Dopaminergic therapy to relieve motor manifestations of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease may inadvertently cause inappropriate hypersexuality (Vilas et al., 2012). Impulse control disorders that include gambling, eating and hypersexuality seem to occur more commonly with dopamine receptor agonist therapy than l-DOPA and can lead to substantial morbidity with social, psychological and legal consequences (Vilas et al., 2012). The unregulated nature of direct dopamine receptor activation may provide potential clues to the underlying mechanisms. However, the pathophysiological basis for these behaviours remains unknown. Interestingly, dopaminergic pathways also play a key role in brain networks involved in the reward system, and pathological responses in this system may underlie addiction (Volkow et al., 2012). Similarities between addictive behaviour and drug-induced hypersexuality in Parkinson’s disease suggest that they may share common pathological brain mechanisms. Thus investigations of brain regions and networks related to dopaminergic-induced hypersexuality in Parkinson’s disease could shed light on these potentially common underlying mechanisms and provide clues for rationale treatment. The study by Politis et al. (2013) in this issue of Brain takes an important step in this direction.
Impulse control disorders including pathological gambling and eating, compulsive shopping and hypersexuality, as well as the dopamine dysregulation syndrome have been described in the context of dopaminergic therapy for Parkinson’s disease (Vilas et al., 2012). Similarly, dopaminergic neurotransmission has been linked to drug abuse (Blum et al., 2012). In fact, the dopamine dysregulation syndrome, with parkinsonian patients craving more and more l-DOPA, shares many clinical features with drug addiction (Katzenschlager, 2011). A pathological reward system may underlie addiction, although the precise role of dopaminergic pathways remains controversial (Berridge, 2007). Recently the theory of incentive salience or incentive sensitization has been proposed as the mechanism by which dopamine acts in reward pathways (Berridge, 2007). Incentive salience means that the ‘wanting’ of stimulus has higher valence than achieving the reward from the stimulus, and this may be mediated by dopaminergic pathways in the mesolimbic system (Berridge, 2012). Hypersexuality in patients with Parkinson’s disease undergoing dopaminergic replacement with l-DOPA or dopaminergic stimulation with receptor agonists represents a good model for studying these mechanisms.
Politis et al. (2013) investigated this issue in 12 patients with hypersexuality in association with Parkinson’s disease and 12 without hypersexuality. They used blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional MRI to determine responses to visual stimuli in brain regions with subjects either ON or OFF l-DOPA. The visual stimuli included images of dopaminergic medication, food, money, gambling, sexually suggestive behaviours and neutral cues like nature scenes. They found in patients with hypersexuality that l-DOPA blocks deactivation of multiple brain regions including the isthmus of the cingulate gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, cuneus, claustrum and the insula. These same regions had decreased activity when patients with hypersexuality were OFF l-DOPA and in control patients with Parkinson’s disease without hypersexuality both OFF and ON l-DOPA. Visual sexual cues also activated additional regions in patients with hypersexuality in both the l-DOPA ON and OFF conditions. These included orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, posterior cingulate and ventral striatum–amongst other regions–not seen in the Parkinson’s disease control group and not previously reported in normal subjects exposed to sexual cues. Ratings of sexual desire (the ‘wanting’) after sexual image exposure correlated with BOLD responses in posterior cingulate and ventral striatum (l-DOPA OFF) and anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex (l-DOPA ON) in the Parkinson’s disease with hypersexuality group, whereas the degree to which each participant liked the images did not correlate with responses in any brain region. Taken together, these findings add evidence to the theory that dopamine acts on reward pathways through salience of the stimulus (Berridge, 2007). Furthermore, dopamine may interfere with control of circuits important for reducing desire in patients with Parkinson’s disease with hypersexuality. Interestingly, sexual visual cues did not induce a response in ventral striatum in the Parkinson’s disease group without hypersexuality, whereas others have demonstrated this in subjects without Parkinson’s disease (Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009; Oei et al., 2012). Politis et al. (2013) did not include a control group without Parkinson’s disease in their study.
There are a couple of potential confounds that could have influenced the findings in this study. The patients with hypersexuality were taking more dopamine agonists than the Parkinson’s disease control group. Dopaminergic agonists may have longer lasting brain effects than l-DOPA that could bias the BOLD responses as well as the behavioural results. Dopaminergic agonists also may have regional and pathway specificity that differs from l-DOPA. Furthermore, since dopaminergic agonists are more likely to produce impulse control disorders in patients with Parkinson’s disease than l-DOPA, the group differences found could be a brain response to drug treatment rather than a manifestation of the disease (Weintraub et al., 2010). This, however, does not negate the importance of identifying circuits that underlie this behaviour. On a more technical note, increasing concern has developed on the potential effects of movement in BOLD functional MRI studies. The authors did a careful analysis to eliminate those scans with identified body movements by excluding volumes that had >2 mm movement and also checked to ensure that movement metrics did not differ across groups. However, this 2 mm standard may not be sufficient. This has become an increasingly critical issue for resting state BOLD studies but may also affect research involving task-related BOLD responses such as the work of Politis et al. (2013), although it is more likely to introduce noise thereby reducing sensitivity for the identification of other BOLD signals (Power et al., 2012).
This study contributes to our understanding of hypersexuality associated with Parkinson’s disease and raises further questions in the analysis of impulse control disorders in patients with Parkinson’s disease exposed to dopaminergic medications. What makes some patients more susceptible to developing impulse control disorders? Do the differences in Parkinson’s disease with and without hypersexuality exist prior to exposure to dopaminergic medications? What premorbid factors or biomarkers predict this proclivity? Reliable biomarkers could identify those patients who should avoid dopamine agonists. Would dopaminergic agonists also block the deactivation of the isthmus of the cingulate gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, cuneus, claustrum and the insula? The report by Politis et al. (2013) is an important step in elucidating the pathophysiology of impulse controls disorders in Parkinson’s disease as well as their prevention and treatment.
Funding
NIH (TR000448, NS41509, NS057105 and NS075321); the American Academy of Neurology; American Parkinson Disease Association (APDA) Centre for Advanced Parkinson’s disease Research at Washington University; Greater St. Louis Chapter of the APDA; McDonnell Centre for Higher Brain Function; Barnes-Jewish Hospital Foundation (Elliot Stein Family Fund and Parkinson Disease Research Fund).
References
- Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine's role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;35:1124–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blum K, Chen AL, Giordano J, Borsten J, Chen TJ, Hauser M, et al. The addictive brain: all roads lead to dopamine. J Psychoactive Drugs. 2012;44:134–43. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2012.685407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katzenschlager R. Dopaminergic dysregulation syndrome in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2011;310:271–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:479–87. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oei NY, Rombouts SA, Soeter RP, van Gerven JM, Both S. Dopamine modulates reward system activity during subconscious processing of sexual stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1729–37. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Politis M, Loane C, Wu K, O’Sullivan SS, Woodhead Z, Kiferle L, et al. Neural response to visual sexual cues in dopamine treatment-linked hypersexuality in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 2013;136:400–11. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilas D, Pont-Sunyer C, Tolosa E. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2012;18(Suppl 1):S80–S84. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(11)70026-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D. Addiction circuitry in the human brain. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2012;52:321–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010611-134625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Stacy M, Voon V, et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:589–95. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]