Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 6.
Published in final edited form as: J Fam Psychol. 2012 Oct 22;27(1):30–41. doi: 10.1037/a0029803

Accounting for the Physical and Mental Health Benefits of Entry Into Marriage: A Genetically Informed Study of Selection and Causation

Erin E Horn 1, Yishan Xu 1, Christopher R Beam 1, Eric Turkheimer 1, Robert E Emery 1
PMCID: PMC3645280  NIHMSID: NIHMS456129  PMID: 23088795

Abstract

Married adults show better psychological adjustment and physical health than their separated/divorced or never-married counterparts. However, this apparent “marriage benefit” may be due to social selection, social causation, or both processes. Genetically informed research designs offer critical advantages for helping to disentangle selection from causation by controlling for measured and unmeasured genetic and shared environmental selection. Using young-adult twin and sibling pairs from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Harris, 2009), we conducted genetically informed analyses of the association between entry into marriage, cohabitation, or singlehood and multiple indices of psychological and physical health. The relation between physical health and marriage was completely explained by nonrandom selection. For internalizing behaviors, selection did not fully explain the benefits of marriage or cohabitation relative to being single, whereas for externalizing symptoms, marriage predicted benefits over cohabitation. The genetically informed approach provides perhaps the strongest nonexperimental evidence that these observed effects are causal.

Keywords: marriage, cohabitation, selection, internalizing, externalizing


Empirical research consistently demonstrates that married individuals are better adjusted psychologically (e.g., Wade & Pevalin, 2004), healthier physically (e.g., Hughes & Waite, 2009; Dupre & Meadows, 2007), and less prone to engaging in risky behaviors (e.g., Horwitz & White, 1991; Power, Rodgers, & Hope, 1999). However, skeptics question how this correlation should be interpreted. A recent essay, “What if marriage is bad for us?” represents the unconvinced: “To say marriage creates wealth is to confuse correlation with causation. If there is more wealth in Manhattan than in Brooklyn, that does not mean that moving to Manhattan will make you wealthier” (Essig & Owens, 2009, p. 1).

Social Selection Versus Social Causation

There is little doubt that married individuals enjoy more psychological and physical health benefits than their unmarried counterparts, but it is uncertain whether this “marriage benefit” is the result of social selection or social causation (Carr & Springer, 2010). The social selection hypothesis posits that better-adjusted, healthier individuals become and remain married, and that this selection effect accounts for observed group differences between married and unmarried individuals. Some empirical evidence supports this hypothesis. For example, psychological well-being strongly predicts the probability of later marriage (Mastekaasa, 1992; Stutzer & Frey, 2006), as do lower psychological distress (Hope, Rodgers, & Powers, 1999), fewer alcohol problems (Horwitz & White, 1991), and lower levels of antisocial behavior (Burt et al., 2010; Barnes & Beaver, 2012). On the other hand, the social causation hypothesis suggests that something about marriage causes positive changes and/or protects against negative changes in mental or physical health. However, the social causation hypothesis is a challenge to support unequivocally, because it is practically and ethically impossible to use experimental methods to study the effects of marriage (i.e., we cannot randomly assign couples to marry or divorce). In nonexperimental designs, it always remains conceptually possible that some unknown or uncontrolled selection factor accounts for group differences.

Selection and Causation Revisited

Causal effects of marriage are often assumed after statistically adjusting for factors known to be associated with selection into marriage. However, traditional social science studies are limited to controlling for measured covariates, typically only environmental ones. Genetically informed family designs, in contrast, control for all genetic selection effects, as well as many unmeasured environmental selection effects.

Genetically informed research designs, and twin and sibling studies in particular, offer traditional correlational studies an additional layer of control for parsing selection from causation. By using sibling pairs of varying degrees of genetic relatedness who have been reared together, it is possible to examine a phenotypic—or observed—relationship after taking into account genetic and shared environmental confounds (Turkheimer & Harden, in press; Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). For example, any observed difference between monozygotic (MZ; identical) twins discordant for a life experience (e.g., marital status) cannot be attributable to genetic or shared environmental selection (D’Onofrio et al., 2005; Kendler et al., 1993), and therefore must be the result of environmental factors not shared by the twins (i.e., that life experience for which they are discordant—marital status).

To illustrate, consider an identical twin pair and a fraternal (dizygotic; DZ) twin pair, each discordant for marital status. Suppose these twins inherited genetic characteristics that increase both their risk for experiencing depressive symptoms and their choice to not marry. Despite observing a relationship between marital status and depression at the population level, we would find no relationship when comparing the MZ twins (i.e., the married member of the pair would not be less depressed than her unmarried cotwin, because the relation is genetically mediated).

Suppose now that growing up, these twin pairs were exposed to environmental factors—such as socioeconomic status, parental divorce, or neighborhood characteristics—that are related to both depression and propensity to marry. A population-level relationship would again be observed, but in this instance it would be caused by familial experiences shared between twins. In this case, we would find no differences in depression associated with marital status when comparing either the MZ or the DZ twins, because the relation is mediated by the environment shared between members of sibling pairs.

Finally, suppose that marital status is causally related to depression. In this case, nonshared experience (either marriage or something correlated with marriage within sibling pairs) accounts for differences in outcomes, even after controlling for genetic and environmental confounds shared by members of the same family. We would find that the married MZ and DZ co-twins were better adjusted than their unmarried counterparts (although differences may be larger for the DZ than the MZ pair, if genetic selection partially accounted for the effect).

In pointing to the benefits of genetically informed designs, we note that observing a significant phenotypic association after controlling for genetic and shared environmental confounds is consistent with a causal relationship. It is not, however, probative, because it is possible that other nonshared environmental factors are responsible for the observed effect (e.g., one twin may have pursued more postsecondary education, which tends to be related to both greater marital stability and less depression). Although such possibilities exist, by controlling for all possible genetic and shared environmental confounds—measured or unmeasured—twin studies provide a rigorous test of whether an observed effect is due to selection. Thus, we use the term quasi-causal to describe a phenotypic association that remains significant after these confounds have been controlled.

