2009) observed GI increases in the occipital lobe, Srivastava et al (2012) noted GI reductions in frontal and parietal lobes relative to controls. In their cross-sectional sample, Srivastava and colleagues further noted that GI in VCFS-affected youth ages 6–15 years (but not in controls) decreased with age, particularly in the parietal lobes. Similarly, Schaer et al showed a significant decrease in the GI in frontal (2006) and parietal lobes (2006, 2008, 2009) of adolescents and adults with VCFS relative to controls. Interestingly, the study by Schaer and colleagues (2009) observed that relative to typical controls, individuals with VCFS had differential rates of cortical maturation that were more magnified in patients who later developed schizophrenia. However, neither study of cortical gyrification in VCFS followed the same cohort over time to investigate longitudinal changes in GI.

In an effort to complement and extend previous studies, we examined longitudinal changes in gyrification patterns in a cohort of youth with VCFS, and explored the extent to which alterations in GI were associated with early symptoms of psychosis. We have previously reported, in the same cohort, that among individuals with VCFS, contractions in the surface morphology of the temporal cortex were associated with overall prodromal symptoms of psychosis (Kates et al, 2011), and longitudinal decrements in volumes of temporal lobe GM were associated specifically with positive prodromal symptoms of psychosis (Kates et al, 2011). The current report expands on these findings as well.

Based on previous GI studies in both VCFS and schizophrenia, we hypothesized that:

  • Hypothesis 1a: Individuals with VCFS will have reduced GI relative to controls and siblings.

  • Hypothesis 1b: On longitudinal assessment, individuals with VCFS will have a slower rate of change in GI as compared to typically developing controls.

  • Hypothesis 2: GI patterns, particularly in the frontal and temporal regions, will be associated with prodromal symptoms. We propose this hypothesis as our findings on cortical surface morphology suggest that psychotic symptoms are related to change in frontal and temporal cortices.

2. Materials and Methods (Details in Appendix I)

2.1. Participants

The current sample, described in Table 1, comprises data acquired for two studies:

  1. A cross-sectional study (Study 1) of children with VCFS between the ages of 6 and 9 years, and age-matched community controls. This group of participants was not invited for longitudinal assessments.

  2. A longitudinal study (Study 2) of 9–15 year old children with VCFS, their unaffected siblings within the same age range, and age-matched community controls. For Study 2, all participants were invited to return for a second time point, three years after their initial visit.

TABLE 1.

Description of demographic profile of the three groups

Baseline Sample VCFS (n-91) Siblings (n-29) Controls (n-54) p-value
Age (Years) 11.33(SD- 2.5) 12.27(SD- 2.11) 10.61(SD- 2.59) 0.01*
Gender
Male 47 (51.6%) 12 (41.4%) 33 (61.1%) 0.22
Female 44 (48.3%) 17 (58.6%) 21 (38.9%)
Handedness
Right 80 (87.9%) 27(93.1%) 49 (90.7%) 0.69
Left 11 (12.1%) 2(6.9%) 5 (9.3%)
Follow-up Sample VCFS (n-58) Siblings (n-21) Control (n-18) p-value
Age (Years) 14.99 (SD- 2.05) 15.19 (SD- 2.15) 15.23 (SD- 2.01) 0.87
Gender
Male 30 (51.7%) 11 (52.4%) 11 (61.1%) 0.78
Female 28 (48.3%) 10 (47.6%) 7 (38.9%)
Handedness
Right 49 (84.5%) 19 (90.5%) 17 (94.4%) 0.49
Left 9 (15.5%) 2 (9.5%) 1 (5.6%)
*

p<0.05

The data from Study 1, and the baseline data from Study 2, are combined in this report to form the “Baseline” sample (age range from 6–15 years). Data from the participants (9 to 15 years of age at baseline) who were followed longitudinally comprise the “Follow-up” sample. At the first time point (Baseline visit, henceforth referred to as Time 1 or T1), all participants underwent an MRI scan. At Follow-up visit (henceforth referred to as Time 2 or T2), three (range: 1.5–5.3) years later, participants underwent a repeat MRI scan using the same protocol as Time 1. In Study 2, attrition between T1 and T2 was 16% for individuals with VCFS, 21% for siblings, and 37% for controls.

2.2. MRI acquisition

Magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired in the axial plane on a 1.5 T Philips Gyroscan scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands). images were imported into BrainImage (Reiss, 2004) for removal of non-brain tissue, and were subsequently spatially normalized using a 3D Talairach grid (Kates et al, 1999; Talaraich and Tournoux, 1998), which was used to determine GI in the four major lobar regions (frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital).

