Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013 Jan;61(1):79–83. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12069

Pulmonary Function Impairment May be An Early Risk Factor for Late-Life Cognitive Impairment

Jean-Sébastien Vidal *,, Thor Aspelund , Maria K Jonsdottir ‡,§, Palmi V Jonsson ‖,#, Tamara B Harris *, Oscar L Lopez **, Vilmundur Gudnason ‡,#, Lenore J Launer *
PMCID: PMC3545414  NIHMSID: NIHMS413567  PMID: 23311554

Abstract

Background

Low pulmonary function (PF) is associated with poor cognitive function and dementia. There are few studies of change in PF in mid-life and late-life cognitive status.

Design and Participants

We studied this is 3,665 subjects from AGES-Reykjavik Study who had at least one measure of forced expiratory volume/ 1 sec (FEV1) and were cognitively tested on average 23 years later. A subset of 1,281 subjects had two or three measures of FEV1 acquired over a 7.8 year period. PF was estimated as FEV1/Height2. Rate of PF decline was estimated as the slope of decline over time. Cognitive status was measured with continuous scores of memory, speed of processing, and executive function, and as the dichotomous outcomes of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia.

Results

Lower PF measured in mid-life predicted lower memory, speed of processing, executive function, and higher likelihood of MCI and dementia 23 years later. Decrease of PF over a 7.8-year period in mid-life was not associated with lower cognitive function or dementia.

Conclusion

Reduced PF measured in mid-life may be an early marker of later cognitive problems. Additional studies characterizing early and late PF changes are needed.

Keywords: Cognition, Dementia, Forced Expiratory Volume, Longitudinal Cohort Studies

INTRODUCTION

Poor pulmonary function leads to a reduced oxygen supply to the brain.1,2 Lower pulmonary function (PF) has been correlated with poor cognitive performance in patient populations as well as in community dwelling subjects with a wide range of health status and ethnicities.17 PF has also been shown to be consistently reduced in demented subjects in cross-sectional studies,8 and is associated with a future risk for dementia.911 These extant studies, based on one measure of PF, may well reflect the strong constitutional and early life environmental factors that affect PF.12 There are few data on whether a decline in PF in mid-life also contributes to late-life cognitive test scores and risk for MCI and dementia. If change, in addition to level of PF, is also associated with cognition, this would strengthen the evidence that PF has a more direct effect on cognition. This is important to study as preventing poor PF with early interventions, such as education campaigns to stop smoking or increase physical activity, or by treating and preventing pulmonary diseases may reduce late-life cognitive impairment.

METHODS

The population-based AGES-RS study (2002–2006) cohort (5764 men and women) is based on surviving members of the Reykjavik Study initiated in 1967 by the Icelandic Heart Association to prospectively study cardiovascular disease in Iceland.13 The Reykjavik Study included a random sample of men and women born between 1907 and 1935 and living in Reykjavik area. By design, sub-cohorts of subjects were examined from one to six times from 1967 to 1996.13

Pulmonary assessment at mid-life

PF was evaluated at each Reykjavik Study examination with Vitalograph Spirometers (Vitalograph Ltd., Buckingham, UK). The same equipment was used throughout the study and was regularly calibrated with a 1 liter syringe. Three attempts were recorded for each forced expiratory volume in 1 s (FEV1). PF was calculated as FEV1 divided by height squared of the subject, as this has been shown in this cohort to be a more stable predictor of outcomes than FEV1 alone.14 Height was taken at the time the first spirometry measure was acquired. In analyses using repeat measures of spirometry we used the same baseline height so FEV1/height2 did not appear to change because of a change in height.