Genetically Informed Research on the Benefits of Marriage

Among the few genetically informed studies of marital status and its correlates completed to date, a few support the selection hypothesis. In a study of middle-aged Danish twins, Osler, McGue, Lund, and Christensen (2008) observed genetic and shared environmental selection into both marriage and divorce across several physical and psychological health outcomes, including depression and self-rated health. Similarly, Johnson, McGue, Krueger, and Bouchard (2004) demonstrated that personality factors and propensity to marry share common genetic influences. On the other hand, some genetically informed studies demonstrate evidence for causation. Barnes and Beaver (2012) and Burt et al. (2010) demonstrated, in separate samples, that marriage predicts desistance from delinquency after taking into account between-family confounds. Prescott and Kendler (2001) observed reduced alcohol consumption following marriage in a sample of female twins. Likewise, in a matched case-sibling control study, Agerbo, Qin, and Mortensen (2006) found risk ratios for completed suicide to be comparable between siblings and controls discordant for marital status. Heath, Eaves, and Martin (1998) demonstrated that being in a marriage-like relationship (marriage or cohabitation) mitigates the impact of genetic liability to depression.

Entry into Marriage or Cohabitation and the Present Study

Benefits associated with marriage are generally more pronounced for relationship dissolution than for entry into marriage (Hope, Rodgers, & Powers, 1999), and research often focuses on exits from marriage while neglecting union formation. As a result, it is difficult to know whether getting married is beneficial, or whether getting divorced is detrimental (or both). For example, substantial negative differences in mental health have been demonstrated repeatedly when comparing married and divorced adults (e.g., Booth & Amato, 1991; Hope et al., 1999; Wade & Pevalin, 2004), but analogous research on union formation is sparse. Furthermore, selection effects often account for the observed benefits of entry into marriage. Still, after accounting for propensity to marry, entry into marriage is associated with less depression (Lamb, Lee, & DeMaris, 2003; Simon, 2002), psychological distress (Strohschein, McDonough, Monette, & Shao, 2005), alcohol abuse (Power et al., 1999; Simon, 2002), and antisocial behavior (King, Massoglia, & Macmillan, 2007; Burt et al., 2010; Barnes & Beaver, 2012).

Another question to consider is whether legal marriage confers benefits over cohabitation, a step toward or alternative to marriage that has grown common in the United States and throughout much of the industrialized world. It follows, therefore, that a rigorous test of the marriage benefit is the comparison of never-married, cohabiting, and married young adults (excluding married individuals who have divorced). In the present study, we make a series of comparisons between these groups to inform whether marriage confers psychological and physical health benefits. We build upon and extend existing research of the marriage benefit, reporting one of the first behavior genetic studies of marriage in early adulthood, and the first genetically informed study to explicitly examine the effect of cohabitation on benefits typically associated with marriage.

Using a nationally representative sample of young adults in the United States, we use biometric quasi-causal modeling techniques to examine levels of internalizing, externalizing, and physical-health behaviors commonly associated with marital status. First, we compare coupled young adults (combining married and cohabiting individuals—what we call a “marriage-like relationship”) with single young adults. We predicted that a benefit of a marriage-like relationship would exist even after adjusting for self-selection (due to prior health levels, as is practiced in traditional correlational studies) and selection effects due to genetic or shared environmental factors. Second, to create a more robust test of the marriage benefit, we compared married young adults with cohabiting young adults to test the hypothesis that marriage confers greater benefits than cohabiting relationships after adjusting for selection factors. Consistent with previous research contrasting the correlates of cohabitation with those of marriage (Brown, 2000; Marcussen, 2005; Wu & Hart, 2002), we predicted that the benefits of marriage-like relationships would be greater for married individuals.

Method

Sample

Data were obtained from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health; Harris, 2009), a nationally representative sample of young adults in the United States. This ongoing longitudinal study includes four complete waves of data collected between 1994 and 2009; details of data collection and survey procedures are described elsewhere (Harris et al., 2009). The present study uses data collected from 3,226 (1,681 females) members of the Add Health genetic subsample (described below), interviewed during Wave III (between August, 2001 and April, 2002) and Wave IV (between January, 2008 and February, 2009). This subsample is diverse, with 54.4% identifying as non-Hispanic Caucasian, 22.1% as African American, 4.8% as Asian American, and 18.7% as bi- or multiracial or other ethnicity. All respondents at this time had reached early adulthood (mean age = 28.88 years; SD = 1.74 years, range = 24–34), making it suitable for studying physical and psychological costs and benefits associated with entry into cohabitation and marriage. Because marital dissolution is reliably associated with increased distress (e.g., Booth & Amato, 1991; Wade & Pevalin, 2004), respondents who reported being married at Wave III but not at Wave IV (2.9% of the genetic subsample) were excluded from analyses to eliminate the possibility that combining divorced and single individuals could inflate negative effects associated with being single. We also excluded respondents who described themselves as homosexual (1.9% of the genetic subsample). Romantic relationships of same-sex and heterosexual couples do not differ in terms of well-being (e.g., Kurdek, 2005), yet it is unclear how these individuals should be grouped regarding marital status. The legal recognition of same-sex marriage in the United States is varied, and for many lesbian or gay couples, cohabitation may be more a necessity than a choice; for many of these respondents, marriage may not be an option at all.

The Add Health genetic sample is composed of participants identified as monozygotic twin pairs (MZ), dizygotic twin pairs (DZ), full biological siblings (FS), half siblings (HS), cousins (CO), and genetically unrelated siblings (NR; n = 3,139 pairs). Twins’ zygosity was determined primarily on the basis of four self-report items concerning similarity of physical features and frequency with which one twin is confused for the other. The questions are standard nonserological determinates of zygosity and demonstrate high validity (greater than 90% accuracy) when compared with DNA-determined zygosity (Loehlin & Nichols, 1976; Spitz et al., 1996). Pairs of undetermined zygosity were excluded from analysis except in cases where DNA was used to identify twins as MZ or DZ (Harris et al., 2009). To ensure no families were overrepresented, one sibling pair from each family was randomly selected for inclusion in statistical analyses. Overall there were 171 MZ pairs, 264 DZ pairs, 712 FS pairs, 213 HS pairs, 93 CO pairs, and 160 NR pairs included in the analyses, for a total of 1,613 sibling pairs.