2.3. Calculation of Gyrification Index

GI was measured following a semi-automated protocol described by Schmitt and colleagues (2002). GI is calculated as the ratio of the length of the inner perimeter of the cortex (including the depths of the sulci) to the length of the outer perimeter (Zilles et al, 1988):

  • GI= Length of inner contour/ Length of outer contour

Behavioral Assessments

The Scale of Prodromal Symptoms (SOPS) (Miller et al, 2003) was used to assess prodromal symptoms of psychosis. The SOPS is a clinician-based instrument (based on self-report and observation) that categorizes prodromal symptoms into four categories: Positive (P), Negative (N), Disorganized (DOS) and General (G) symptoms. Total (T) score was calculated as the sum of P, N, DOS and G scores. For most assessments, Total and Positive subscale scores were used.

Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using STATA SE 11.0. All major lobar regions were examined – frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital. Further, as the two hemispheres differ in their GI patterns due to functional specialization, left and right GI for each lobe were analyzed separately (Armstrong et al, 1995). Cross-sectional analyses were carried out using MANCOVA, with age and gender as covariates. For longitudinal analyses, we used repeated measures MANOVA, controlling for gender. GI change score was calculated as the difference between GI at T2 and T1. This change score was used in ZIP regressions with P and Total scores. Throughout, we corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction; p<0.006 (0.05/8) was considered as statistically significant for the cortical regions.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Profile (Table 1)1

At Time 1, as was expected due to the combination of the two samples, a significant difference was noted in the ages between the three groups (p= 0.01). Neither gender distribution (p= 0.22) nor handedness (p= 0.69) differed between groups. At time 2, the three groups did not differ significantly in age (p= 0.87), gender (p= 0.78) or handedness (p= 0.49).

3.2. Cross-sectional Analyses

3.2.1. Comparison of GI among the three groups (MANCOVA) (Table 2 &3)

TABLE 2.

Description of GIs (mean (SD)) of the three study groups over the two time periods.

VCFS Siblings Controls
T1 (n=91) T2 (n=58) T1(n=29) T2(n=21) T1 (n=54) T2 (n=18)
R Frontal 3.09
(0.36)
2.81
(0.35)
3.26
(0.34)
3.04
(0.31)
3.28
(0.39)
3.09
(0.34)
L Frontal 3.07
(0.41)
2.82
(0.38)
3.21
(0.41)
3.08
(0.35)
3.30
(0.35)
3.04
(0.31)
R Parietal 3.17
(0.46)
2.91
(0.36)
3.47
(0.49)
3.21
(0.32)
3.57
(0.43)
3.37
(0.43)
L Parietal 3.01
(0.40)
2.71
(0.33)
3.23
(0.35)
3.03
(0.34)
3.36
(0.44)
3.04
(0.43)
R Temporal 2.76
(0.45)
2.49
(0.37)
2.97
(0.48)
2.68
(0.51)
2.83
(0.51)
2.60
(0.40)
L Temporal 2.67
(0.41)
2.45
(0.36)
2.78
(0.45)
2.61
(0.41)
2.60
(0.53)
2.49
(0.38)
R Occipital 2.05
(0.36)
1.99
(0.36)
2.82
(0.46)
2.17
(0.44)
2.05
(0.53)
2.05
(0.49)
L Occipital 2.01
(0.29)
2.05
(0.35)
2.17
(0.39)
2.19
(0.51)
2.04
(0.47)
2.05
(0.43)
TABLE 3.

MANCOVA controlled for age and gender among the three diagnoses at T1 (df-2, 1, 170) & T2 (df-2, 1, 93) and repeated measures MANOVA, controlled for gender (df- 2, 1, 93) between T1 and T2.

T1 (n-174) T2 (n-97) T2-T1 (n-97)
Group-Time Interaction
F P F P F P
Right
Frontal 7.57 0.0001* 6.34 0.0006* 0.36 0.6992
Parietal 14.64 0.000* 16.67 0.0000* 0.30 0.7427
Temporal 1.48 0.2215 1.60 0.1946 0.71 0.4923
Occipital 2.25 0.0839 1.11 0.3499 0.32 0.7273
Left
Frontal 10.83 0.0000* 9.11 0.0000* 0.11 0.8934
Parietal 13.52 0.000* 12.66 0.0000* 0.20 0.8212
Temporal 1.02 0.3843 1.03 0.3826 0.05 0.9549
Occipital 2.92 0.0354 0.83 0.4796 1.16 0.0141

Applying Bonferroni correction for cortical regions: * p (0.05/8) = 0.006

Table 2 (df-3, 170) presents the GIs (average and SD) for lobar regions in the three groups at the two time points. Table 3 reports the MANCOVA results comparing the GIs in the frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital regions of the three groups at each time point controlling for age and gender.