Assessment of cognition, and dementia and MCI later in life (2002–2006)

Cognitive assessment in AGES-RS study has already been detailed.15 In brief, we administered a battery of six different cognitive tests that included a modified version of the California Verbal Learning Test,16 the Figure Comparison Test,17 the Digit Symbol Substitution Test,18 the Stroop Test,19 a shortened version of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Spatial Working Memory test,20 and the Digits Backward test.18 From these tests, three cognitive domain composite Z-scores were calculated: memory, speed of processing and executive function.15

Dementia case ascertainment was a three-step process previously described13 that included a screening based on the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test, a diagnostic neuropsychological test battery, an informant interview, and a neurological examination. A consensus diagnosis of dementia and MCI was made by a panel including a geriatrician, neurologist, neuropsychologist, and neuroradiologist. Dementia was classified according to DSM-IV criteria.21 MCI was defined as having a borderline score (i.e.< −1.5 SD scores based on the distribution of scores in a cohort sub-sample) in at least one cognitive domain or as having one abnormal test result in at least two other domains not severe enough to be classified as dementia.22

Potential confounding and moderating variables

In addition to age and sex, analyses were adjusted for factors associated with both PF and cognitive outcomes.23 Variables measured concurrently with the first PF measure included education level (> high school yes/no), occupation (manual, intermediate and professional), BMI and moderate to vigorous physical activity in youth and midlife. Variables measured in the late-life exam concurrently with cognition included: presence of depressive symptoms measured by the 15-point Geriatric Depression Scale;24 self-reported history of a doctor’s diagnosis of coronary heart disease; chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD); hypertension by doctor’s report, use of anti-hypertensive medication, or measured systolic and diastolic blood pressure; diabetes by doctor’s report, use of anti-diabetic drugs, or fasting glucose levels; and life-time history of smoking (never, former, current).

Based on previous reports we investigated whether presence of the Apolipoprotein E ε*4 allele,25 a genetic susceptibility factor for dementia, modified the association of PF to dementia. Apolipoprotein E (Apo E) genotype was determined by standard DNA amplification and a restriction isotyping method.26 Subjects were classified as having no Apo E ε4 alleles or having 1 or 2 Apo E ε4 alleles.

Analytical sample

We excluded baseline spirometry assessments made before March 1, 1976 (n=1,522) because, as described previously, there were a significant number of outlier measurements from that period;14 measures made between 1991 and 1996 (n=51) by subjects who were older than 70 years of age;13 subjects with no PF assessments (n=8), and those with incomplete cognitive data (n=518). These exclusions resulted in an analytical sample of 3,665 subjects with complete data on PF and cognitive status (see supplemental figure). Compared to subjects not included in the analysis, those included were younger, more likely to be women, were less likely to be depressed or smokers and more often had a manual occupation.

The 3,665 included 128 MCI and 288 demented subjects. Of the 3,665, 1,281 subjects (of which 1,151 were not demented or had MCI) had multiple PF assessments to estimate change in PF; 874 subjects had two and 407 had three assessments. Differences between those with one compared to those with multiple measures are shown in supplementary table 1. Analyses on cognitive domains included only subjects with no dementia or MCI (n=3,249 for the first PF assessment and n=1,151 for the change in PF); analyses on dementia and MCI included all subjects (n=3,665 for the first PF assessment and n=1,281 for the change in PF).

Statistical analysis

For the baseline analyses, descriptive comparisons and hypothesis testing were examined by quartile of PF, using analysis of variance or logistic regression. The change in PF was estimated by the coefficient of PF linearly regressed against age in each of the 1281 subjects who had at least two pulmonary assessments corresponding to the change in FEV1 per year. The change results are expressed as the change in cognitive score, or likelihood of dementia or MCI associated with each standard deviation change in PF. All models were first adjusted for age and sex (Model 1) and then further adjusted for the aforementioned potential confounders (Model 2). We entered into model 1, the PF and Apo E ε4 main effect terms and their cross product to test the interaction between the two variables. Analyses were conducted with the statistical software package SAS Version 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C., USA). In all analyses, the 2 sided α–level of 0.05 was considered significant

RESULTS

The sample of 3,665 subjects had a mean age of 76 years and 33 percent were male. Mean age at the first spirometry assessment was 52 years (SD 5.3), which was on average 23 years prior to the cognitive assessment. Low FEV1/height2 was associated with older age, lower education, less mid-life physical activity; and presence of COPD, diabetes, high blood pressure and smoking (Table 1). Among subjects with multiple PF assessments, PF decreased by an average of 1.9% per year.

Table 1.