Measures

Marital status

Marital status was determined from a household roster completed by respondents. Similar procedures for determining family structure in the Add Health sample have been used elsewhere (Peris & Emery, 2004). Respondents identified individuals with whom they lived as (a) husband/wife; (b) partner/boyfriend/girlfriend; or (c) one of 17 other types of family member (e.g., parent, sibling, grandparent, etc.). We made two orthogonal contrasts in the current analyses. For the first contrast, respondents indicating residence with a spouse or romantic partner were coded as coupled (1); remaining respondents were coded as single (0). For the second contrast, those reporting living with a spouse were coded as married (1); respondents reporting living with a romantic partner were coded as cohabiting (0).

Approximately 40.9% of the sample was single, 17.4% cohabiting, and 41.7% married. There were no significant age differences between coupled (M = 28.85, SD = 1.74) and single (M = 28.92, SD = 1.74) individuals (t = −.997, df = 3220, p = .319), or between married (M = 28.85, SD = 1.76) and cohabiting (M = 28.85, SD = 1.69) individuals (t = −.003, df = 1903, p = .998). No gender differences existed in the coupled–single classification (χ2 = .778, df = 1, p = .378), but women were more likely to be married (54.6% vs. 45.3% of males) in the married–cohabiting marital-status classification (χ2 = 5.894, df = 1, p = .015). Marital status did not differ by ethnicity (European American vs. ethnic minority) for either the coupled-single classification (χ2 = 1.749, df = 1, p = .186) or the married-cohabiting distinction (χ2 = .001, df = 1, p = .976).

Internalizing behavior

We used two measures to operationalize internalizing behavior: depressive symptom count and endorsement of suicidal ideation. Depressive symptom count was assessed using nine items from the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). The original 20-item CES-D Scale is a reliable measure of the frequency of depressive symptoms in young-adult samples (Radloff, 1991). Respondents indicated the extent to which they experienced a cluster of depressive symptoms (bothered by things, could not shake off blues, felt just as good as others, trouble keeping mind on things, felt depressed, too tired to do things, enjoyed life, felt sad, and people disliked me) during the past seven days: never or rarely (0), sometimes (1), a lot of the time (2), most of the time or all of the time (3). Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω (McDonald, 1999) for these items (after reverse coding where appropriate) demonstrated adequate internal consistency (αWave3 = .81, ωWave3 = .74; αWave4 =.82, ωWave4 =.75). We created latent scores composed of these items, and maintained structural invariance across waves. Respondents also indicated whether they had seriously thought about committing suicide in the 12 months prior to data collection (no = 0; yes = 1). Approximately 6.5% of the sample reported having such thoughts during the past year at Wave III and 7.1% at Wave IV.

Externalizing behavior

We used two measures to operationalize externalizing behavior: alcohol use and antisocial behavior. A latent variable comprised of four drinking items was created to examine alcohol use, maintaining structural invariance across waves. Respondents indicated their frequency of drinking, heavy drinking (defined as five drinks or more in a row for males, four drinks for females), and drunkenness during the 12 months prior to data collection: never (1), once or twice (2), once a month or less (3), 2 or 3 days a month (4), 1 or 2 days a week (5), 3 to 5 days a week (6), every day or almost every day (7). Respondents also indicated how many drinks they typically consumed on each occasion. Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω for these items indicated adequate reliability (αWave3 = .89, ωWave3 = .88; αWave4 = .88, ωWave4 = .87).

To assess antisocial behavior, a dichotomous variable was created indicating none (0) or any (1) participation in any of 10 criminal acts during the 12 months prior to data collection: damage property, petty theft, burglary, threaten another with a weapon, sell drugs, grand theft, gang fighting, buy/sell stolen goods, and write a bad check. Approximately 25.8% of the sample reported committing illegal acts during the past year at Wave III and 14.8% at Wave IV.

Health behavior

We used two measures to operationalize health behavior: subjective physical health and cigarette use. Respondents rated their general health as excellent (5), very good (4), good (3), fair (2), or poor (1; MWave3 = 3.99, SDWave3 = .87; MWave4 = 3.63, SDWave4 = .93). This single-item measure of health is considered a “gold standard” of overall health and has been widely used in psychological research (Idler & Benyamini, 1997). It is established as a valid measure of mortality, and tends to be superior even to objective physician ratings in predicting mortality (Ferraro & Farmer, 1999; Idler & Benyamini, 1997). Respondents also reported the number of days in the past month they smoked and the number of cigarettes smoked on each occasion. From this, we created a measure of cigarettes smoked per day (MWave3 = 3.46, SDWave3 = 7.23; MWave4 = 3.48, SDWave4 = 7.37).

Statistical Analyses

For all outcomes in each orthogonal contrast, we fit two structural equation models to the data using the robust weighted least-squares (WLSMV) estimation option in the structural equation modeling software Mplus v. 6.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010b). WLSMV is a pairwise present estimation method, and assumes data to be missing completely at random (Muthén & Muthén, 2010a). Bias-corrected bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals around parameter estimates were computed using 1,000 independent resamplings from the data. These confidence intervals are corrected for nonnormality of parameter-estimate distributions, and as such, may not be symmetric around parameter estimates (Muthén & Muthén, 2010a). Goodness of fit was assessed using the root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1993). Values below .05 indicate close fit, and values up to .08 represent reasonable errors of approximation (Steiger, 1990). We also report values for the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990) and the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI; Bentler, 1990). Values .95–1.00 indicate good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

In biometric models with twin and sibling pairs, the variance of a variable is partitioned into three latent constructs: additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and nonshared environmental (E). Variance decomposition is achieved by constraining the extent to which twins covary on these components. The additive genetic components (A) correlate at ratios appropriate for the proportion of segregating genes shared by sibling dyads (r = 1.0 for MZ twins, 0.5 for DZ twins and full siblings, 0.25 for half siblings, and so on). The shared environmental components (C) correlate at unity for all pairs, since this represents environmental experiences shared by members of a sibling pair. The nonshared environmental components (E) comprise experiences and environments unique to an individual, and thus are not permitted to correlate between siblings. To provide sufficient degrees of freedom to estimate between- and within-pair variance parameters, we fixed thresholds of marital status and binary outcomes to empirically estimated values (see Prescott, 2004). Means and variances of continuous measures were estimated in the same model.

Just as individual indicators (single variables) have their own ACE components, correlated indicators (e.g., the relation between marital status and mental or physical health) may share variance attributable to genes or shared environments, and this covariance may spuriously inflate their phenotypic relationship. The greater the extent to which variables covary through A or C, the more their true relationship is inflated. The effect of marital status on mental or physical health may operate through any ACE variance components, thus A and C must be controlled for to get an unbiased estimate of the marriage benefit.