Significant differences were noted among the three groups at both time points. At T1, significant differences were noted in the frontal (Right: F= 7.57, p= 0.00, Left: F=10.83, p= 0.00) and parietal regions (Right: F= 14.64, p= 0.00, Left: F=13.52, p= 0.00) bilaterally. At T2, the three groups continued to differ significantly in the same regions (right frontal: F= 6.34, p= 0.00, Left frontal: F=9.11, p= 0.00, right parietal: F= 16.67, p= 0.00, left parietal: F=12.66, p= 0.00) bilaterally. Post-hoc analyses revealed that these differences were driven by participants in the VCFS group who had significantly lower GI’s in all regions than the other two groups (reported in Table 2). GI scores were the highest for the sibling group, with the exception of the right frontal and parietal regions, which were highest in community controls.

3.3. Longitudinal Analyses

3.3.1. Comparison of GI among the three groups over the two time periods (Repeated Measures MANOVA with gender as covariate) (Table 3)

Repeated measures MANOVA (df- 2, 94) examining changes in GI over time showed no significant group-X-time interactions. However, post-hoc analyses revealed a similar time effect in all groups. GI’s among all three groups changed significantly over time (p<0.01), i.e., GI at baseline in frontal, parietal and temporal regions was significantly higher than GI at follow-up among all three groups. These differences held when corrected for multiple analyses in all regions except right parietal (p= 0.008). In both occipital lobes, the GI at follow-up was not significantly different from the GI at baseline (right occipital lobe (F= 1.06, p= 0.31); left occipital lobe (F= 0.74, p= 0.39)).

3.3.2. Correlations of GI with SOPS score (ZIP regression) (Table 4)

TABLE 4.

Zero-inflated Poisson regression (n=57)

Positive Total
(N+P+G+DOS)
Right Z P Z P
Frontal 0.15 0.878 2.65 0.008
Parietal −0.15 0.879 0.74 0.459
Temporal −2.05 0.040 −0.69 0.491
Occipital −0.77 0.440 −2.74 0.006*
Left
Frontal −2.05 0.041 −0.86 0.392
Parietal −1.17 0.242 2.68 0.007
Temporal −0.59 0.556 −0.31 0.758
Occipital −3.09 0.002 * −2.37 0.018

Applying the Bonferroni correction: * p (0.05/ 8) = 0.006

Vuong Test: p<0.05 for all regressions.

ZIP analyses revealed negative correlations between GI change scores in all areas and the positive SOPS score, suggesting that lower GI change scores were associated with positive SOPS. SOPS Positive Symptom score correlated significantly with GI scores in right temporal (p= 0.040), left frontal (p= 0.041), and left occipital (p= 0.002) lobes, while SOPS total scores correlated with right frontal (p= 0.008), right parietal (p= 0.007) and both occipital GI scores (right occipital region: p= 0.006, left occipital region: p= 0.018). When Bonferroni - corrected for multiple comparisons, the negative association between SOPS Positive symptoms and left occipital GI persisted. Similarly, a negative correlation between right occipital GI and the SOPS Total score remained statistically significant, although the parietal and frontal regions were close to surviving the correction.

4. Discussion

In this longitudinal study, individuals with VCFS were followed over a three year period, along with their siblings and community controls. Our first hypothesis was partially supported. Individuals with VCFS had lower gyrification indices relative to age and sex-matched siblings and community controls in frontal and parietal (but not temporal) lobes. This pattern of lower baseline GI among individuals with VCFS was also noted in the same regions during the follow-up visit 3 years later. Qualitative observations of anomalous cortical gyrification, including pachygyria and polymicrogyria in the frontal and parietal regions were among the earliest structural abnormalities reported in individuals with VCFS (Bingham et al, 1998; Bird and Scrambler, 2000; Cramer et al, 1996; Ehara et al, 2000; Ghariani et al, 2002; Kawame et al, 2000; Koolen et al, 2004; Sztriha et al, 2004; Worthington et al, 2000). Schaer et al reported that compared to typically developing individuals, those with VCFS had lower GIs in the frontal (2006) and parietal regions (2006, 2008, 2009). Similar to our finding, Schaer et al (2006) did not observe differences in the temporal GIs among individuals with VCFS and typically developing controls. These GI reductions in individuals with VCFS may reflect aberrant/ weaker intercortical connections in frontal and parietal cortices. Barnea-Goraly et al (2003) reported reduced fractional anisotropy in the frontal and parietal regions.

Longitudinal analyses identified that GIs among all three groups decreased over the three years. However, no significant differences were noted between the three groups. This time effect was not noted in the occipital lobe, i.e., the rate of decrease in GI in the occipital lobe is slower than other regions. Srivastava et al (2012) noted that, relative to typically developing children, children with VCFS had both higher and lower rates of age-related change in GI in various parts of the parietal lobe; however none of these changes were significantly different. These authors do not report significant differences in the developmental rates of the occipital lobe, although their cross-sectional results are not directly comparable to our longitudinal findings. A longitudinal exploration of cortical changes by our group (Kates, 2011) noted that changes in cortical morphology were similar among individuals with VCFS, their siblings and typically developing controls. These morphological changes represented changes in protrusions and contractions of the cortical surface, putatively associated with changes in the local volumes of cortical brain regions (Petersen, 2010). In contrast, our measure of gyrification represents changes in the degree of cortical complexity or convolutions of the brain. As such, cortical gyrification can be used to infer changes in the shape, as opposed to local volumes, of the cortical surface.

Our second hypothesis examined the association between longitudinal changes in GI and development of psychosis. Several authors (Harris et al, 2004b; Jou et al, 2005) have proposed that aberrant gyrification patterns influence the development of psychiatric symptoms including psychosis. Harris et al (2004b) noted that abnormal GIs could be a trait marker for schizophrenia. When we explored the correlation of rate of change of GI with the incidence of prodromal symptoms, after correcting for multiple analyses, we found that longitudinal decreases in GI in the left occipital lobe were associated with greater positive scores whereas in the right occipital lobe they were associated with total SOPS scores. As noted earlier, the rates of change of GI were the slowest in the occipital lobes as compared to the other regions. However, in individuals with VCFS, greater longitudinal reduction in GI in the occipital lobe was associated with positive symptoms. It is however still possible that further longitudinal follow-up may reveal a catch-up in the change of GI in the occipital regions. The differential rate of change in the occipital lobe and association with prodromal symptoms suggests that occipital lobar connections may become dysfunctional or fail to develop normally, thereby influencing onset or development of prodromal symptoms.

Several studies (Schaer et al 2009, Bearden et al, 2007) have suggested that individuals with VCFS have cortical abnormalities in the regions responsible for visuospatial functioning. In a cross-sectional study of a similar age group, Bearden et al (2009) noted increased gyral complexity in the occipital lobe. There are contradictory findings about the involvement of the occipital lobe in schizophrenia, with some studies (Davatzikos et al, 2005; Onitsuka et al, 2007; Zipursky et al, 1992) reporting reductions and others (Giuliani et al, 2005; Goldstein et al, 1999; Shenton 2001) noting no significant changes. Onitsuka et al (2007) have reported reductions in the gray matter areas of the visual association area. They further suggest that these changes may contribute to early visual processing abnormalities noted in schizophrenia.

We also noted that the right frontal and left parietal change scores attained near significance with total SOPS scores. This was in the opposite direction of the occipital lobe, suggesting that in individuals with prodromal symptoms, GIs in the frontal and parietal regions increase on follow-up. This is also discrepant from the longitudinal trajectory noted in these regions in all three groups; GIs in these regions decrease as a general rule. Accordingly, it appears that when the direction of change in frontal and parietal cortical complexity deviates from that of both neurotypical individuals and most subjects with VCFS, prodromal symptoms appear. Further, the correlation with total scores and not positive scores suggest that these areas may be involved in other symptoms including disorganization, negative and general or may influence the intensity of the prodromal symptoms. Parietal and frontal cortical changes have been reported in individuals with increased risk for developing schizophrenia (Prasad et al, 2010). Further, our group (Kates et al, 2011) noted differential longitudinal trajectories in parietal and frontal cortical surface morphology in individuals with VCFS at high risk for developing schizophrenia. Most studies of schizophrenia associate positive symptoms with the temporal lobe. Although we have recently noted associations between changes in temporal gray matter volume and surface morphology and prodromal symptoms of psychosis in individuals with VCFS (Kates, 2011), we were unable to demonstrate here an association with changes in temporal lobe GI. It is possible that temporal GI changes may become evident on further follow-up.

GI reflects cortical folding and is established by the age of 6–9 months following which GI decreases until it stabilizes in adulthood (Armstrong et al, 1995). The morphogenetic model (Toro & Burnod, 2005) suggests that the cortical plate develops at a different rate than the inner plate, which results in cortical folding. Van Essen (1997) discussed a mechanical model which explains that the pattern of cortical folding is determined by the mechanical tension exerted by the longer subcortico-cortical (in a radial fashion) and shorter local intercortical axons (in a tangential manner) during development. More compact longer axons and weaker local (cortical interconnections) axonal development leads to smaller gyri with smaller sulci, thereby decreased GI. On the other hand, looser long radial axons and stronger local interconnections lead to increased GI. Additionally, authors (Guerrini et al, 2000) have suggested that gyrification abnormalities can coexist due to vascular anomalies. Schaer et al noted that individuals with congenital heart disease have reduced gyrification than individuals with normal heart functioning. They proposed that these could be secondary to a common genetic mechanism through TBX1 or the consequence of reduced blood flow resulting from cardiac dysfunction. A final hypothesis that has been proposed is that synaptic pruning may be involved in the formation of sulci and gyri. White et al (2003) propose a model based on pruning of the connections in the sulci that can contribute to further remodeling of the gyral system. Thus, measurement of GI provides some indication of both neurodevelopmental (such as neuronal migration and proliferation leading to alteration in neuronal tension and altered pruning) and neurodegenerative (including vascular changes) processes. Our cross-sectional and longitudinal findings of differences in GI among the three groups suggest that prodromal symptoms in VCFS may be a developmental rather than degenerative process, although the influence of the latter process cannot be ruled out completely.

Our results should be examined in the context of their limitations. Although we used a previously validated (Kates et al., 1999) 3D Talaraich grid to identify lobar regions, our method for measuring GI specifically was limited in its 2D anatomical definition. Additionally, all statistical analyses were done using the most restrictive method (repeated measures MANOVA and Bonferroni correction) available. While it demonstrates the robustness of our findings (Keselman et al, 1991), it also increases the chances of Type II errors. We also noted that siblings of VCFS had higher GI values as compared to typically developing controls. A similar finding was noted in the rate of change of orbitolateral cortical volumes examined in the same group (siblings had a greater increase in volumes than typically developing controls). We believe that these changes may reflect a familial influence rather than any particular genetic pattern. Further, we did not exclude controls who had ADHD or learning disabilities to ensure a control sample that would appropriately match for our higher functioning VCFS subjects. So their lower GI values may have reflected their developmental challenges to some extent.

To summarize, the study affirms earlier findings that individuals with VCFS have reduced cortical folding than typically developing individuals. The study also noted that GI in individuals with VCFS changes at a rate similar to typical controls. However, this longitudinal trajectory appears to be deviant in “high-risk” individuals with VCFS. Finally, we identified significant associations between longitudinal alteration in the GI of the occipital lobe and prodromal symptoms, raising the question of whether deficits in visual processing in VCFS may represent a vulnerability marker for the development of psychosis.

Supplementary Material

01

Acknowledgments

In addition to the funding agencies listed above, the authors are grateful to all study participants and their families.

Role of funding source:

The study was funded by the National Institutes of Health (MH64824, MH65481, to Dr. Kates), and by a Young Investigator Award from the National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD; now the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation) to Dr. Kunwar. Neither agency had a role in the study design, data collection and analysis, or preparation of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

In study 2, participants who did not follow up for the second part of the study (11.9 years) were noted to be similar in age as compared to the participants who followed up (12.5 years). This held true between the three groups as well. Further, no differences were noted in the GIs of any lobar regions between these two groups. When the three groups were treated separately, only the siblings who dropped out were noted to have significantly lower GIs in the frontal regions and left parietal region. These differences, however, did not survive Bonferroni correction.

Author Disclosure:

Conflict of interest:

Drs. Shprintzen and Antshel, and Anne Marie Higgins received research support from Aton Pharma during the past three years. The rest of the authors declare that no financial support or compensation has been received from any individual or corporate entity over the past 3 years for research.

Contributors:

Arun Kunwar analyzed the imaging datasets and wrote a portion of the manuscript.

Seethalakshmi Ramanathan completed the statistical analyses and wrote a portion of the manuscript.

Joshua Nelson analyzed the imaging datasets and managed the references.

Kevin M. Antshel conducted the cognitive assessments on all participants, and a portion of the psychiatric assessments.

Wanda Fremont conducted a portion of the psychiatric assessments on the participants.

Anne Marie Higgins recruited all participants and managed the logistics of their participation at both time points of the study.

Robert J. Shprintzen contributed to the overall design of the protocol.

Wendy R. Kates designed the study protocol, oversaw subject recruitment and assessment, and wrote a portion of the manuscript.

References

  1. Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Omran H, Curtis M, Zilles K. The ontogeny of human gyrification. Cereb Cortex. 1995;5(1):56–63. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnold PD, Siegel-Bartelt J, Cytrynbaum C, Teshima I, Schachar R. Velo-cardio facial syndrome: Implications of microdeletion 22q11 for schizophrenia and mood disorders. Am Jr Genet (Neuropsychiatr Genet) 2001;105:354–362. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnea-Goraly N, Menon V, Krasnow B, Ko A, Reiss A, Eliez S. Investigation of White Matter Structure in Velocardiofacial Syndrome: A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1863–1869. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bassett AS, Chow EW. 22q11 deletion syndrome: A genetic subtype of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:882–891. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00114-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bearden CE, van Erp TG, Dutton RA, Tran H, Zimmermann L, Sun D, Geaga JA, Simon TJ, Glahn DC, Cannon TD, Emanuel BS, Toga AW, Thompson PM. Mapping cortical thickness in children with 22q11.2 deletions. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(8):1889–1898. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bearden CE, van Erp TG, Dutton RA, Lee AD, Simon TJ, Cannon TD, Emanuel BS, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai EH, Thompson PM. Alterations in midline cortical thickness and gyrification patterns mapped in children with 22q11.2 deletions. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(1):115–126. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bingham PM, Lynch D, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E. Polymicrogyria in chromosome 22 detection syndrome. Neurology. 1998;51(5):1500–1502. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.5.1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bird LM, Scambler P. Cortical dysgenesis in 2 patients with chromosome 22q11 deletion. Clinical Genetics. 2000;58(1):64–68. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2000.580111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonnici HM, William T, Moorhead J, Stanfield AC, Harris JM, Owens DG, Johnstone EC, Lawrie SM. Pre-frontal lobe gyrificationindex in schizophrenia, mental retardation and comorbid groups: an automated study. Neuroimage. 2007;35(2):648–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cahn W, Pol HEH, Lems EGTE, van Haren NEM, Schnack HG, van der JA, Schothorst PF, van Engeland H, Kahn RE. Brain Volume Changes in First-Episode Schizophrenia A 1-Year Follow-up Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:1002–1010. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.11.1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carlson C, Sirotkin H, Pandita R, Goldberg R, McKie J, Wadey R, Patanjal SR, Weissman SM, Anyane-Yeboa K, Warburton D, Scambler P, Shprintzen R, Kucherlapati R, Morrow BE. Molecular definition of 22q11 deletions in 151 velocardio facial syndrome patients. Am J Hum Genet. 1997;61:620–629. doi: 10.1086/515508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chow EW, Mikulis DJ, Zipursky RB, Scutt LE, Weksberg R, Bassett AS. Qualitative MRI findings in adults with 22q11 deletion syndrome and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1436–1442. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00150-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Corsona PW, Nopoulosa P, Andreasena NC, Heckela D, Arndta S. Caudate size in first-episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients measured using an artificial neural network. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46(5):712–730. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00079-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cramer SC, Schaefer PW, Krishnamoorthy KS. Microgyria in the distribution of the middle cerebral artery in a patient with DiGeorge syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology. 1996;11(6):494–497. doi: 10.1177/088307389601100619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Davatzkikos C, Shen D, Gur RC, Wu X, Liu D, Fan Y, Hughett P, Turetsky BI, Gur RE. Whole-brain morphometric study of schizophrenia revealing a spatially complex set of focal abnormalities. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:1218–1227. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ehara H, Maegaki Y, Takeshita K. Pachygyria and polymicrogyria in 22q11 deletion syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2003;117A(1):80–82. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.10508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eliez S, Schmitt JE, White CD, Reiss AL. Children and Adolescents With Velocardiofacial Syndrome: A Volumetric MRI Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:409–415. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Feinstein C, Eliez S, Blasey C, Reiss AL. Psychiatric disorders and behavioral problems in children with velocardiofacial syndrome: usefulness as phenotypic indicators of schizophrenia risk. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;15:312–318. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01231-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ghariani S, Dahan K, Saint-Martin C, Kadhim H, Morsomme F, Moniotte S, Verellen-Dumoulin C, Sebire G. Polymicrogyria in chromosome 22q11 deletion syndrome. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology. 2002;6(1):73–77. doi: 10.1053/ejpn.2001.0544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Giuliani NR, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Francis A, Buchanan RW. Voxel-based morphometry versus region of interest: a comparison of two methods for analyzing gray matter differences in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;74:135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goldstein JM, Goodman JM, Seidman LJ, Kennedy DN, Makris N, Lee H, Tourville J, Caviness VS, Jr, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Cortical abnormalities in schizophrenia identified by structural magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:537–547. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.6.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Green T, Gothelf D, Glaser B, Debbane M, Frisch A, Kotler M, Weizman A, Eliez S. Psychiatric disorders and intellectual functioning throughout development in velocardiofacial (22q11.2 deletion) syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:1060–1068. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b76683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Guerrini R, Barkovich AJ, Sztriha L, Dobyns WB. Bilateral frontal polymicrogyria: a newly recognized brain malformation syndrome. Neurology. 2000;54(4):909–913. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.4.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Harris JM, Whalley H, Yates S, Miller P, Johnstone EC, Lawrie SM. Abnormal cortical folding in high-risk individuals: a predictor of the development of schizophrenia? Biological Psychiatry. 2004b;56(3):182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Harris JM, Yates S, Miller P, Best JJ, Johnstone EC, Lawrie SM. Gyrification in first-episode schizophrenia: a morphometric study. Biological Psychiatry. 2004a;55(2):141–147. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00789-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Byne W, Wei TC, Spiegel-Cohen J, Geneve C, Kinderlehrer R, Haznedar MM, Shihabuddin L, Siever LJ. Three-dimensional analysis with MRI and PET of the size, shape, and function of the thalamus in the schizophrenia spectrum. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1190–1199. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.8.1190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jou RJ, Hardan AY, Keshavan MS. Reduced cortical folding in individuals at high risk for schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophrenia Research. 2005;75(2–3):309–313. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kates WR, Warsofsky IS, Patwardhan A, Abrams MT, Liu AM, Naidu S, Kaufmann WE, Reiss AL. Automated Talairach atlas–based parcellation and measurement of cerebral lobes in children. Psychiatry Res. 1999;91:11–30. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(99)00011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kates WR, Burnette CP, Jabs EW, Rutberg J, Murphy AM, Grados M, Geraghty M, Kaufmann WE, Pearlson GD. Regional Cortical White Matter Reductions in Velocardiofacial Syndrome: A Volumetric MRI Analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:677–684. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01002-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kates WR, Miller AM, Abdulsabul N, Antshel KM, Conchelos J, Fremont W, Roizen N. Temporal Lobe Anatomy and Psychiatric Symptoms in Velocardiofacial Syndrome (22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome) J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2006;45(5):587–595. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000205704.33077.4a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kates WR, Bansal R, Fremont W, Antshel KM, Hao X, Higgins AM, Liu J, Shprintzen RJ, Peterson BS. Mapping Cortical Morphology in Youth with Velo-Cardio-Facial (22q11.2 Deletion) Syndrome. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2011;50(3):272.e2–282.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kates WR, Antshel KM, Faraone SV, Fremont WP, Higgins AM, Shprintzen RJ, Botti J, Kelchner L, McCarthy C. Neuroanatomic Predictors to Prodromal Psychosis in Velocardiofacial Syndrome (22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome): A Longitudinal Study. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;(10):945–952. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.027. 15;69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kawame H, Kurosawa K, Akatsuka A, Ochiai Y, Mizuno K. Polymicrogyria is an uncommon manifestation in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2000;94(1):77–78. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20000904)94:1<77::aid-ajmg16>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Keselman HJ, Keselman JC, Shaffer JP. Multiple pairwise comparisons of repeated measures means under violation of multisample sphericity. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110(1):162–170. [Google Scholar]
  35. Koolen DA, Veltman JA, Renier W, Droog RP, van Kessel AG, de Vries BB. Chromosome 22q11 deletion and pachygyria characterized by array-based comparative genomic hybridization. American Journal of Medical Genetics A. 2004;131(3):322–324. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lynch DR, McDonald-McGinn DM, Zackai EH, Emanuel BS, Driscoll DA, Whitaker LA, Fischbeck KH. Cerebellar atrophy in a patient with velocardiofacial syndrome. J Med Genet. 1995;32:561–563. doi: 10.1136/jmg.32.7.561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, Cadenhead K, Cannon T, Ventura J, McFarlane W, Perkins DO, Pearlson GD, Woods SW. Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:703–715. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mitnick RJ, Bello JA, Shprintzen RJ. Brain anomalies in velocardiofacial syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1994;54:100–106. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320540204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Murphy KC, Jones LA, Owen MJ. High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:940–945. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Onitsuka T, McCarley RW, Kuroki N, Dickey CC, Kubicki M, Demeo SS, Frumin M, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Shenton ME. Occipital Lobe Gray Matter Volume in Male Patients with Chronic Schizophrenia: A Quantitative MRI Study. Schizophr Res. 2007;92(1–3):197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pearlson GD, Marsh L. Structural brain imaging in schizophrenia: a selective review. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(5):627–649. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00071-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Peterson BS. Form Determines Function: New Methods for Identifying the Neuroanatomical Loci of Circuit – Based Disturbances in Childhood Disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 49(6):533–538. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.03.010. 201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Prasad KM, Goradia D, Eack S, Rajagopalan M, Nutche J, Magge T, Rajarethinam R, Keshavan MS. Cortical surface characteristics among offspring of schizophrenia subjects. Schizophr Res. 2010;116(2–3):143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pol HEH, Schnack HG, Bertens MGBC, van Haren NEM, van der Tweel I, Staal WG, Baaré WFC, Kahn RS. Volume Changes in Gray Matter in Patients With Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:244–250. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Reiss AL. BrainImage. Stanford, CA: Stanford University; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  46. Sallet PC, Elkis H, Alves TM, Oliveira JR, Sassi E, Campi de Castro C, Busatto GF, Gattaz WF. Reduced cortical folding in schizophrenia: an MRI morphometric study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(9):1606–1613. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schaer M, Schmitt JE, Glaser B, Lazeyras F, Delavelle J, Eliez S. Abnormal patterns of cortical gyrification in velo-cardio-facial syndrome (deletion 22q11.2): An MRI study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2006;146:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schaer M, Debbané M, Bach CM, Ottet MC, Glaser B, Thiran JP, Eliez S. Deviant trajectories of cortical maturation in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS): a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Schizophr Res. 2009;115(2–3):182–190. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schaer M, Cuadra MB, Tamarit L, Lazeyras F, Eliez S, Thiran JP. A surface-based approach to quantify local cortical gyrification. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2008;27(2):161–170. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2007.903576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schaer M, Glaser B, Cuadra MB, Debbane M, Thiran JP, Eliez S. Congenital heart disease affects local gyrification in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(9):746–753. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03281.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schmitt JE, Watts K, Eliez S, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, Reiss AL. Increased gyrification in Williams syndrome: evidence using 3D MRI methods. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2002;44(5):292–295. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201002109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frummin M, McCarley RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2001;49:1–52. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00163-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shprintzen RJ. Velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Progress in Pediatric Cardiology. 2005;20:187–193. [Google Scholar]
  54. Shprintzen RJ, Goldberg R, Golding-Kushner KJ, Marion RW. Late- onset psychosis in the velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1992;42:141–142. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320420131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Simon TJ, Ding L, Bish JP, McDonald-McGinn DM, Zackai EH, Gee J. Volumetric, connective, and morphologic changes in the brains of children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: an integrative study. NeuroImage. 2005;25:169–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Srivastava S, Buonocore MH, Simon TJ. Atypical developmental trajectory of functionally significant cortical areas in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(1):213–223. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sztriha L, Guerrini R, Harding B, Stewart F, Chelloug N, Johansen JG. Clinical, MRI, and pathological features of polymicrogyria in chromosome 22q11 deletion syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2004;127A(3):313–317. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Swillen A, Devriendt K, Legius E, Prinzie P, Vogels A, Ghesquiere P, Fryns J. The behavioural phenotype in velo-cardio-facial syndrome (VCFS): from infancy to adolescence. Genet Couns. 1999;10:79–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart-New York: Georg Thieme Verlag; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  60. Tan GM, Arnone D, McIntosh AM, Ebmeier KP. Meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies in chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velocardiofacial syndrome) Schizophrenia Research. 2009;115:173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Toro R, Burnod Y. A Morphogenetic Model for the Development of Cortical Convolutions. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:1900–1913. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Van Amelsvoort T, Daly E, Robertson D, Suckling J, Ng V, Critchley H, Owen MJ, Henry J, Murphy KC, Murphy DGM. Structural brain abnormalities associated with deletion at chromosome 22q11. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178:412–419. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.5.412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Van Essen DC. A tension-based theory of morphogenesis and compact wiring in the central nervous system. Nature. 1997;385:313–318. doi: 10.1038/385313a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vataja R, Elomaa E. Midline brain anomalies and schizophrenia in people with CATCH 22 syndrome. Br J Psychiatry. 1998;172:518–520. doi: 10.1192/bjp.172.6.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. White T, Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, Magnotta V. Gyrification abnormalities in childhood- andadolescent-onset schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(4):418–426. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Worthington S, Turner A, Elber J, Andrews PI. 22q11 deletion and polymicrogyria—cause or coincidence? Clinical Dysmorphology. 2000;9(3):193–197. doi: 10.1097/00019605-200009030-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PWR, David AS, Murray RM, Bullmore ET. Meta-Analysis of Regional Brain Volumes in Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:16–25. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zilles K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Kretschmann HJ. The human pattern of cortical gyrification. Anat Embryology. 1988;179:173–179. doi: 10.1007/BF00304699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zipursky RB, Lim KO, Sullivan EV, Brown BW, Pfefferbaum A. Widespread cerebral gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:195–205. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030027004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES