Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Trends Cogn Sci. 2012 Oct 2;16(11):533–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.005

Table 1.

Intrinsic Networks and their functional description in our constructionist framework

Network
name(s)
Brain regions included Tasks observed in Psychological operation in a
constructionist ontology
“limbic” [27] *medial temporal lobe,
subgenual anterior
cingulate cortex, medial
and lateral orbitofrontal
cortex
  • emotion and affective experiences and perceptions [9, 17]

  • • autobiographical memory [18]

  • • pain [22]

  • • motivated behavior [64]

core affect generation: representing
visceromotor states from prior
experience or engaging visceromotor
control of the body to create the core
affective tone (pleasure or
displeasure with some degree of
arousal) that is a basic feature of all
conscious experience, and that directs
basic approach/withdrawal
behaviors.
“salience
network” [33]
---
“ventral attention
network” [27]
---
“cingulol
opercular
network” [65]
---
network #8 [26]
anterior midcingulate
cortex (aMCC), bilateral
dorsal anterior insula and
frontal operculum
  • emotion and affective experiences and perceptions [9, 17]

  • aversion [23]

  • language [24]


  • language [24]

responses. Likely, this network can be
decomposed further into aspects that
represent bodily states (a ventral
anterior insula network) and use
bodily states to drive attention and
behavior (a dorsal anterior insula
network) [34]
“default network”
[27]
---
network #4 [26]
medial prefrontal cortex,
retrosplenial area,
posterior cingulate
cortex/precuneus, medial
temporal lobe
(hippocampus, entorhinal
cortex), bilateral superior
temporal sulcus
  1. emotion and affective experiences and perceptions [9, 17]

  2. representations of the self [68]

  3. autobiographical memory [18]

  4. prospection [18]

  5. theory of mind [18]

  6. moral reasoning [20]

  7. context-sensitive visual perception [69]

  8. spontaneous thought [70]

  9. semantics, phonology, sentence processing [19]

  10. emotion regulation (in which a current emotional state is reconceptualized as a different emotional state) [71]

  11. placebo response to a painful stimulus (in which sensations are re-conceptualized as nonnociceptive) [72]

conceptualization: representing
prior experiences (i.e., memory or
category knowledge). During
autobiographical memory or
representation of concept knowledge,
this process simulates prior sensorymotor
experiences. During perception
of objects, this process helps make
meaning of sensations from the world
in a context-specific manner. During
emotion, this process helps make
meaning of sensations from the body
in a context-specific manner.
“frontoparietal
network” [27]
---
“executive
control network”
[33]
---
network #9 [26]
prefrontal cortex, inferior
parietal lobe, inferior
parietal sulcus,
precuneus, and middle
cingulate cortex (mCC)
  • alerting to a stimulus after a cue [74]

  • working memory [75]

activity in other networks to create a
unified conscious field during the
construction of a mental state (e.g.,
selecting some conceptual content
when meaning is made of sensations
and inhibiting other content; selecting
some sensations for conscious
awareness and inhibiting others).
“dorsal attention
network” [27]
---
network #9 [26]
bilateral frontal eye
fields, dorsal posterior
parietal cortex, fusiform
gyrus, area MT+
  • top-down control of visuospatial attention [76]

visuospatial attention: modulating
activity in exteroceptive sensory
regions (e.g., selecting which visual
sensation are selected for conscious
awareness and inhibiting others).
This process may be specific to visual
sensations given the import of these
sensations in human evolution.
“sensorimotor”
[27]
precentral and
postcentral gyri
(sensorimotor cortex),
Heschl’s gyrus (primary
auditory cortex) cortex,
posterior insula
  • audition [77]

  • somatovisceral sensation [78]

exteroceptive sensory perception:
representing auditory and tactile
sensations
“visual” [27] occipital lobe
  • vision [79]

exteroceptive sensory perception:
representing visual sensations

Note: By examining how brain networks perform during emotions as well as mental states that are typically considered to be other psychological faculties (e.g., memory, perception, theory of mind, etc.), it is possible to use a more robust form of reverse inference to hypothesize the “lowest common denominator” that is the best psychological description for each network. The result is a hypothesis about the functional architecture of the brain that is rooted in basic psychological processes (rather than the set of commonsense faculties now in use; On the need for a new functional architecture in cognitive neuroscience, see [46, 49, 52]). *Although Yeo et al. did not include subcortical structures in their analysis, we include subcortical structures in this network based on their known anatomical connections. We include the nuclei of the basal ganglia, which are involved in orchestrating effortful behavior and motor control [80]. We also hypothesize that the central nucleus of the amygdala, which is involved in producing autonomic responses [80], and the midbrain periacqueductal gray, which is involved in coordinating coherent physiological and behavioral responses [80] are part of this network. The basal ganglia, the amygdala and the periacqueductal gray all project to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), the major cortical site in Yeo et al.’s limbic network.