Controlling for the between-family variables A and C involves a simple mediation analysis (Baron & Kenny, 1986). First, an outcome is regressed onto marital status to get an estimate of its total effect (parameter bPhen in Figure 1a). The outcome is then simultaneously regressed onto both marital status and its between-family (A and C) variance components (see Figure 1b). The direct effect of marital status on the outcome (parameter b′Phen in Figure 1b) is examined while accounting for its indirect effects (parameters bA and bC in Figure 1b). A quasi-causal effect of marital status on the outcome is indicated when its direct effect (parameter bPhen) is significantly different from zero, holding constant its indirect effects (parameters bA and bC; Turkheimer & Harden, in press). In other words, when genes and the shared environment do not fully mediate the relation between marital status and the phenotype, the remaining association cannot be due to genetic or environmental selection, and the social causation explanation is supported. On the other hand, nonrandom selection into marriage is supported if the direct effect of marital status on the phenotype (bPhen) is no longer statistically different from zero when the mediators (A and C) have been included in the model.1 If genes and environments shared between siblings fully mediate the relation between marital status and the phenotype, the relation is explained by familial selection effects rather than unique experience (E), and causation is ruled out.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Phenotypic (a) and biometric (b) models fit to the data. ams2,cms2, and ems2 are the additive genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental (ACE) variance components of marital status; ap2,cp2, and ep2 are the ACE components of the phenotype; bPhen is the total effect of marital status on the phenotype; bPhen is the direct effect of marital status on the phenotype controlling for indirect effects (bA and bC). Although not shown for clarity, ACE components of the phenotypes were constrained to correlate exactly as is shown for the ACE components of marital status. Covariates were partialled from marital status and the phenotype in both models.

As is often done in traditional correlational studies, in all analyses we included gender (0 = female, 1 = male), ethnicity (European American = 0, ethnic minority = 1), mean-centered age, and Wave III scores as covariates. The first model for each outcome was a simple linear (probit where appropriate) regression of phenotype onto marital status (as in Figure 1a). We then fit a mediation model to each outcome demonstrating a statistically significant phenotypic effect.

Results

Coupled Versus Single

Phenotypic models

To estimate the total effect of marital status on a given phenotype, each phenotype was first regressed onto the coupled–single marital status classification, controlling for gender, ethnicity, age, and Wave III score. These estimates and their corresponding bootstrapped standard errors are presented in Table 1. Compared with singlehood, marriage or cohabitation was significantly associated with less self-reported depression (bPhen = −.050, 95% CI = −.096 to −.005), less alcohol use (bPhen =−.577, 95% CI = −.792 to −.422), fewer cigarettes smoked per day (bPhen= −1.011, 95% CI = −1.547 to −.324), lower risk for suicidal ideation (bPhen =−.495, 95% CI = −.743 to −.296), and lower risk for engaging in antisocial behavior (bPhen =−.099, 95% CI = −.216 to −.012). No physical health differences existed between coupled and single individuals (bPhen =−.008, 95% CI = −.102 to .073) after adjusting for covariates.

Table 1.

Unstandardized Parameter Estimates for Phenotypic and Biometric Models Comparing Coupled (1) and Single (0) Individuals

Estimate [95% CI] Internalizing
Externalizing
Health
Depression Suicidal ideation Alcohol use Antisocial behavior Smoking Physical health
Variance components of marital status
 a2 .048 [.000, .113] .044 [.000, .101] .068 [.000, .135] .037 [.000, .089] .047 [.000, .158] .063 [.000, .168]
 c2 .031 [.000, .079] .031 [.001, .067] .021 [.000, .066] .026 [.000, .062] .035 [.000, .080] .030 [.000, 061]
 e2 .139 [.089, .229] .136 [.087, .215] .133 [.083, .209] .112 [.074, .187] .151 [.083, .158] .140 [.122, 27.077]
Regression coefficients
 Total model
  bPhen −.050 [−.096, −.005] −.495 [−.743, −.296] −.577 [−.792, −.422] −.099 [−.216, −.012] −1.001 [−1.547, −.324] −.008 [−.102, .073]
 Mediated model
bPhen
−.113 [−.300, −.007] −.926 [−1.938, −.295] −.688 [−1.080, −.223] −.158 [−.693, .097] −1.357 [−4.132, .376]
  bA .159 [−.080, .561] 1.108 [−.575, 2.834] .275 [−.927, 1.006] .151 [−.542, 1.099] .896 [−3.876, 5.248]
  bC .159 [−.080, .561] 1.108 [−.575, 2.834] .275 [−.927, 1.006] .151 [−.542, 1.099] .896 [−3.876, 5.248]
Covariates
 Effect on marital status
  Wave III score −.243 [−.336, −.160] −.123 [−.237, .011] .008 [−.012, .030] −.110 [−.157, −.059] .002 [−.004, .007] .003 [−.009, .016]
  Gender −.036 [−.075, .014] −.035 [−.072, .013] −.038 [−.077, .012] −.029 [−.066, .011] −.036 [.081, .012] −.042 [−.092, .003]
  Ethnicity −.199 [−.238, −.161] −.192 [−.232, −.156] −.199 [−.236, −.162] −.176 [−.210, −.142] −.206 [−.248, −.165] −.204 [−.259, −.158]
  Age .051 [.035, .072] .049 [.032, .067] .052 [.033, .070] .045 [.030, .061] .051 [.033, .070] .051 [.036, .072]
 Effect on phenotype
  Wave III score .467 [.387, .535] .962 [.656, 1.443] .570 [.513, .619] .488 [.388, .597] .766 [.288, .940] .459 [.416, .501]
  Gender −.104 [−.143, −.069] −.288 [−.459, −.069] .681 [.565, .797] .313 [.263, .368] 1.231 [.509, 1.867] .091 [.012, .160]
  Ethnicity .037 [−.009, .078] −.361 [−.625, −.147] −.593 [−.743, −.450] .113 [.002, .206] −2.615 [−3.628, −1.889] −.147 [−.218, −.071]
  Age −.001 [−.015, .011] .040 [−.025, .110] −.043 [−.083, .007] −.024 [−.051, .008] .070 [−.113, .316] −.022 [−.044, −.001]
Goodness of fit
 RMSEA (CFI/TLI) .027 (.934/.938) .058 (.979/.982) .051 (.921/.928) .072 (.971/.975) .094 (.948/.956) .065 (.975/.979)

Note. Bolded values are significant at p < .05; a2, c2, and e2 are the additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) variance components of marital status; bPhen and bPhen are the total and direct effects, respectively, of marital status on the phenotype; bA and bC are the indirect effects of marital status on the phenotype due to the A and C components of marital status. In each model, bA and bC were constrained to be equal.

Probit regression weights.

Biometric models

To estimate the direct and indirect effects of marital status on phenotype, each outcome demonstrating a statistically significant phenotypic association with marital status was regressed simultaneously onto marital status and its between-family (A and C) components. These estimates are also presented in Table 1. Compared with being single, being coupled remained statistically significantly associated with fewer depressive symptoms ( bPhen=-.113, 95% CI = −.300 to −.007), lower risk of suicidal ideation ( bPhen=-.926, 95% CI = −1.938 to −.295), and less alcohol use ( bPhen=-.688, 95% CI = −1.080 to −.223) after taking into account genetic and shared environmental selection effects. The significant direct effect of marital status on these outcomes is consistent with a quasi-causal relation. Direct effects of marital status on risk for antisocial behavior ( bPhen=-.158, 95% CI = −.693 to .097) and cigarettes smoked per day ( bPhen=-1.357, 95% CI = −4.132 to .376) were no longer significant, suggesting that the relationship is mediated by genetic and/or shared environmental confounds. The nonsignificant direct effect of marital status on these outcomes is evidence for nonrandom selection into marriage (or marriage-like relationships) due to genetic or shared environmental factors.

Married Versus Cohabiting

Phenotypic models

Table 2 presents the results for the phenotypic regressions of each outcome onto marital status as defined by married versus cohabiting, controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and Wave III score. Compared with cohabitation, marriage was associated with significantly lower risk for suicidal ideation (bPhen= −.590, 95% CI = −1.072 to −.305) and antisocial behavior (bPhen = −.332, 95% CI = −.509 to −.224). No phenotypic relations were observed for depressive symptom count (bPhen =.003, 95% CI = −.057 to .064), alcohol use (bPhen = .092, 95% CI = −.124 to .643), cigarette use (bPhen =−.245, 95% CI = −1.073 to .812), or subjective physical health (bPhen =.024, 95% CI = −.063 to .113), after controlling for the covariates.

Table 2.

Unstandardized Parameter Estimates for Phenotypic and Biometric Models Comparing Married (1) and Cohabiting (0) Individuals

Estimate [95% CI] Internalizing
Externalizing
Health
Depression Suicidal ideation Alcohol use Antisocial behavior Smoking Physical health
Variance components of marital status
 a2 .166 [.000, .502] .221 [.076, .409] .142 [.000, .408] .150 [.042, .319] .160 [.000, .368] .184 [.036, .403]
 c2 .000 [.000, .000] .000 [.000, .000] .000 [.000, .124] .000 [.000, .000] .001 [.000, .236] .000 [.000, 000]
 e2 .409 [.142, .753] .404 [.225, .771] .395 [.203, .628] .326 [.178, .528] .332 [.159, .557] .399 [.230, .666]
Regression Coefficients
 Total model
  bPhen .003 [−.057, .064] −.590 [−1.072, −.305] .092 [−.124, .643] −.332 [−.509, −.224] −.245 [−1.073, .812] .024 [−.063, .113]
 Mediated model
bPhen
.117 [−.293, .863] −.326 [−.754, −.067]
  bA −1.761 [−3.435, −.967] −.067 [−1.311, 1.171]
  bC [0] [0]
Covariates
 Effect on marital status
  Wave III score −.428 [−.851, −.234] −.027 [−.081, .274] −.306 [−.560, −.150] −.261 [−.370, −.145] −.013 [−.026, −.005] .136 [.045, .240]
  Gender −.116 [−.228, .028] −.149 [−.267, −.001] −.132 [−.231, .019] −.121 [−.219, −.004] −.124 [−.226, −.002] −.132 [−.236, −.001]
  Ethnicity −.186 [−.286, −.078] −.147 [−.265, −.002] −.170 [−.268, −.074] −.153 [−.239, −.062] −.148 [−.239, −.053] −.171 [−.265, −.071]
  Age .071 [.022, .123] .055 [.007, .110] .067 [.025, .112] .060 [.021, .103] .061 [.022, .105] .066 [.023, .114]
 Effect on phenotype
  Wave III score .601 [.467, .711] −.314 [−.125, 1.130] 1.592 [1.107, 2.280] .225 [.055, .401] .668 [.509, .866] .527 [.440, .594]
  Gender −.124 [−.200, −.060] −1.042 [−1.559, −.613] .770 [.571, .987] .352 [.173, .479] 1.596 [.666, 2.478] .025 [−.093, .167]
  Ethnicity .069 [.017, .125] −.038 [−.506, .417] −.294 [−.497, −.110] .140 [−.004, .280] −1.539 [−2.482, −.644] −.230 [−.348, −.110]
  Age −.016 [−.042, .008] −.015 [−.193, .136] −.048 [−.132, .024] −.030 [−.087, .025] .079 [−.254, .383] −.019 [−.061, .026]
Goodness of Fit
 RMSEA (CFI/TLI) .027 (.864/.872) .041 (.960/.965) .061 (.724/.749) .074 (.841/.867) .078 (.841/.870) .064 (.880/.902)

Note. Bolded values are significant at p < .05; a2, c2, and e2 are the additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) variance components of marital status; bPhen and bPhen are the total and direct effects, respectively, of marital status on the phenotype; bA and bC are the indirect effects of marital status on the phenotype due to the A and C components of marital status.

Probit regression weights.

Biometric models

Once again, we found evidence for non-random selection into marriage due to genetic and shared-environmental factors: Risk of suicidal ideation ( bPhen=.117, 95% CI = −.293 to .863) was no longer significantly associated with marital status after accounting for genetic and shared-environmental confounds. Consistent with the social causation hypothesis, however, marital status was quasi-causally related to antisocial behavior ( bPhen=-.326, 95% CI = −.754 to −.067), such that married individuals fared better than cohabiting individuals.