Cohort Characteristics by Quartiles of Mid-life Forced Expiratory Volume in 1 Second / Height2: AGES-Reykjavik Study

General characteristics Forced Expiratory Volume in 1 second / Height2a
Pb
1st Quartile (low) 2nd Quartile 3rd Quartile 4th Quartile (high)
N=919 N=917 N=912 N=917
Mid-life variables,cM (SD)
  Age at 1st pulmonary assess. 54.2 (5.6) 53.2 (5.4) 52.1 (5.2) 49.7 (5.2) <.0001
  BMI 25.3 (4.0) 25.5 (3.7) 25.4 (3.5) 243.9 (3.4) 0.21
  FEV1/Height2 0.77 (0.12) 0.95 (0.09) 1.07 (0.09) 1.24 (0.12) -
  Past physical activity, % 26.4 30.2 35.3 36.1 0.00002
  Occupation Class, %
    Manual work 57.9 55.4 56.6 55.2 0.30
    Intermediate 30.0 31.2 27.9 29.5
    Professional 12.1 13.4 15.5 15.3
Late-life variables,d M (SD)
  Age 77.6 (5.2) 76.8 (5.2) 76.0 (5.2) 74.4 (5.0) <.0001
  Men, % 32.7 32.8 32.8 32.7 -
  Higher education, % 21.3 24.1 27.3 30.2 0.0001
  Ever smoker, % 64.6 55.9 51.2 51.1 <.0001
  Height 165.2 (9.1) 165.7 (9.3) 166.1 (9.1) 166.2 (9.0) 0.16
  Weight 73.5 (14.9) 75.1 (14.5) 75.9 (14.4) 75.2 (14.3) 0.30
  Depression (GDS >=6), % 9.0 5.6 6.4 6.3 0.03
  Previous CHD, % 23.0 20.8 18.6 17.5 0.02
  COPD, % 6.6 3.7 1.8 2.0 <.0001
  Diabetes, % 12.8 10.9 11.4 8.2 0.01
  High Blood Pressure, % 85.4 83.0 81.0 74.4 <.0001
  Coronary Artery Calciume 737 (1019) 643 (987) 621 (1011) 561 (991) 0.37
  APOE 4 carrier, % 26.4 27.0 27.5 29.2 0.57
Cognition, M (SD)
  Memory f −0.22 (0.95) −0.07 (0.98) 0.07 (0.99) 0.22 (1.01) 0.0002
  Speed f −0.25 (1.11) −0.08 (1.04) 0.09 (0.92) 0.24 (0.85) <.0001
  Working f −0.19 (1.02) −0.11 (0.99) 0.07 (0.97) 0.23 (0.95) <.0001
Cognitive status, %
  MCI 10.6 8.9 7.5 4.6 0.0006
  Dementia 5.6 4.4 2.1 1.9

Abbreviations: BMI: Body Mass Index: [Weight (in kgs) / Height2 (in meters)]; M (SD): Mean (Standard Deviation); GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale; CHD Coronary Heart Disease; COPD: Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease.

a

Sex specific quartiles.

b

Analysis of variance or multinomial logistic regression adjusted for sex by virtue of sex-specific quartiles of FEV1 and age.

c

Measured at the time of the first spirometry;

d

Measured at the time of cognitive assessment;

e

Agatston score;

f

Demented and MCI subjects not included (n=416).

Level of PF and cognition

Among the 3, 249 non-demented subjects, performance in the three cognitive domains increased as FEV1/height2 quartile increased (P for linear trend < 0.001 for all three cognitive domains). Coefficients were slightly attenuated after adjustments for demographic and cardiovascular risk factors and disease (Model 2, Table 2). The interaction between the presence of Apo E ε4 and PF was not significant (P >0.15) for any of the cognitive domains.

Table 2.

Association of Cognitive Measures to Mid-life Pulmonary Function Level and Change: AGES-Reykjavik Study

Cognitive outcome Forced Expiratory Volume in 1 second / Height2, n=3665
Model 1 Model 2

β(SE) a P β(SE)a P
Memory b 0.07 (0.02) <.0001 0.05 (0.02) 0.006
Speed b 0.08 (0.02) <.0001 0.04 (0.01) 0.003
Working Memoryb 0.09 (0.02) <.0001 0.06 (0.02) 0.0004
ORa 95% CI ORa 95% CI

   MCI 0.78 0.68, 0.89 0.0004 0.80 0.69, 0.93 0.003
   Dementia 0.68 0.55, 0.83 0.0002 0.68 0.55, 0.84 0.0004
Slope of FEV1, n=1281
β(SE)a β(SE)a

Memory c,d 0.04 (0.03) 0.20 0.02 (0.03) 0.39
Speed c,d 0.02 (0.02) 0.32 0.03 (0.02) 0.25
Working Mem c,d 0.03 (0.03) 0.24 0.04 (0.03) 0.18
ORa (95% CI) ORa (95% CI)

MCIe 1.34 1.04, 1.73 0.02 1.36 1.04, 1.77 0.03
Dementiae 1.15 0.83, 1.58 0.40 1.15 0.81, 1.64 0.44

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.

a

For 1 SD increase of FEV1/ Height2.

b

Demented and MCI subjects excluded (N=416).

c

Demented and MCI subjects excluded (N=130).

Model 1: linear model or multinomial logistic regression model adjusted for age and sex;

Model 2: model 1 + higher education, occupation class, mid-life BMI and physical activity; presence of depressive symptoms, COPD, coronary heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, and smoking habits.

d

Change in score with change in PF

e

Likelihood of outcome with less change in PF over time.

The prevalence of dementia among the 3,665 subjects was 5.6 percent in the lowest quartile of FEV1/height2 and 1.9 percent in the highest FEV1/height2 quartile (Table 1) and the prevalence of MCI was 10.5 and 4.8 percent respectively. Mean FEV1/height2 was 0.93 (SD=0.22) in dementia cases, 0.97 (0.20) in MCI cases, and 1.01 (0.20) in the other subjects. For every increase of one SD of FEV1/height2, there was a decreased likelihood of MCI (0.78 (95% confidence interval, 0.68, 0.89) or dementia (0.68; 0.55, 0.83).(Table 2). The Apo E ε4 and PF interaction was not significant for the presence of dementia or MCI (P=0.64).

Change in pulmonary function

For the 1,281 subjects (1,151 with no MCI or dementia) with at least two assessments of PF, the mean loss of FEV1/Height2 was 1.92 (1.61) percent per year, over an average 7.8-year (2.6) period (total loss of 14.3 percent). In this sub-sample, the association of the baseline FEV1/height2 to cognition was similar to that in the total sample (memory: β=0.08 (SE=0.03), P=0.02, speed: β=0.06 (SE=0.03), P=0.02 and executive function: β=0.10 (SE=0.03), P=0.003). However, there were no significant associations of change in FEV1/Height2 to memory (P=0.88), speed of processing (P=0.80) or executive function (P=0.97) among the non-demented subjects (Table 2). Compared to the normal subjects, subjects with MCI had a smaller decline in PF (P=0.04); no relationship was found with dementia (P=0.39).

DISCUSSION

We found subjects with lower FEV1/height2 at mid-life were more likely, 23 years later, to have lower cognitive test scores or to develop MCI or dementia. Adjustments for a range of factors associated with cerebro-vascular disease or PF, including smoking status, mid-life physical activity and occupation, and late-life COPD attenuated the association of PF to memory, but otherwise did not change the associations of interest. The change in PF over a 7.8-year period was, however, not significantly associated with our cognitive outcomes, with the exception of subjects with MCI, who had a smaller change in PF on average than the subjects who were not MCI or demented.

Strengths of this study include a particularly well-described cohort13 and the availability of multiple cognitive test-scores, aggregated into three normally distributed cognitive domains.15 The dementia and MCI assessment was a multidisciplinary process that was based on a standardized adjudication process.13 Pulmonary assessments were made according to a standardized protocol administered on average 23 years before the measure of cognitive status. Variability due to the spirometry equipment was minimized by maintaining and using the same equipment over time. Not only does this give us insight into early PF characteristics of persons with later cognitive decline it also reduces the error introduced to PF measures when subjects are cognitively impaired and may not be able to fully comply with the protocol. We were also able to control for important confounding factors including cardiovascular risk factors and disease, although residual confounding cannot be excluded.

The predictive value of mid-life PF for cognitive performance later in life has been reported elsewhere.911 Based on previous studies on this topic the mechanism through which PF may affect cognitive function is generally thought to be related to poor oxygen supply to the brain. The brain only accounts for 2% of the total body weight, yet it consumes 20% of the body energy,27 almost exclusively through complete oxidation of glucose.28 Subtle variations of the cerebral blood flow and oxygen saturation are likely to have a significant effect on brain function. Moreover, PF doesn’t only reflect lung disease, but also heart disease and other vascular disorders, which in turn could be the cause of cognitive deficits by themselves. However, studies suggest the mechanism is complicated by the complex effects of constitutional and environmental factors that both contribute to pulmonary and cognitive development.29,30 Indeed, a relationship between PF and cognition can already be observed very early in life, among pre-teens31 and young adults.5 In the British 1946 birth cohort,32 the relationship between PF at 43 years old and the cognitive ability 10 years later was no longer significant when the model was adjusted for cognitive ability at 15 years old suggesting the importance of early life exposures and experiences on late-life functioning. Emery et al. found in the cross-twin correlation analyses of 222 Swedish twin pairs, that FEV1 was associated with cognition at baseline and six years later, and concluded that the genetic effects accounted for more than the environmental effects.12

We found no association between change in PF and late life cognitive scores. Apart from constitutional factors, other factors may explain this lack of association. Our follow-up duration of 7.8 years may have been too short to capture an association, even though PF decreased by 1.9% per year, which is slightly higher than the 1% per year reported in the literature for non-smoking adults.33 The change in PF was measured on average 23 years before the cognitive assessment and patterns of differential change during the interval may have obscured the relationship. Also, since PF predicts mortality in this cohort,14 survival effects may also play a role. For instance, if subjects with fast declining PF died earlier than those with slower declines in PF, those potentially at the highest risk for cognitive impairment would not have survived to the AGES-RS baseline. Lastly, two measurement issues that could have influenced the results are regression to the mean and a ‘floor effect’. Regression to the mean would overall reduce the amount of change PF and a floor effect would truncate the change because subjects with low baseline PF could not substantially decline. These measurement errors may explain the finding that persons with MCI had a slower rate of decline in PF than cognitively normal subjects.

In summary this study shows, subjects with lower mid-life PF had on average lower cognitive scores, and were more often diagnosed with MCI and dementia 23 years later. On the other hand the decrease of PF at mid-life was not associated with cognitive performance, MCI or dementia. These data suggest early efforts to maintain pulmonary health may have a positive influence on late life cognition.

Supplementary Material

Supp FigureS1&TableS1

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank the IHA clinic staff for their invaluable contribution.

This work was supported by the National Institute of Aging (N01-AG-1-2100), National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program and the Hjartavernd (the Icelandic Heart Association) and the Althingi (the Icelandic Parliament).

Sponsor's Role: The sponsors had no role in determining the content and presentation of results.

Abbreviations

PF

pulmonary function

AGES

Age Gene/Environment-Reykjavik Study

FEV1

forced expiratory volume in 1 sec

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

SD

standard deviation

BMI

body mass index

COPD

chronic obstructive pulmonary disease

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: none declared.

The author’s individual contributions are: Conception and design: Launer, Gudnason, Lopez; Acquisition of data: Jonsson, Jonsdottir; Analysis and interpretation of data: Vidal, Launer, Aspelund, Jonsson, Jonsdottir; Drafting of the manuscript: Vidal, Launer; Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Aspelund, Jonsdottir, Lopez, Harris, Gudnason, Jonsson; Statistical expertise: Aspelund; Obtained funding: Launer, Harris, Gudnason.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Incalzi RA, Chiappini F, Fuso L, et al. Predicting cognitive decline in patients with hypoxaemic COPD. Respir Med. 1998;92:527–533. doi: 10.1016/s0954-6111(98)90303-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stuss DT, Peterkin I, Guzman DA, et al. Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: Effects of hypoxia on neurological and neuropsychological measures. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19:515–524. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Albert MS, Jones K, Savage CR, et al. Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychol Aging. 1995;10:578–589. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.4.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Allaire JC, Tamez E, Whitfield KE. Examining the association between lung functioning and cognitive performance in African American adults. J Aging Health. 2007;19:106–122. doi: 10.1177/0898264306297190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Anstey KJ, Windsor TD, Jorm AF, et al. Association of pulmonary function with cognitive performance in early, middle and late adulthood. Gerontology. 2004;50:230–234. doi: 10.1159/000078352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cook NR, Evans DA, Scherr PA, et al. Peak expiratory flow rate and 5-year mortality in an elderly population. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;133:784–794. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.van Boxtel MP, Paas FG, Houx PJ, et al. Aerobic capacity and cognitive performance in a cross-sectional aging study. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1997;29:1357–1365. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199710000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schaub RT, Munzberg H, Borchelt M, et al. Ventilatory capacity and risk for dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2000;55:M677–M683. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.11.m677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chyou PH, White LR, Yano K, et al. Pulmonary function measures as predictors and correlates of cognitive functioning in later life. Am J Epidemiol. 1996;143:750–756. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a008812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Guo X, Waern M, Sjogren K, et al. Midlife respiratory function and Incidence of Alzheimer's disease: A 29-year longitudinal study in women. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Alonso A, Jacobs DRJ, Menotti A, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and dementia mortality: 40 years of follow-up in the Seven Countries Study. J Neurol Sci. 2009;280:79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Emery CF, Pedersen NL, Svartengren M, et al. Longitudinal and genetic effects in the relationship between pulmonary function and cognitive performance. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1998;53:P311–P317. doi: 10.1093/geronb/53b.5.p311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Harris TB, Launer LJ, Eiriksdottir G, et al. Age, Gene/Environment Susceptibility- Reykjavik Study: Multidisciplinary applied phenomics. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165:1076–1087. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwk115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chinn S, Gislason T, Aspelund T, et al. Optimum expression of adult lung function based on all-cause mortality: Results from the Reykjavik study. Respir Med. 2007;101:601–609. doi: 10.1016/j.rmed.2006.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Saczynski JS, Jonsdottir MK, Sigurdsson S, et al. White matter lesions and cognitive performance: The role of cognitively complex leisure activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:848–854. doi: 10.1093/gerona/63.8.848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, et al. California Verbal Learning Test. Second Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:763–776. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1955. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, et al. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): A factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia. 1994;5:266–281. doi: 10.1159/000106735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fourth Edition - DSMIV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Viegi G, Pistelli F, Sherrill DL, et al. Definition, epidemiology and natural history of COPD. Eur Respir J. 2007;30:993–1013. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00082507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Almeida OP, Almeida SA. Short versions of the geriatric depression scale: A study of their validity for the diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1999;14:858–865. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(199910)14:10<858::aid-gps35>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Giltay EJ, Nissinen A, Giampaoli S, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype modifies the association between midlife lung function and cognitive function in old age. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;28:433–441. doi: 10.1159/000255600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Eiriksdottir G, Aspelund T, Bjarnadottir K, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype and statins affect CRP levels through independent and different mechanisms: AGES-Reykjavik Study. Atherosclerosis. 2006;186:222–224. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2005.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Reinmuth OM, Scheinberg P, Bourne B. Total cerebral blood flow and metabolism: A new method for the repeated serial measurement of total cerbral blood flow using iodoantipyrine (1131) with a report of determination in normal human beings of blood flow, oxygen consumption, glucose utilization and respiratory quotient of the whole brain. Arch Neurol. 1965;12:49–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Raichle ME, Mintun MA. Brain work and brain imaging. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29:449–476. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Needleman HL, Schell A, Bellinger D, et al. The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood. An 11-year follow-up report. N Engl J Med. 1990;322:83–88. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199001113220203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yolton K, Dietrich K, Auinger P, et al. Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:98–103. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Suglia SF, Wright RO, Schwartz J, et al. Association between lung function and cognition among children in a prospective birth cohort study. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:356–362. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181656a5a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Richards M, Strachan D, Hardy R, et al. Lung function and cognitive ability in a longitudinal birth cohort study. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:602–608. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000170337.51848.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gore CJ, Crockett AJ, Pederson DG, et al. Spirometric standards for healthy adult lifetime nonsmokers in Australia. Eur Respir J. 1995;8:773–782. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp FigureS1&TableS1

RESOURCES