Discussion

We used a genetically informed research design to provide a powerful test of whether social selection or social causation accounted for various mental and physical health benefits associated with entry into marriage and/or cohabitation. In this first genetically informed study of cohabitation, we predicted that, relative to being single, marriage or cohabitation would be associated with better psychological and physical health, and that these associations would hold after accounting for genetic and shared environmental selection. We also predicted that, after controlling for selection, marriage would confer greater mental and physical health benefits than cohabitation.

Our hypotheses were partially supported. While physical health benefits associated with marriage appeared to be due entirely to nonrandom selection into marriage, a different pattern emerged for internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Holding constant the influence of genetic and shared environmental selection into marriage, we found a quasi-causal protective effect of being coupled on self-reported depression, risk of suicidal ideation, and alcohol use. For example, married or cohabiting MZ twins had mean depression scores that were .13 SD lower than their single co-twins, and were just one fourth as likely to report suicidal ideation. We also observed a quasi-causal protective effect of marriage (compared with cohabitation) on risk for engaging in antisocial behavior. As a concrete example of this overall pattern, cohabiting MZ twins were 67% more likely than their married co-twins to have engaged in antisocial behavior. These latter findings extend upon previous genetically informed studies of the benefits of marriage for externalizing symptoms (Barnes & Beaver, 2012; Burt et al., 2010; Prescott & Kendler, 2001): Like marriage, it appears that cohabitation is protective against alcohol use, but is not protective against antisocial behavior.

The differences between internalizing and externalizing behavior are particularly noteworthy. Marriage and cohabitation both offer positive supports such as a shared emotional life, companionship, and practical assistance, all of which may lead to greater happiness and lower levels of depression. Similarly, marriage and cohabitation both protect against negative experiences such as loneliness and social isolation, factors that may increase the risk for anxiety and depression.

On the other hand, it is of considerable interest that marriage offers benefits over cohabitation in relation to externalizing problems. Young adults who marry rather than enter or maintain a cohabiting relationship exhibit a greater commitment to one another and perhaps adopt longer time horizons in relation to both their intimate partnerships and other aspects of their lives (Emery, Horn, & Beam, in press; Stanley & Markman, 1992). Delay of gratification is a well-known protective factor against externalizing behavior among children. It may well be that young people who marry similarly look further into the future in relation to their intimate partnerships, their antisocial behavior, and, we would predict, other long-term investments, ranging from buying a home to having children to managing expected life difficulties. It is also possible that husbands and wives have or assume “permission” to monitor their partner’s behavior more closely, fostering greater engagement in prosocial activities and less engagement in antisocial ones.

Although we found some important (and apparently causal) benefits associated with relationship and marital status, we expect that considerable variation exists in when and whether cohabitation or marriage benefits individual well-being. Emerging research suggests that cohabitation differs, for example, when couples cohabit with the intention of marrying versus when they do not (Rhoades, Stanley, & Markman, 2009b). Furthermore, considerable research suggests that relationship quality is a critical link between marriage and positive mental health (Brown, 2000; Gove, Hughes, & Style, 1983; Marcussen, 2005). Relationship quality may be an important moderator of the observed similarity between marriage and cohabitation, an important topic for future research, but one that is beyond the scope of the present study. Similarly, relationship duration and stability (Marcussen, 2005) may impact the relation between marriage or cohabitation and individual well-being, and perhaps account for some differences between the two statuses. Finally, the presence of children in the home can be either a positive or a negative for married or cohabiting couples, another topic for future research (Beam et al., 2011). In short, marriages and cohabiting relationships are both heterogeneous, and some forms of cohabitations apparently resemble marriage more than others. Parsing the nature of cohabiting relationships is an important consideration for future research (see Rhoades, Stanley, & Markman, 2009a; Rhoades et al., 2009b), including in genetically informed research designs.

Given clear evidence of the long-term benefits of marriage—for example, regarding longevity (Brockmann & Klein, 2004; Sbarra, Law, & Portley, 2011)—we were somewhat surprised to find that selection effects fully accounted for the physical health “benefit” of entry into marriage. On the one hand, this finding underscores our general concern with selection effects and the benefits of using genetically informed family designs to detect and control for them. On the other hand, we recognize that the possible physical health benefits of marriage may accrue over the life course (Brockmann & Klein, 2004). Young adults generally are in good physical health in comparison with adults in later life, making any differences more difficult to detect. Moreover, we observed that being in a coupled relationship (married or cohabiting) was associated with at least one behavior that is often linked to better, long-term physical health—less alcohol use. If single individuals engage in more unhealthy behaviors—such as heavier alcohol use—we may well observe a causal effect of marital status on physical health in future waves of this sample or in other, older adult samples.

Limitations

Genetically informed designs offer many strong methodological benefits, but we note several limitations of the present study. We did not explicitly compare marriage with singlehood (in order to conduct orthogonal contrasts), thus limiting our ability to detect direct differences between these two groups. At the same time, the inclusion of a cohabiting group allowed us to examine the experience of a substantial portion of our sample (approximately 20%) and of the United States population. Moreover, the inclusion of the cohabiting group provided a rigorous test of the causal impact of marriage per se, and our use of orthogonal contrasts kept our comparisons statistically independent. Future research, in addition to replicating the present findings, may consider alternative contrasts among these groups, for example married versus unmarried and cohabiting versus single.

We also excluded two groups from our analyses: individuals who had experienced marital dissolution and respondents endorsing homosexuality. Our rationale for excluding divorced individuals lies in our primary interest for conducting the present analyses—mental and physical health benefits associated with entry into marriage. Many of the consequences of divorce for adults are better studied later in life when many more people will have divorced, although early divorce and contemporaneous effects of divorce are both of interest to marriage benefit researchers. We did not address either topic in this report, and thus highlight that our findings are relevant for young adults in a marriage (or marriage-like relationship) who have not experienced divorce. Regarding gay and lesbian individuals, research shows that same-sex and heterosexual couples do not differ in terms of well-being (e.g., Kurdek, 2005), and investigating whether the marriage benefit extends to same-sex marriage is an important topic as same-sex marriage becomes more widely recognized. Add Health sample sizes do not allow for such analyses, however, and we believe same-sex relationships are best studied separately until they are more completely accepted and institutionalized.

Finally, we controlled for gender in our analyses rather than explicitly testing for gender-typed expressions of the marriage benefit (e.g., less depression in women vs. less alcohol use in men; Simon, 2002). Of note, in analyses we do not present here, we did not find evidence of gender differences for any of the outcomes presented in this report. However, it is possible that we may be lacking statistical power to detect such differences in this sample. Gender-specific marriage benefits should be addressed by future research on this topic.

Despite these limitations, we believe that the present study contributes to the marriage benefit literature in important ways. This is the first genetically informed study to include cohabitation as a marital status, and it is also the only genetically informed comprehensive study of the marriage benefit in young adults. The strengths of the genetically informed design allow us to provide strong evidence that (a) some putative effects of marriage are attributable to nonrandom selection, (b) others are due to cohabitation either inside or outside of marriage, and (c) still other observed benefits indeed do arise from marriage above and beyond cohabitation.

Implications

Our findings have implications for primary and clinical interventions. The past decade has witnessed legislation supporting marriage promotion initiatives such as the Marriage and Fatherhood Provisions of the Deficit Reduction Act of 2005. Our findings that marriage-like relationships apparently are (despite age, gender, ethnicity, genes, and rearing environment) causally protective against internalizing and externalizing problems are critical to the rationale behind such efforts that assume causation, not just correlation. With respect to more direct clinical interventions, meta-analyses demonstrate that social support and marital status are positively related to medical treatment adherence (DiMatteo, 2004), and social support is related to psychotherapy treatment success (Roehrle & Strouse, 2008). Patients with spouses or partners may have a built-in social support network that can be recruited in the treatment of externalizing and internalizing problems to increase treatment effectiveness.

Genetically Informed Research in Family Psychology

As a final note, we wish to emphasize the broader utility of genetically informed research in distinguishing correlation from causation in family psychology. The present study focuses on marital status, a phenotype for which twins and siblings may be discordant. Other behavior genetic studies of family processes have taken this approach as well, examining such topics as the relationships between marital support and depressive symptoms (e.g., Beam et al., 2011; South & Krueger, 2008; Spotts et al., 2004), and child psychological adjustment and parent–child conflict (e.g., Klahr, McGue, Iacono, & Burt, 2011; Spanos, Klump, Burt, McGue, & Iacono, 2010). Methods have also been developed to examine experiences shared by siblings. Samples of children of twins and children of siblings have been used to study the relationship between children’s psychological adjustment and parental marital instability (e.g., D’Onofrio et al., 2005), marital conflict (Harden et al., 2007), psychopathology (e.g., Singh et al., 2011; Slutske et al., 2008), family functioning (Schermerhorn et al., 2011), and family structure (e.g., Mendle et al., 2009). These are but a few examples of contributions behavior genetic research has made to the field of family psychology, and we encourage researchers to turn their ttention to genetically informed research as a quasi-experimental tool in the correlational study of families.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by a grant from the United States Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health (NIH), National Institute of Child Health & Human Development to Robert E. Emery (1R01HD056354-01), and an award from the NIH, National Institute on Aging (T32AG020500). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Aging or the NIH.

Footnotes

1

These processes are not necessarily mutually exclusive—nonrandom selection and quasi-causation may co-occur.

References

  1. Agerbo E, Qin P, Mortensen PB. Psychiatric illness, socioeconomic status, and marital status in people committing suicide: A matched case-sibling-control study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006;60:776–781. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.042903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnes JC, Beaver KM. Marriage and desistance from crime: A consideration of gene– environment correlation. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2012;74:19–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011 .00884.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beam CR, Horn EE, Hunt SK, Emery RE, Turkheimer E, Martin NG. Revisiting the effect of marital support on depressive symptoms in mothers and fathers: A genetically informed study. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:336–344. doi: 10.1037/a0023758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structure models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Booth A, Amato P. Divorce and psychological stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1991;32:396–407. doi: 10.2307/2137106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brockmann H, Klein T. Love and death in Germany: The marital biography and its effect on mortality. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2004;66:567–581. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00038.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown SL. The effect of union type on psychological well-being: Depression among cohabitors versus marrieds. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2000;41:241–255. doi: 10.2307/2676319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–159. [Google Scholar]
  10. Burt SA, Donnellan B, Humbad M, Hicks BM, McGue M, Iacono WG. Does marriage inhibit antisocial behavior? An examination of selection vs causation via a longitudinal twin design. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:1309–1315. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carr D, Springer K. Advances in families and health research in the 21st century [Special Issue, Decade in review] Journal of Marriage and Family. 2010;72:743–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00728.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. DiMatteo MR. Social support and patient adherence to medical treatment: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2004;23:207–218. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PA, Martin NG. A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:570–586. doi: 10.1037/ 0021-843X.114.4.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dupre ME, Meadows SO. Disaggregating the effects of marital trajectories on health. Journal of Family Issues. 2007;28:623–652. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06296296. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Emery RE, Horn EE, Beam CR. Marriage and improved well-being: Using twins to parse the correlation, asking how marriage helps, and wondering why more people don’t buy a bargain. In: Garrison M, Scott E, editors. Marriage at the crossroads: Law, policy, and the brave new world of twenty-first-century families. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; pp. 126–141. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  16. Essig L, Owens L. What if marriage is bad for us? The Chronicle of Higher Education. 2009 Oct 9; Retrieved from http://www2.econ.iastate.edu/classes/econ362/hallam/NewspaperArticles/MarriageBad.pdf.
  17. Ferraro KF, Farmer MM. Utility of health data from social surveys: Is there a gold standard for measuring morbidity? American Sociological Review. 1999;64:303–315. doi: 10.2307/2657534. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gove WR, Hughes M, Style CB. Does marriage have positive effects on the psychological well-being of the individual? Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24:122–131. doi: 10.2307/2136639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D’Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG. Marital conflict and conduct problems in children of twins. Child Development. 2007;78:1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00982.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harris KM. Machine-readable data file and documentation. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; 2009. The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health): Waves I & II, 1994–1996; Wave III, 2001–2002; Wave IV, 2007–2009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Harris KM, Halpern CT, Whitsel E, Hussey J, Tabor J, Entzel P, Udry JR. The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research design [WWW document] 2009 Retrieved from http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design.
  22. Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Martin NG. Interaction of marital status and genetic risk for symptoms of depression. Twin Research. 1998;1:119–122. doi: 10.1375/136905298320566249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hope S, Rodgers B, Power C. Marital status transitions and psychological distress: Longitudinal evidence from a national population sample. Psychological Medicine. 1999;29:381–389. doi: 10.1017/S0033291798008149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Horwitz AV, White HR. Becoming married, depression, and alcohol problems among young adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1991;32:221–237. doi: 10.2307/2136805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hughes ME, Waite LJ. Marital biography and health at mid-life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2009;50:344–358. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38:21–37. doi: 10.2307/2955359. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2955359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Johnson W, McGue M, Krueger RF, Bouchard TJ. Marriage and personality: A genetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;86:285–294. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kendler KS, Neale MC, MacLean CJ, Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Kessler RC. Smoking and major depression: A causal analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50:36–43. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820130038007. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/content/abstract/50/1/3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. King RD, Massoglia M, Macmillan R. The context of marriage and crime: Gender, the propensity to marry, and offending in early adulthood. Criminology. 2007;45:33–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2007 .00071.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Klahr AM, McGue M, Iacono WG, Burt SA. The association between parent–child conflict and adolescent conduct problems over time: Results from a longitudinal adoption study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120:46–56. doi: 10.1037/a0021350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kurdek LA. What do we know about gay and lesbian couples? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14:251–254. doi: 10.1111/ j.0963-7214.2005.00375.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lamb KA, Lee GR, DeMaris A. Union formation and depression: Selection and relationship effects. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65:953–962. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00953.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Loehlin JC, Nichols RC. Heredity, environment, and personality: A study of 850 sets of twins. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  35. Marcussen K. Explaining differences in mental health between married and cohabiting individuals. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2005;68:239–257. doi: 10.1177/019027250506800304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mastekaasa A. Marriage and psychological well-being: Some evidence on selection into marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1992;54:901–911. doi: 10.2307/353171. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. McDonald RP. Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mendle J, Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Van Hulle CA, D’Onofrio BM, Brooks-Gunn J, Lahey BB. Associations between father absence and age of first sexual intercourse. Child Development. 2009;80:1463–1480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01345.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 6. Los Angeles, CA: Authors; 2010a. [Google Scholar]
  40. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus V. 6.0 [Computer software] Los Angeles, CA: Authors; 2010b. [Google Scholar]
  41. Osler M, McGue M, Lund R, Christensen K. Marital status and twins’ health and behavior: An analysis of middle-aged Danish twins. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70:482– 487. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816f857b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Peris TS, Emery RE. A prospective study of the consequences of marital disruption for adolescents: Predisruption family dynamics and postdisruption adolescent adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:694–704. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Power C, Rodgers B, Hope S. Heavy alcohol consumption and marital status: Disentangling the relationship in a national study of young adults. Addiction. 1999;94:1477–1487. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999 .941014774.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Prescott CA. Using the Mplus computer program to estimate models for continuous and categorical data from twins. Behavior Genetics. 2004;34:17–40. doi: 10.1023/B:BEGE.0000009474.97649.2f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Associations between marital status and alcohol consumption in a longitudinal study of female twins. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2001;62:589–604. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Radloff LS. The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1991;20:149–166. doi: 10.1007/BF01537606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rhoades GK, Stanley SM, Markman HJ. Couples’ reasons for cohabitation: Associations with individual well-being and relationship quality. Journal of Family Issues. 2009a;30:233–258. doi: 10.1177/0192513X08324388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rhoades GK, Stanley SM, Markman HJ. The pre-engagement cohabitation effect: A replication and extension of previous findings. Journal of Family Psychology. 2009b;23:107–111. doi: 10.1037/ a0014358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Roehrle B, Strouse J. Influence of social support on success of therapeutic interventions: A meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training. 2008;45:464–476. doi: 10.1037/a0014333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sbarra DA, Law RW, Portley RM. Divorce and death: A meta-analysis and research agenda for clinical, social, and health psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:454–474. doi: 10.1177/1745691611414724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schermerhorn AC, D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Lichtenstein P, Neiderhiser JM. A genetically informed study of associations between family functioning and child psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47:707–725. doi: 10.1037/a0021362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Simon RW. Revisiting the relationships among gender, marital status, and mental health. AJS: American Journal of Sociology. 2002;107:1065–1096. doi: 10.1086/339225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Singh AL, D’Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Martin NG. Parental depression and offspring psychopathology: A children of twins study. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:1385–1395. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Slutske WS, D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Heath AC, Martin NG. Searching for an environmental effect of parental alcoholism on offspring alcohol use disorder: A genetically informed study of children of alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:534–551. doi: 10.1037/a0012907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. South SC, Krueger RF. Marital quality moderates genetic and environmental influences on the internalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:826–837. doi: 10.1037/a0013499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Spanos A, Klump KL, Burt SA, McGue M, Iacono WG. A longitudinal investigation of the relationship between disordered eating attitudes and behaviors and parent–child conflict: A monozygotic twin differences design. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:293–299. doi: 10.1037/a0019028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Spitz E, Moutier R, Reed T, Busnel MC, Marchaland C, Roubertoux PL, Carlier M. Comparative diagnoses of twin zygosity by SSLP variant analysis, questionnaire, and dermatoglyphic analysis. Behavior Genetics. 1996;26:55–63. doi: 10.1007/BF02361159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Spotts EL, Neiderhiser JM, Ganiban J, Reiss D, Lichtenstein P, Hansson K, Pederson NL. Accounting for depressive symptoms in women: A twin study of associations with interpersonal relationships. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2004;82:101–111. doi: 10.1016/ j.jad.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stanley SM, Markman HJ. Assessing commitment in personal relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1992;54:595–608. doi: 10.2307/353245. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1990;25:173–180. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Strohschein L, McDonough P, Monette G, Shao Q. Marital transitions and mental health: Are there gender differences in the short-term effects of marital status change? Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61:2293–2303. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stutzer A, Frey BS. Does marriage make people happy, or do happy people get married? The Journal of Socio-Economics. 2006;35:326–347. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2005.11.043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Turkheimer E, Harden KP. Behavior genetic research methods: Testing quasi-causal hypotheses using multivariate twin data. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of research methods in personality and social psychology. 2. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; (in press) [Google Scholar]
  65. Turkheimer E, Waldron M. Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:78–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wade TJ, Pevalin DJ. Marital transitions and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2004;45:155–170. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wu Z, Hart R. The effects of marital and nonmarital union transition on health. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2002;64:420–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00420.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES