Abstract
Although the physiological function of sleep is not completely understood, it is well documented that it contributes significantly to the process of learning and memory. Ample evidence suggests that adequate sleep is essential for fostering connections among neuronal networks for memory consolidation in the hippocampus. Sleep deprivation studies are extremely valuable in understanding why we sleep and what are the consequences of sleep loss. Experimental sleep deprivation in animals allows us to gain insight into the mechanism of sleep at levels not possible to study in human subjects. Many useful approaches have been utilized to evaluate the effect of sleep loss on cognitive function, each with relative advantages and disadvantages. In this review we discuss sleep and the detrimental effects of sleep deprivation mostly in experimental animals. The negative effects of sleep deprivation on various aspects of brain function including learning and memory, synaptic plasticity and the state of cognition-related signaling molecules are discussed.
Keywords: Modified Multiple Platform, LTP, LTD, electrophysiology, anxiety, neurogenesis, nicotine, exercise.
1. INTRODUCTION
Deficits in cognitive function, as a consequence of sleep loss are particularly prevalent in modern societies where social and occupational demands make it imperative to sacrifice sleep, to increase productivity. These deficits are especially detrimental when sleep loss is chronic, directly causing disruption in the learning and memory processes at the cellular level. This may imply that sleep plays an essential role in cognitive functions such as attention, emotion and memory, independent of any physical manifestations of sleep loss such as sleepiness or drowsiness [1].
The idea that the function of sleep is merely to restore exhausted brain metabolism offers a limited view into the complex processes that occur in the sleeping brain [2,3]. Expert consensus indicates that sleep is an active phenomenon that incorporates brain regions involved in information processing during wakefulness. This is suggested by experiments that show an increase in sleep amount after learning [4] and improved memory after rapid eye movement (REM) sleep [5,6], together with the presence of certain waking brain activity patterns during REM sleep [7,8]. Interestingly, current evidence further suggests that sleep may be a privileged time window, free of interference from external sensory inputs, that allows the brain to consolidate newly acquired memories [9,10].
The idea that sleep is involved in memory processing dates back to the 19th century with the belief that dreaming could alter associative memories acquired prior to sleep. The general view today is that these two processes are in fact dynamic chemical processes consisting of various types and stages that interact extensively to influence brain functions [11,12]. In fact, it is likely that different stages of sleep are important to certain phases of memory, which are also processed across various brain regions. A particular emphasis has been placed on the hippocampus, where sleep both before and after learning, has been shown to contribute to episodic memory consolidation and neuronal plasticity [10,13-17]. Episodic memory is considered explicit, or declarative memory, which involves conscious and intentional recall of faces, spatial arrangements, objects, and events [18]. This type of memory primarily relies on the hippocampus and associated medial temporal lobe structures as well as neocortical areas for long-term storage [10,18-20]. Several excellent reviews have discussed in great length, some of the highly complex core issues concerning the effects of sleep deprivation (SD) on several aspects including cognitive function, role of stress, sleepiness, role of circadian disruption, and distinctions between human and animal studies [21,22]. Our intention is not to re-visit the same ground already covered extensively by experts who have contributed immensely to sleep literature, instead, our primary purpose is to provide an overview for molecular neuroscientists seeking a concise account of SD effects on learning and memory and associated signal transduction mechanisms. Herein, we discuss sleep deprivation and its detrimental effects on various aspects of brain function including learning and memory, synaptic plasticity and cognition-related signaling enabled via key signal transduction molecules.
2. SLEEP, SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND BRAIN FUNCTION
Sleep, like other physiological processes, is regulated by circadian timing systems in the hypothalamus and hallmarked by the cyclic occurrence of two main phases: Non-rapid-eye movement (NREM) sleep followed by REM sleep [23-25]. Non-REM sleep includes lighter sleep stages 1 (N1) and 2 (N2) followed by the deeper, more restful, slow-wave sleep (SWS) or N3 (formally; stages 3 and 4). Polysomnographic studies in humans were the first to definitively show that different sleep stages manifest and alternate regularly in a composed manner classified as sleep cycles. A complete night of sleep consists of five to six major cycles, and although all sleep cycles in a night last about 90 minutes, the duration of each of the two major sleep phases changes as the night progresses with REM sleep increasing and NREM sleep decreasing [26]. The deepest sleep occurs during stage N3 NREM sleep and is characterized by increased parasympathetic tone and an EEG dominated by large amplitude, low frequency delta waves. In contrast, REM sleep is a period of increased brain activity similar to that observed during wakefulness and is hallmarked by events including a generalized loss of skeletal muscle tone that occurs in temporal association with rapid eye movements [26]. The cyclical occurrence of sleep is well structured, as each sleep stage possesses a particular chemical, cellular and anatomical signature [26].
Sleep shares similarities between humans and animals but there are also distinct differences that may offer insight into the function of sleep. For instance, in adult mammals, there is a cyclic alternation between sleep and wakeful states similar to humans, where NREM sleep always precedes REM sleep, although the alternation between sleep states is more rapid [27]. Sleep stage amounts, daily distributions and temporal factors of ultradian rhythm remain largely species-specific but tend to be directly correlated to the size of the animal and inversely related with basal metabolic rate [27]. Furthermore, depending on the predation risk, prey animals such as birds do not generally engage in lengthy bouts of REM sleep due to its characteristic muscle atonia and high arousal threshold [28]. Also, sleep structure in reptiles, amphibians and fish is more ambiguous in that even though there is an apparent absence of electrophysiological correlates of sleep, they still show behavioral signs of a homeostatic sleep-like state. Another distinctive feature found in aquatic mammals and birds is unihemispheric SWS, a unique state where one cerebral hemisphere shows waking electroencephalographic (EEG) activity while the other simultaneously shows SWS activity. This feature is thought to reconcile the need for sleep and breathing in aquatic mammals while maintaining vigilance against predators [29].
Regardless of the species however, the physiological need for sleep is undoubtedly one of the strongest drives found in animals and humans, which is best signified by the complex yet redundant neurochemistry involved in sleep. Such a process must be vital if it is worth the risk of decreased vigilance and vulnerability to predatory attack. Accordingly, the detrimental effects of SD on different physiological aspects such as: immunological factors, body temperature regulation, endocrine secretion patterns and learning and memory processes are thought to stem from homeostatic imbalance [31]. It is thought that as the organism repeatedly attempts to restore homeostasis by trying to sleep throughout the period of forced wakefulness, the stress of being continuously awake becomes greater and an “allostatic load” develops [26,30]. The allostatic load represents a change in the set point for homeostasis and if left unchecked can lead to structural and functional remodeling of brain regions involved in memory such as the hippocampus [26,30].
Although the structure of sleep has been well characterized in animals and humans, the physiological function of sleep is still not well understood. One of the most intriguing hypotheses is that sleep makes fundamental contributions to memory processes. Indeed, numerous studies show that sleep could contribute to the consolidation of memory and synaptic changes after initial induction of plasticity during wakefulness [31-34]. In fact, SD studies have been useful in showing that sleep can modulate excitatory synaptic transmission and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent signaling in the hippocampus [35-38], which may create the molecular milieu conducive to both functional and structural synaptic plasticity [39-41]. In view of that, a functional imaging study in humans has shown that after a learning task, hippocampal activity increases during sleep [43], while in animals sleep prior to learning can enhance hippocampus dependent memory [44].
Evidence suggests that, certain neuromodulators and cellular processes i.e. gene expression, protein translation mediate the reactivation of previously encoded memory traces and their eventual consolidation in the neocortex. Much of the literature supports a long-term integrative or consolidative role for different phases of sleep in recently acquired memory [15,44]. During NREM sleep, there is synchronized and graded activity between thalamocortical neural networks, which is thought to conduct the initial integration of new memories into a pre-existing long-term memory neural network in the neocortex [10,41]. This process could involve selective reactivation of a particular neuronal network rather than all indiscriminately to prime proper integration of new information on each NREM cycle, specifically at the depolarizing peaks of EEG delta waves. After this initial stage, the subsequent REM sleep phase disconnects the thalamocortical neural networks between the hippocampus and long-term memory stores in the neocortex, which seemingly allows for local NMDA dependent synaptic consolidation processes to take place in these brain regions [10]. This process of selective synaptic strengthening or weakening could serve to better interweave the newly consolidated memories into the existing network and effectively tighten the newly reformed long-term memory network [42].
Much of the evidence for the influence of sleep on memory processes is derived from examining the effects of sleep loss in humans and in animals [34,45-47]. In this respect, sleep loss or deprivation is usually defined as experimentally induced lack of sleep for a certain period of time [1]. Total SD seems to be especially detrimental to brain function in humans and animals; whether it is executive decision making [45], categorization [47], spatial memory [17,48] or contextual fear conditioning [49]. This seems particularly relevant in the context of certain occupations in modern societies e.g. medical interns, airline pilots, the military etc. where lack of sleep is sometimes forced at a time when accurate and timely mental performance is crucial to operational effectiveness or even survival [45,50]. Under such circumstances, performance usually depends on attention to the spatial context at hand and the ability to adapt accordingly, which is primarily provided by the hippocampus in conjunction with higher brain areas.
3. ANIMAL MODELS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION
Many protocols have been developed for experimental SD each with relative advantages and disadvantages. However, there seems to be a common theme across all models to differentiate the stress of forced wakefulness from that of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis response [51]. With that said, it is important to note that just as there are different stages of sleep, there are also different models that claim to selectively abolish a particular stage of sleep. However, there is some uncertainty as to the validity of these methods in modeling human sleep loss, as it is complicated by other factors such as sleep apnea or insomnia, which is difficult for animal models to mimic. Despite this, sleep apnea and insomniac patients who show fragmented sleep and long sleep latencies, respectively, exhibit stress symptoms such as elevated blood levels of cortisol and epinephrine [52,53].
3.1. Forced Activity Model
One of the oldest forced locomotion SD models utilized to abolish total sleep (REM and NREM cycles) for an extended period is the rotating disk over water model. This involves the implantation of electrodes for EEG monitoring and consists of a metal disk suspended over a shallow pool of water. To control for non-specific environmental confounders, a yoked-control animal that does not come into contact with the water is positioned opposite to the sleep-deprived animal. The electrodes are connected to a computer which detects when the animal enters a prohibited sleep phase upon which the disk is rotated at a low speed of 1.33 rpm and the animal awakens and walks along the disk to avoid falling into the water [54,55]. In this case, the yoked control would also have to walk opposite the disk movement and may also be sleep deprived but from an unspecified sleep phase. Forced locomotion paradigms, such as the treadmill method, are very effective SD methods and have been shown to reduce total sleep to around one tenth of baseline after just 24 hours of total SD [54].
The treadmill model that has gained popularity in recent years involves placing animals on a treadmill that moves to awaken the animal upon sleep onset or whenever a prohibited sleep phase is entered. In order to sleep-deprive animals, the forced treadmill paradigm applies forced locomotion 3 seconds on and 12 seconds off while it applies forced locomotion for 15 minutes on and 60 minutes off for control animals. Thus, this ensures both groups walk the same distance but that the controls are able to rest more than their counterparts [56,57]. However, this model may cause fatigue as the animals frequently lose muscle tone after SD, which can confound behavioral observation [58]. Also, the very fact that both the rotating disk and treadmill techniques depend on forced activity can lead to possible confounder stress [54,55,57,59]. Such a bias is evident in the forced treadmill model in that control animals tend to engage in more physical activity compared with their sleep-deprived counterparts, which represents a physical stressor that is fundamentally different than REM SD [51].
3.2. Gentle Handling Model
Although it is virtually impossible to selectively block one particular phase or stage of sleep without affecting the others, partial SD models offer a useful method to acutely abolish parts of the sleep cycle for relatively shorter periods of time. The gentle handling paradigm involves introducing tactile, olfactory or visual stimulus when the animals enter a particular sleep phase (REM or NREM) monitored by EEG recordings or visual observation [60-65]. This paradigm is useful for shorter deprivation periods e.g. 2-8 hours, because it requires dedicated and continuous supervision by the examiners. There is also the issue of animals adapting to the novel stimuli, which significantly limits the duration of sleep loss [66]. In addition, introduction of novel stimuli to an environment may not only keep animals awake, but it becomes a potential confounder by enriching the animal’s environment and perhaps affecting subsequent behavioral performance [67].
3.3. Head Lifting Model
A more recent technique, known as the head-lifting method, is believed to be more specific for REM SD. This SD method is not applicable for long durations or multiple animals at once as it requires the researcher to recognize the onset of REM sleep by monitoring polysomnographic recordings of an animal housed in a different room. Within 2-3 seconds of REM onset the researcher pushes a mechanical lever connected to the rat through a series of pulleys and a flexible wire, which awakens the animal by gently lifting its head [68]. This method is useful in abolishing REM sleep episodes within seconds, making it relatively REM sleep specific, although it is a laborious and time consuming method. Additionally, this method is neither applicable for long durations of time nor useful for multiple animals.
3.4. Enlarged Environment
The enlarged environment method was developed to accomplish total SD up to about four hours. In this method, sleep loss is carried out by stimulating the animal’s natural tendency to explore novel surroundings by providing the animal with an enlarged environment. This method seems to be stress-free since levels of corticosterone were shown to be unaltered after sleep loss using this method [36]. However, similar to the novel objects exposure method, the possible confounding influence of the enrichment inherent in the enlarged environment method on the learning process of behavioral tasks cannot be overlooked [68].
3.5. The Modified Multiple Platform Technique
Also known as the “water tank” or “columns-in-water” or “inverted flowerpot” paradigm, the modified multiple platform was designed to achieve mainly REM SD. In this respect, this method is effective in suppressing about 95% of REM sleep, but can interfere with up to 40% of NREM sleep as well [51,69,70]. The prototype of the multiple platform technique was the single inverted flowerpot method, first used by Jouvet and colleagues [71], which consisted of a single narrow platform or upside down flowerpot immersed to about 2 cm above the water level in a water tank [71]. Upon entering REM sleep and losing muscle tone, the animal on the platform subsequently comes in contact with the water and awakens [73-76]. The multiple platforms method was developed to alleviate forced immobility associated with the single platform technique, which allows for greater freedom of movement. This technique was later expanded into the less intrusive modified multiple platform method, allowing multiple animals from already socially stable cages to be sleep-deprived together on multiple columns [75,77]. This is thought to simultaneously address psychosocial, immobilization and isolation stress often observed in the previous versions of the technique [70,75]. In this respect, cage-mates that are sleep-deprived together as socially stable groups have decreased levels of stress hormones such as plasma corticosterone and ACTH compared to socially unstable groups [75]. Also, in an effort to separate the effects of REM SD from other stressors, numerous different functions have been compared. For instance, Na-K-ATPase activity in the brain stem, cerebellum and cerebrum is increased by REM SD, while stressors such as immobility stress and swimming stress do not, which seems to support the distinctiveness of the effects of this type of sleep loss [78].
4. EFFECTS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION ON THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM
As SD experiments play a key role in elucidating the functions of sleep, many of the first studies documented characteristic behavioral changes after prolonged wakefulness. In the late 1800s, sleep researchers began to keep animals and humans awake and monitored the subsequent effects to infer the function of sleep from these observations [79]. These first studies, mainly performed with dogs, cats and humans, did not possess adequate controls for the methodology used and therefore produced largely inconsistent findings ranging from central nervous system abnormalities to metabolic disruptions including anxiety, body temperature changes and weight gain [80]. Decades later, Dement and colleagues would focus on the psychiatric consequences of REM SD in animals and humans. Results from these studies suggested that REM sleep was strictly homeostatically regulated as evidenced by the more frequent attempts to enter into REM sleep and a robust REM sleep rebound during recovery [81]. Most notably, these studies also observed behavioral disturbances such as difficulty concentrating, anxiety and irritability [82,83]. Both animal and human studies have confirmed that sleep plays an important homeostatic role and that chronic SD is a potent stressor that leads to metabolic and cognitive disturbances in brain areas involved in learning, memory and emotion such as the hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex [30,84]. Furthermore, it seems that under conditions of SD, there is a deactivation and reorganization of executive control in the prefrontal cortex away from focused thinking [85] towards more hallucinatory mentation of the sort observed in psychiatric patients [86,87]. The advent of the EEG allowed for objective monitoring and characterization of brain activity during sleep and prolonged wakefulness as well as the effect of other stimuli on these states. For instance, sleep deprived individuals display fatigue-related elevation in delta and theta wave activity as measured by EEG, which manifests physically as sleepiness, impaired alertness and performance deficits [88]. However, standing upright position attenuates this effect of SD even after 48 hours of SD, seemingly counteracting the negative effects of SD [88]. These findings seem to offer support to anecdotal evidence, implying that upright posture can increase alertness and inhibit sleepiness in individuals who suffer from short or disrupted sleep [89,90].
Both short- and long-term SD imaging studies in humans have shown reduced alertness and impairment in not only simple tasks such as tests of reaction time or attention, but complex tasks as well that test working memory, logical reasoning and decision making [91-93]. Positron emission tomography (PET) studies have shown that 24-72 hours of SD attenuated global brain glucose metabolism, by 6-8% and as much as 15% in the prefrontal cortex, frontal cortex and thalamus, which are key areas involved in mediating attention and higher order cognitive functions [85,94]. However, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) showed that even after 35 hours of SD, regional cortical activity depended upon the task being performed. For example, arithmetic calculations and verbal memory testing, which normally activate the temporal lobe, caused decreased and increased activity, respectively, in the prefrontal cortex and parietal lobe of the right hemisphere [94,95]. This finding is further supported by EEG studies showing that the dominance of functional cluster activity in the frontal lobe shifts toward the right hemisphere during total SD in humans [96]. Moreover, after partial SD, participants maintain performance on a continuous attention task while simultaneously displaying a relative increase in EEG activity in the prefrontal cortex [97]. Thus, it is possible that this attenuation of SD induced impairment is accomplished by recruiting collateral brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex, which are able to compensate for inadequate task performance by increasing conscious motivation during the course of an experiment.
5. EFFECTS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION ON LEARNING AND MEMORY
Several lines of evidence from animal and human studies suggest that SD adversely affects cognitive functions such as attention, learning acquisition, working memory and reference memory. For example, sleep loss impairs learning ability and hippocampus dependent memory performance in rodents as measured by various behavioral paradigms including the Morris water maze (MWM) [98], the radial arm water maze (RAWM) [99] and the novel arm recognition task [100].
In particular, SD in rodents has been shown to impair the ability to retain new information and disrupt memory consolidation [34,48,101-105] in proportion to the amount of sleep loss [106]. In this respect, certain increments of time seem to be necessary for consolidation of new memories and their integration onto already existing circuits of the broader memory network. Thus, integration would require coordinated reactivation of the already existing memories affected by the new information through consecutive sleep cycles so as to avoid possible interference from the overlaying of new information [41]. For instance, 6 hours of SD achieved by gentle handling failed to disrupt spontaneous alternation behavior (SAB) in rat whereas 10 hours of SD was enough to decrease SAB in sleep deprived animals compared to controls [106]. This effect can be seen with other behavioral tests such as the MWM, where 6 hours of SD by gentle handling impaired spatial memory, but not learning, and completely failed to influence nonspatial learning or memory in rats [48]. Additionally, even longer bouts, up to 72 hours, of REM SD in rats also showed similar impairment in spatial learning and memory in the MWM [107]. Additionally, in rats that were REM sleep-deprived prior to training in the RAWM, spatial learning acquisition and short-term memory was significantly impaired [99,108,109] (Fig. 1). It also seems that 24 hr REM SD after RAWM training impairs long term memory (measured at the end of the 24h SD; Fig. 2). In contrast, other reports have shown that SD had no effects on memory function [110,111]. This discrepancy could be attributed to differences in the type of memory tested, complexity of the task and training, stage of sleep disrupted, duration of sleep loss, number of animals used and the experimental protocols employed.
Fig. (1).
Short-term memory (30 min after training) performance in the radial arm water maze (RAWM) by rats sleep deprived for 24 hour (untreated SD) and/or treated with caffeine (caff), nicotine (Nic) or exercise (Exer). The SD of each group revealed marked impairment of short-term memory indicated by the significantly higher number of errors in RAWM. (*) denotes significant difference (p < 0.001; 10-12 rats/group) from control as well as all treatment groups. Inset: diagram of the RAWM (adapted from: Alhaider et al. [108], Zaagar et al. [99]).
Fig. (2).
Long-term memory (24h after training and immediately following post-training SD) performance in the radial arm water maze (RAWM) by rats with 24 hour sleep deprivation (untreated SD) and/or treatment with caffeine (caff), nicotine (Nic) or exercise (Exer). The SD of each group revealed marked impairment of long-term memory indicated by the significantly higher number of errors in RAWM. (*) denotes significant difference (p < 0.001; 10-12 rats/group) from all other groups; (#) denotes significant difference from SD as well as control groups (p<0.05; n=10-12 rats). Note that, unlike nicotine treatment, neither caffeine treatment nor exercise is effective in totally reversing the effect of SD. (adapted from: Alhaider et al. [108]; Aleisa et al [109]).
These findings are especially pertinent in humans as chronic sleep loss is ever-present in modern societies and primarily hinders brain regions involved in learning and memory processes leading to a significant disruption in brain functioning.
6. SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND ANXIETY
Adequate amount of sleep maintains good mood and cognitive acuity and promotes physiological balance and resilience. Unfortunately, increasing demands of modern societies for vocational or lifestyle reasons leave more people sleep deprived, so much so that SD is no more a one night occurrence but a major public health issue. It is now well known that SD negatively impacts the overall quality of life [112]. SD also is frequently comorbid with several physical and mental illnesses, as either a consequence or a contributing factor, thus increasing the enormity of the impact. Relevant to this, several human studies suggest involvement of SD in development of pathological anxiety [113-118]. Anxiety as a disorder is generally represented by feelings of exaggerated worry, apprehension, uncertainty, fear and tension, affecting an estimated 40 million adults [119,120] in a given year in the United States. Unlike the normal, transient anxiety response caused by a stressful event such as public speaking, pathological anxiety lasts at least 6 months and may worsen if left untreated [120]. In fact, persistent insomnia is reported to be associated with an increased risk of developing anxiety disorder [121]. This highly critical association between sleep disturbance and anxiety begs for an extensive inquiry [122,123]. Consistent with the hypothesis that SD intensifies anxiety, an interesting study by Sagaspe and colleagues [113] reported an increase in self-reported anxiety following a night of SD. Relevant to the present discussion, many animal studies using rodent models of SD also have suggested a causal role of SD in anxiety-like behavior [123-128].
Even though SD and anxiety seem to go hand-in-hand, a clear consensus on whether this relationship is co-relational or causal is clearly lacking. For example, some animal studies have reported an anxiolytic-like effect of SD [74,129-132]. Basically, in these studies REM SD in mice and rats results in increased exploration in the elevated plus maze test and increased exploratory activity in the standard open field test indicating a reduction in anxiety. Some researchers interpret increased locomotion as an anxiolytic-like effect of SD in rodents. However, activity based tasks may not be a true measure of anxiety-like behavior in SD as REM SD is known to cause hyperactivity in some animal models [74] and has even been suggested as a model of mania [133].
Several studies have reported that SD decreases anxiety-like behavior while others suggest an increase in anxiety-like behavior. One study reports that, 72 h of REM SD in mice results in anxiogenic behaviors in the elevated plus maze and standard open field tests of anxiety [134]. Furthermore, 72 h treadmill induced SD is reported to increase anxiety-like behavior in open-field and elevated-plus maze tests [127]. SD for 72 h induced by grid suspended over water method caused anxiety-like behavior when tested in mirror chamber, elevated plus maze and zero maze tests [126] in addition to weight loss and impaired locomotor activity [124]. In contrast, other studies report that SD decreases anxiety. REM SD is reported to result in increased exploration in the elevated plus maze indicative of reduced anxiety in mice [135] and rats [132,136]. In addition, REM SD results in increased exploratory activity in open field test [136,137].
Later, Pokk and Zharkovsky [129] studied the effect of SD on the behavioral changes caused by diazepam withdrawal in mice. They reported that 12h after the last dose of diazepam, the mice were REM sleep deprived for 24 h on small platform and that this SD paradigm attenuates the anxiogenic effect of diazepam withdrawal when tested in the elevated plus-maze. Pokk and Zharkovskym [130-131] in two other studies reported that SD of mice for 24 h using the small platform technique caused behavioral excitation that was reflected by an increase in the locomotor activity in the elevated plus maze test. Suchecki et al. [132], using two models of SD in which rats are REM sleep-deprived either by the classical single platform method or by the modified multiple platform method, report that REM SD induces a differential modification of exploratory behavior in the elevated plus maze. Individually sleep deprived rats exhibit increased locomotor activity, while group sleep-deprived rats display less anxiety-like behavior. Furthermore, stress-induced reduction in open field locomotion is reported to be reversed by 12 h SD during the resting phase, an intervention known to have antidepressant effects in humans [138]. Thus, it is quite apparent that the discrepant interpretations, specifically with regard to the animal models, are most likely a result of variations in the SD protocols used and different anxiety tests employed. Human studies have added to even more confusion. For example, one human study reports an increase in self-reported anxiety after 36 h of SD [113] while another suggests that one night of total SD decreases anxiety in depressed patients with no change of anxiety in patients with panic disorder [118]. Low sample size or lack of data on the complications from previous illnesses or co-occurrence of comorbid diseases seems to be the hidden variable.
It is important to consider that SD is not an isolated problem but more often associated with diseases including hypertension, anxiety disorders or depression as a cause or an effect phenomenon. This makes it hard to isolate symptoms/problems associated solely with SD. While a mechanistic basis for the relationship between SD and anxiety remains largely enigmatic, an emerging link is the involvement of oxidative stress in this process. Vollert et al. [139] recently reported that SD is associated with specific changes in oxidative stress and antioxidant enzyme levels as well as alterations in anxiety-like behavior of rats. Interestingly, in this study, treadmill exercise training, through modulation of oxidative stress prevented anxiety-like behavior. In the past, several theories suggest that SD causes oxidative damage, an imbalance between reactive oxygen species (ROS) production and clearance by endogenous antioxidant defense system [134], in discrete areas of the brain [140-142]. This is supported by the fact that sleep is known to decrease oxidative stress [143] by removing oxidants produced during wake time [144]. Therefore, it seems that sleep induces repair, restoration and detoxification [143,144].
In humans, regular physical exercise is reported to promote healthy sleep. It is reported to have antidepressant and anxiolytic effects and known to be protective against stress [145-147]. Although the anxiolytic and antidepressant effects of exercise training are relatively mild in normal individuals, greater anxiolytic and antidepressant effects are reported in depressed subjects [148-151]. However, the molecular basis for these beneficial effects of exercise observed in humans remains unclear. Animal studies do offer some insights into these mechanisms. Specifically, the study of Vollert and colleagues [139] shed new light into these largely unknown biological underpinnings of SD associated anxiety-like behaviors in rats and underscore the causal role of oxidative stress in anxiety-like behavior.
7. SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND NEUROGENESIS
Hippocampal neurogenesis is a life long process in which newborn neurons continue to mature and integrate in the functional network of the dentate gyrus [152-154]. Adult neurogenesis in certain areas of the brain has been observed in mammalian species examined including humans [155-157]. Any impairment in these processes may lead to pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders [158-165].
Sleep is implicated in brain plasticity and mood regulation, hence a functional relationship between neurogenesis, sleep, anxiety, memory and depression seems possible [137,166,167]. Relevant to this, sleep loss is reported to inhibit hippocampal cell proliferation [57] and neurogenesis [168], processes implicated in learning and memory [138,169]. These effects have been produced with multiple days of total SD [170,171], sleep restriction or sleep fragmentation [166, 172,173]. However, shorter durations of SD have produced conflicting results in several rodent studies [174-178].
Some studies suggest that cell proliferation is unchanged by SD [176] while others suggest that acute SD is among the conditions that upregulate hippocampal neurogenesis [177] or that short-term 12 h SD transiently produces more hippocampal progenitor cells via cell cycle acceleration [178]. Species differences are likely responsible for the inconsistent observations [179]. A number of investigators have raised the question of whether the effect of SD on hippocampal neurogenesis is an outcome of circadian disorganization rather than loss of sleep per se. This is a valid question since hippocampal neurons show circadian expression of clock genes in vivo [180-182], and are exposed to daily rhythms of hormones and neurotransmitters, many of which regulate cell proliferation. Recently, however, a report by Mueller et al. [170] suggests that inhibition of hippocampal neurogenesis by SD is independent of circadian disruption, implying involvement of other mechanisms.
Although consensus on the “theory of neurogenesis” that determines SD-induced regulation of learning and memory is lacking, the exact molecular mechanisms governing this relationship remain an enigma. Several prevailing theories implicate a role of brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in this process. BDNF is considered a pivotal modulator of neuronal plasticity, neurogenesis, and cognition [183]. Involvement of oxidative stress in SD induced impairment of neurogenesis and consequent learning and memory deficiency is an attractive concept, even though detailed mechanisms of oxidative stress on neurogenesis remain unknown. Moreover, oxidative stress seems to impair neurogenesis [184,185] and antioxidants can reverse this effect [184,186]. Relevant to this, treadmill exercise training or a physical exercise mimetic (e.g. the antioxidant drug tempol) is reported to prevent increased anxiety-like behavior induced by SD [139] or by direct pharmacological induction of oxidative stress [187] in rats. It remains to be seen whether this behavioral protective effect of treadmill exercise or tempol is mediated by increased neurogenesis. Of note is the fact that SD elicits pro-inflammatory effects [188], which compromise overall immunity and good health. The inflammation-induced impairment of neurogenesis via action of oxidative stress [189-191] prompts us to speculate an intriguing network of possibilities as illustrated in Fig. (3), where oxidative stress appears as a key culprit.
Fig. (3).
Working hypothesis for the involvement of oxidative stress in the effects of sleep deprivation.
8. SLEEP DEPRIVATION PROMOTES INFLAMMATION
It is well known that SD compromises the immune function in humans and experimental animals by provoking a pro-inflammatory response via increased cytokine secretion [188,193,194] including IL-1, tumor necrosis factor (TNF) and IL-6 release [195,196]. C-reactive protein (CRP), a molecule produced by the liver in response to inflammation [197,198], increases upon total and partial SD in healthy volunteers [199,200]. CRP is a crucial marker for SD-induced inflammation, as it is not regulated by circadian variation [199]. Thus, all well-known markers of acute inflammatory system including IL-1 beta, TNF-alpha, IL-6 and CRP exhibit responsiveness to sleep manipulation. However, our knowledge of molecular mechanisms that regulate psychological stress-induced inflammation is not very deep. Several mechanisms have been suggested including involvement of vascular mechanisms, altered stress response system, blood pressure and catecholamine upregulation as possible contributing factors to inflammation during SD [201]. Involvement of metabolic systems (insulin resistance) as putative mechanisms for increased inflammation during SD also has been proposed. Additionally, there is evidence indicating that enhanced pro-inflammatory cytokine signaling may promote ROS generation and lead to oxidative damage [189-192]. There is also evidence suggesting that pharmacological induction of oxidative stress leads to increased CRP-1, TNF-alpha and IL-6 [202] and this might be one mechanism that links SD induced inflammation to neuropsychiatric diseases. Therefore, it is imperative to develop a mechanistic understanding of sleep loss-associated inflammation and its impact on mental health.
9. SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY
Of the various forms of synaptic plasticity, long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) are the most important. Based on the proposition that memory is encoded by changing the strength of synaptic connections among networks of neurons, both LTP and LTD may have an important role in learning and memory and therefore believed to be the cellular correlates of these functions.
9.1. Effect on LTP
Although the effects of sleep and SD on learning and memory have been studied rather extensively, the impact of SD on hippocampal LTP has just recently been investigated. Hippocampal LTP, a neural substrate of learning and memory [203], has two major types; early phase (E-LTP) and late phase (L-LTP), which correlate with short and long-term memory respectively. Both types have been identified in the CA1 and DG areas of hippocampus and require activation of different protein kinases. Additionally, L-LTP requires synthesis of new protein. The inhibitory effects of SD on hippocampal LTP were predicted based on the finding that SD impaired hippocampus-dependent learning and memory [101,204]. The effect of 12 hours of forced locomotion model of SD on LTP was first reported by Campbell and colleagues [55] who showed impaired LTP in area CA1 of rat hippocampal slices. In another investigation, single platform 24, 48 or 72 hours SD impaired both induction and maintenance of LTP in area CA1 of rat hippocampal slices with greater impairment after 72 hour SD compared to that of 24 hours [205]. Both studies reported elevation in the levels of glucocorticoids in sleep-deprived animals due to the use of a stressful method of SD [55,205]. However, elimination of the adrenal stress response to SD by the corticosterone inhibitor, metyrapone [206] or by adrenalectomy [207] did not prevent the effect of SD on learning and memory. Moreover, when a single large platform is used, which introduces an immobilization stress effect but allows the animal to sleep, it does not show impairment of LTP suggesting that the impairment of LTP during SD is not due to elevated stress hormones [34].
Sleep deprivation for 72 hours in the modified multiple platform, which eliminates immobilization and isolation stresses, inhibits LTP in both CA1 and DG areas in hippocampal slices and reduces membrane excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons but not that of the DG granule cells [34]. The finding that SD inhibits LTP of the DG area without impairing membrane excitability of DG granule cells, suggests that the reduction in membrane excitability due to SD is not the main mechanism for the impairment of LTP [34]. In agreement with these studies, recent report showed impairment of LTP in both CA1 and DG areas of urethane anesthetized rats after 24 and 48 h of SD in the modified multiple platform [108]. Furthermore, there was no change in the basal synaptic transmission during SD in CA1 or DG areas [108,208,209]. In these in vivo recordings, 24 or 48 h SD caused near total inhibition of E-LTP in both CA1 and DG areas [99,109] (Fig. 4). Additionally, SD for 24 h caused total inhibition of the L-LTP in both CA1 and DG areas of the hippocampus [209,210].
Fig. (4).
Sleep deprivation (SD) severely suppressed Hippocampal early LTP (E-LTP) represented by increases in fEPSP slope of population spike evoked by HFS of the Schaffer collaterals/commissural synapses in area CA1 of anesthetized rats (measured at 60 min after HFS: 8 pulses at 400 Hz/10s repeated 8 times). Chronic caffeine, nicotine or regular exercise treatment of sleep-deprived rats prevented the deleterious effect of SD. (*) indicates significant difference from all groups (p <0.05-0.001, n=5-7 rats). Inset is population spikes from a representative exercise set of experiments; calibrations, 5 mV/5 ms, apply to all traces. (adapted from: Alhaider et al. [108]; Aleisa et al. [109]; Zagaar et al. [99]).
9.2. Effect on LTD
The effect of SD on LTD is variable and needs to be closely investigated. While one study reported a normal LTD but impaired LTP in area CA1 of rat hippocampal slices after 72 hour SD in multiple platform [34], another study in the mouse hippocampal slices reported impaired LTP with an increase in the magnitude of LTD evoked at 5 Hz, but decreased at 1 Hz [36]. More recent reports showed an enhancement of LTD in anesthetized rats [211] or in rat hippocampal slices [212,213]. These contradictory results can be explained by the use of different SD models, different protocol to induce LTD and different species of animals among others. For example; in addition to different animal species and electrophysiological methods used, the SD paradigms used are variable; some investigators used single platform [34,36] while others used gentle handling [211-213] for varying durations of SD. Therefore, whereas SD suppresses LTP in hippocampal CA1 [34-36,109,210,211,214] and dentate gyrus (DG) areas [108,215,216], LTD, induced by paired-pulse facilitation, is not affected after 24 hours of sleep loss [214].
The negative impact that SD has on synaptic plasticity is thought to be a product of the underlying deleterious changes in plasticity-related intracellular signaling. For instance, hippocampal glutamate NMDA receptors, which are critical for LTP induction, show negative alterations in receptor subunit composition and turnover after 24 hours of REM SD. It has been shown that SD reduces the expression of NMDA receptors, decreases the levels of the GluN1 subunit of NMDA receptor and lowers excitatory postsynaptic currents in area CA1 [37]. However, recently, it has been reported that although 72 hours of SD decreases the expression of GluN2B receptor subunit in the frontal cortex and hippocampus, it does not change GluN1 subunit [217]. Thus, it seems that the duration of SD may have differential effects on NMDA receptors. Interestingly, after 30 days of 20 hours/day sleep restriction, there was less NMDA-induced excitotoxicity in these rats [218]. This is probably due to loss of neurons as well as decreased number of NMDA receptors. In addition, 12 hours of SD impairs both phosphorylation and membrane trafficking of hippocampal glutamate alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors, which are crucial in maintaining synaptic plasticity [219]. These changes may affect the activity of several downstream signaling molecules. In particular, the expression of phosphorylated calcium calmodulin kinase (P-CaMKII) is markedly decreased after 24 hours of SD whereas the levels and activity of calcineurin are increased [107,108].
10. SLEEP DEPRIVATION DISRUPTS COGNITION-RELATED SIGNALING MOLECULES
The negative impact of SD on LTP and synaptic plasticity is thought to be a product of disruptive changes in intracellular signaling molecules and receptors including NMDA [220,221] and AMPA receptors [100]. For instance, molecular studies have shown that the expression of key signaling proteins and trophic factors, e.g. CREB and BDNF, involved in LTP and memory are impaired in the hippocampus after 8, 24, and 48 hours of SD [46, 108]. Also, SD can negatively impact other intracellular signaling pathways such as the cAMP/PKA pathway, which plays an important modulatory role in LTP and memory. Sleep loss, for as little as 5 hours, increases the levels of phosphodiesterase IV, which in turn impairs LTP expression and decreases the levels of cAMP [222]. We have shown impaired synaptic plasticity in various brain disorders to be correlated with impaired levels of P-CaMKII and P-CREB during expression of E-LTP and L-LTP, respectively [223-226]. In the following sections we briefly discuss the effect of SD on a number of signaling proteins essential for expression of synaptic plasticity.
10.1. Calcium Calmodulin-dependent Protein Kinase II (CaMKII)
CaMKII is critical in the expression of LTP in the hippocampus [227-230]. The negative impact of SD on memory is thought to be mediated by deleterious changes in intracellular signaling indicated by the down-regulation of basal and stimulated P-CaMKII protein expression in both the CA1 and DG areas [108]. Other molecular studies support this conclusion showing that the expression and activation of CaMKII are impaired in the hippocampus after various durations of SD [46,208]. For instance, the expression of P-CaMKII during E-LTP is markedly decreased after 24 hours of SD whereas the level of the phosphatase calcineurin, which dephosphorylates CaMKII, is increased [108,208]. Sleep deprivation for 24 hours reduced the ratio of P-CaMKII:total CaMKII at the basal and stimulated levels in the DG area, which may indicate that SD mainly hinders the phosphorylation process of CaMKII under these conditions [99,108]. In area CA1, however, the ratio of basal P-CaMKII:total CaMKII was largely unchanged across the groups, implying that SD primarily reduces P-CaMKII levels by decreasing the overall synthesis of CaMKII. The down regulation of P-CaMKII by SD (Table 1) has far-reaching consequences on other molecules and receptors e.g. AMPA and NMDA receptors that ultimately lead to suppression of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus [219,220].
Table 1.
Summary of the Effects of SD and/or Regular Exercise, or Caffeine on the Basal Levels of Signaling Molecules Important for E-LTP and L-LTP Expression
SD | Exercise | Exercise/SD | Caffeine | Caffeine/SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
P-CaMKII | Decrease | No change | No change | No change | No change |
Total CaMKII | Decrease | No change | No change | Increase | No change |
Calcineurin | No change | No change | No change | No change | No change |
BDNF | Decrease | Increase | Increase | No change | No change |
P-CREB | Decrease | Increase | Increase | No change | No change |
Total-CREB | Decrease | No change | No change | No change | No change |
CaMKIV | Decrease | No change | No change | No change | No change |
Protein levels of P-CaMKII, total-CaMKII, calcineurin, BDNF, P-CREB, total-CREB, and CaMKIV in area CA1 of sleep-deprived, exercised, and exercised/ sleep-deprived rats were determined by immunoblot analysis and compared to those of the control rats. All increases or decreases are significantly different from control (p<0.05-0.001; n=5-7 animals) (adapted from: Alhaider et al. [99,108,208]).
10.2. Calcineurin (PP2B)
Protein phosphatases are responsible for the dephosphorylation and deactivation processes of previously phosphorylated molecules. Activation of calcineurin (PP2B) by Ca2+-calmodulin leads to the activation of protein phosphatase 1 (PP1), which in turn inactivates P-CaMKII [231,232]. Evidence indicates that calcineurin reduces post-synaptic activity and impairs LTP in the hippocampus [233,234]. It has been reported that over-expression of calcineurin in the hippocampus impairs LTP [234] and that pharmacological inhibitors of calcineurin facilitate LTP in hippocampal slices [233].
We have shown that basal protein levels of calcineurin in the total homogenate of the CA1 and DG areas were not affected by 24 h of SD [99,108] (Table 1). Sleep loss, even for 72 h, does not affect the basal levels of calcineurin in the hippocampus [17]. Furthermore, it has been reported that SD does not influence the level of PP1, which is activated by calcineurin [48]. Although SD does not seem to interfere with the basal protein levels of calcineurin in the CA1 and DG areas of the hippocampus, the possibility that SD may affect only calcineurin activity or the level/activity of another phosphatase cannot be excluded. In fact, the activity of calcineurin has been found to increase after 72 h of SD [107]. Thus, SD seems to enhance the activity of existing calcineurin without affecting its expression.
10.3. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF)
Endogenous BDNF plays an essential role in spatial learning and memory as well as hippocampal LTP. Studies in BDNF-knockout mice show impairment in spatial memory [235] and hippocampal LTP [236,237]. Furthermore, the gene expression of BDNF has been reported to be increased in the hippocampus after spatial learning tasks [238,239], and after the induction of LTP [240-244].
BDNF influences synaptic plasticity by raising the efficiency of synaptic transmission through activation of CaMKII and CREB [245,246]. Particularly in the hippocampus, BDNF enhances activation of CaMKII and CREB, probably by a mechanism that involves the release of Ca2+ from the internal stores through the tropomyosin kinase B (TrkB) receptor/phospholipase Cγ pathway [245,247,248]. Therefore, it is possible that SD impairs E-LTP by suppressing the increase of BDNF and P-CaMKII at the basal and stimulated levels. Twenty four hour SD reduces the basal levels of BDNF in the CA1 (Table 1) and DG areas, which is consistent with the finding that 8 and 48 h periods of SD decrease the gene expression and the protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus [46]. In our studies, the decreased basal levels of up-stream effectors such as BDNF in the CA1 and DG areas of sleep-deprived rats may be considered a contributing factor to the reduction of both P-CaMKII and P-CREB basal levels. Additionally, the SD-induced suppression of BDNF up-regulation after stimulation is indicative of decreased availability of extracellular BDNF [99,108,208,209].
10.4. cAMP Response Element Binding Protein (CREB)
CREB is a transcription factor involved in the regulation of CRE-containing genes. Accumulating evidence has shown that expression of L-LTP and long-term memory requires CREB activation [249-252]. Induction of repressor or activator forms of CREB affects long-term memory negatively or positively, respectively, without affecting short-term memory [249,253].
CREB mediates the synthesis of proteins that are important for long-term synaptic plasticity and memory. The gene expression of CREB has been found to decrease after 8 and 48h periods of sleep loss in the hippocampus [46]. This reduction could be attributed to the significant decrease in the upstream modulators of CREB including extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) and calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV (CaMKIV). In fact, it has been reported that sleep loss decreases the levels of ERK phosphorylation [48]. Moreover, evidence shows that 24 h SD decreases the basal protein levels of total-CREB and P-CREB in CA1 and DG areas [209] (Table 1). Furthermore, repetitive stimulation in anesthetized sleep-deprived anesthetized rats fails to induce the expected up-regulation of phosphorylated CREB, which may be responsible for the impairment of L-LTP in the CA1 area of sleep-deprived rats [209].
10.5. Calcium/Calmodulin-dependent Protein Kinase IV (CaMKIV)
The transcriptional activator CaMKIV is present in the nuclei and cytosol of neurons of various brain areas including the hippocampus [254,255]. Upon repetitive stimulation, the levels of CaMKIV increase resulting in phosphorylation and activation of CREB [256,257]. Mice with mutation in CaMKIV show impairment in L-LTP, long-term memory and CREB phosphorylation in the hippocampus [258,259].
Our published and unpublished findings revealed reduced basal protein levels of CaMKIV in CA1 and DG areas (Table 1) of the hippocampus of the sleep-deprived rats. However, repetitive stimulation produced the expected up-regulation of CaMKIV in sleep-deprived rats [209]. It seems that reduced basal CaMKIV levels may be a factor that contributes to the reduced levels of CREB phosphorylation in sleep-deprived rats leading to impairment of L-LTP.
11. COUNTERING THE EFFECTS OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION ON LEARNING AND MEMORY
To offset the effects of sleep loss and to sustain performance during periods of sleepiness or prolonged wakefulness, pharmacological measures are often required. Commonly used stimulants, including caffeine and nicotine, have different mechanisms of action and varying side effect profiles. In this section, we review some successful measures to counteract the neurobiological deleterious effects of SD in rodent models.
11.1. Nicotine
Nicotine treatment is known to attenuate the impairment of learning and memory and LTP associated with several mental disorders including Alzheimer’s disease and chronic psychosocial stress [224,260]. Treatment of animals acutely with nicotine (1 mg/kg; subcutaneously, twice/daytime) during 24 or 48 h SD in the modified multiple platform significantly prevented or attenuated SD-induced impairment of learning, short-term and long-term memory as measured in the RAWM (Figs. 1 and 2). Moreover, extracellular recording of field potentials from CA1 and DG areas of the hippocampus in urethane-anesthetized rats showed that concurrent, acute treatment with nicotine prevented SD-induced impairment of LTP [109,210] (Fig. 4).
The action of nicotine could be due to a number of possible mechanisms. It has been suggested that nicotine increases the probability of coincidence between presynaptic transmitter release and postsynaptic depolarization, thus raising the probability of LTP induction [261,262]. Furthermore, desensitization of α7-nicotininc acetylcholine receptors (α7 nAChR) reduces the release of gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) from interneurons, thus indirectly increasing pyramidal cell excitability [263] this, in turn, facilitates LTP by decreasing its threshold of induction [264]. Another possible mechanism by which nicotine may counter the deleterious effects of SD is through BDNF. Nicotine treatment increases BDNF mRNA and protein levels [224,265]. In contrast, SD reduces the expression and the protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus including the DG area [46,108] and reduces the protein levels of the phosphorylated form of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase 2 (phospho-ERK2), which is a major target of BDNF receptor activation [48]. Therefore, it is possible that nicotine treatment may have prevented the effects of SD on memory and synaptic plasticity by restoring normal levels of BDNF.
11.2. Caffeine
Caffeine is a common ingredient in numerous beverages that are consumed on a daily basis worldwide. The consumption of caffeine-containing beverages is known to increase during sleep loss to improve alertness and enhance concentration [266]. Indeed, low doses of caffeine have positive effects on learning and memory [267] and chronic caffeine intake has been shown to alleviate cognitive impairment in different animal models of brain disorders including Alzheimer’s disease [268], Parkinson disease [269], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [270], age-related cognitive decline [271] and scopolamine-induced amnesia [272].
In sleep deprived rats chronic caffeine treatment (0.3 g/L of drinking water) prevented learning and memory impairment induced by 24 h SD in the modified multiple platform method [108,208] (Figs. 1 and 2). Chronic caffeine treatment also prevented the SD-induced impairment of E-LTP and L-LTP of the CA1 and DG areas of the hippocampus [108] (Fig. 4). At the molecular level, chronic caffeine prevented SD-induced deficits in the basal and induced levels of a variety of signaling molecules including CaMKII and BDNF [108,208,209] (Table 1:see section 10).
It has been suggested that caffeine may act mainly by inhibiting adenosine receptors [273]. There are at least four adenosine G-protein coupled receptors subtypes: A1, A2a, A2b and A3, of which the A1 subtype is highly expressed in the hippocampus. Adenosine activation of A1 receptors inhibits release of glutamate and suppresses LTP in rat hippocampal slices and thus disrupting the process of learning and memory at the synaptic level [274]. In addition, adenosine stabilizes magnesium ion on postsynaptic NMDA receptors, thus preventing NMDA receptor-mediated excitation [275,276]. It has been reported that SD, through induced production of nitric oxide, enhances extracellular adenosine in the basal forebrain leading to enhanced sleep intensity [277]. Therefore, by antagonizing the effects of adenosine, chronic caffeine may sustain the homeostasis in the brain.
11.3. Physical Exercise
Regular aerobic exercise is a promising non-pharmacological approach that can ameliorate memory impairment in a variety of conditions including brain injury [278-280], and can enhance cognitive function [281], prevent memory decline in aging [282,283], decrease anxiety related behaviors [139,187] and attenuate oxidative stress [284,285,287].
In sleep-deprived rats the impact of 4 weeks of regular treadmill exercise on SD induced impairment of spatial learning and memory, synaptic plasticity and related signaling molecules in area CA1 of the rat hippocampus was examined [99]. Rats were regularly exercised on a treadmill and subsequently sleep-deprived for 24 h in the modified multiple platform method. Testing of learning and memory performance in the RAWM showed that although sedentary sleep deprived rats were severely impaired, the performance of exercised sleep deprived rats was normal (Figs. 1 and 2). Additionally, the markedly impaired E-LTP in the sedentary sleep deprived animals was normal in the exercised sleep deprived group (Fig. 4). At the molecular level, immunoblot analysis of CA1 area before (basal levels; Table 1) and after expression of E-LTP indicated that the significant down-regulation of BDNF and phosphorylated CaMKII levels seen in sleep-deprived animals, was prevented with regular treadmill exercise.
Although the precise mechanism by which regular exercise prevents the deleterious effects of SD on memory and LTP is not clearly understood, neurotrophic factors, particularly BDNF, possess most of the properties that could mediate the beneficial effects of exercise on neuronal transmission and connectivity as well as activity-dependent plasticity. Considerable evidence has shown that by up-regulating BDNF, exercise seems to foster the signaling cascades responsible for synaptic plasticity and cognition [286]. Exercise not only induces the expression of a wide array of plasticity-related genes, but can also up-regulate BDNF by engaging aspects of epigenetic regulation to facilitate BDNF gene transcription [287]. These mechanisms may prepare the synapses structurally and functionally to facilitate activity dependent plasticity in the presence of the SD-induced insult. The protective mechanism of regular exercise against SD-induced E-LTP and L-LTP impairment in the hippocampus is accompanied by increasing levels of BDNF [99] (Table 1), which through its modulatory effects can enhance activity of various signaling cascades to ultimately prevent the decrease in the levels of P-CaMKII and P-CREB in sleep-deprived rats.
12. CONCLUDING REMARKS
Overall, the work presented in this review generally highlights aspects of SD that have been insufficiently studied namely the effect of SD on the brain at the electrophysiological and neuromolecular levels. Electrophysiological and molecular studies have been carried out in animals and although the immediate application of animal data to humans is clearly inappropriate, evidence obtained from human studies recapitulates the basic themes emerging from animal research.
The interpretation of SD-induced alterations in brain function is complicated by the variety of SD paradigms and protocols used to induce sleep loss. The complications result from the variable length of time of SD, time of day, and most importantly whether the effect of sleep loss is tainted by stresses resulting from unfamiliar environment, social isolation or movement restriction. Use of different strains in different studies is another complicating factor. However, recent experiments increasingly use SD paradigms and protocols that are designed to limit exposure to these stresses and other complicating factors. Thus, it is important for researchers to study the effect of SD uncontaminated by activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. This can be successfully done through elimination of the adrenal stress response to SD by treatment with the corticosterone inhibitor, metyrapone or by adrenalectomy [206]. In a different approach, we have shown that stress influences the hippocampal area CA1 differently than the DG area. For example, psychosocial stress impairs LTP in area CA1 without affecting the DG area [288,289]. We measured the effect of SD in both areas using electrophysiological and biochemical methods and found that SD negatively impacts LTP and related signaling molecules markedly, in both the CA1 and DG areas [108,208]. This led us to conclude that the memory impairment, LTP suppression and disruption of related molecular cascades seen in rats sleep-deprived in the columns-in-water method are actually the results of extended wakefulness as opposed to any non-specific stresses as a consequence of being in the aquarium environment.
ABBREVIATIONS
- AMPA
= α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionic acid
- BDNF
= brain derive neurotrophic factor
- CA1
= Cornu Amonis 1
- CaMKII
= Calcium calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II
- CRP
= C-reactive protein
- CREB
= cAMP response element binding protein
- DG
= Dentate Gyrus
- EEG
= Electroencephalogram
- E-LTP
= early phase long-term potentiation
- ERK2
= extracellular signal-regulated kinase
- L-LTP
= late phase long-term potentiation
- LTD
= long-term depression
- MWM
= Morris Water Maze
- GluN1
= a subunit of NMDA glutamate receptor
- GluN2B
= a subunit of NMDA glutamate receptor
- NMDA
= N-methyl-D-aspartate
- NREM
= non Rapid Eye Movement
- RAWM
= Radial Arm Water Maze
- REM
= Rapid Eye Movement
- ROS
= reactive oxygen species
- SAB
= Spontaneous alternation behavior
- SD
= sleep deprivation
- SWS
= slow wave sleep
- TNF
= Tumor necrosis factor
- TrkB
= tropomyosine kinase B
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES/CONFLICT OF INTEREST/ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The University of Houston, a Carnegie Tier One Research Institution, supported this work by a number of SGP awards (to KAA) and by an NIH/NIMH (to SS) Grant # 1R15MH093918) This work was not an industry supported study. The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Orzel-Gryglewska J. Consequences of sleep deprivation. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ Health . 2010;23:95–114. doi: 10.2478/v10001-010-0004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kalia M. Neurobiology of sleep. Metabolism . 2006;55:S2–S6. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2006.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Luppi PH. Neurochemical aspects of sleep regulation with specific focus on slow-wave sleep. World J. Biol.Psychiatry . 2010;11(Suppl 1 ):4–8. doi: 10.3109/15622971003637611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lucero MA. Lengthening of REM sleep duration consecutive to learning in the rat. Brain Res. 1970;20:319–22. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90299-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Smith C, Kitahama K, Valatx JL, Jouvet M. Increased paradoxical sleep in mice during acquisition of a shock avoidance task. Brain Res. 1974;77(2 ):221–30. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90786-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Smith C, Lapp L. Increases in number of REMS and REM density in humans following an intensive learning period. Sleep . 1991;14(4 ):325–30. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.4.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Timo-Iaria C, Negrao N, Schmidek WR, Hoshino K, Lobato de Menezes CE, Leme da Rocha T. Phases and states of sleep in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1970;5:1057–62. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(70)90162-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cantero JL, Atienza M, Stickgold R, Kahana M, Madsen JR, Kocsis B. Sleep-dependent theta oscillations in the human hippocampus and neocortex. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:10897–10903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10897.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bryson D, Schacher S, Diekelmann S, Born J. Behavioral analysis of mammalian sleep and learning. Perspect Biol. Med. 1969;13:71–79.10. doi: 10.1353/pbm.1969.0042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:114–26. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleppe. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:144–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron . 2004;44:121–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rauchs G, Desgranges B, Foret J, Eustache F. The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. J. Sleep Res. 2005;14:123–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00450.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gais S, Albouy G, Boly M, Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, Desseilles M, Rauchs G, Schabus M, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Maquet P, Peigneux P. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:18778–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705454104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Orban P, Rauchs G, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Maquet P, Peigneux P. Sleep after spatial learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2006;103:7124–29. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510198103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep memory and plasticity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006;57:139–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Yoo S S, Hu P T, Gujar N, Jolesz F A, Walker M P. A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:385–392. doi: 10.1038/nn1851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Errara M, Iaria G, Tempesta D, et al. Sleep to find your way: the role of sleep in the consolidation of memory for navigation in humans. Hippocampus . 2008;18:844–51. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Burgess N, Maguire EA, O'Keefe J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron . 2002;35:625–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00830-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cohen NJ, Squire LR. Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Sci. NY. 1980;210:207–10. doi: 10.1126/science.7414331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kirwan CB, Gilbert PE, Kesner RP. The role of the hippocampus in the retrieval of a spatial location. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 2005;83:65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Faraut B, Boudjeltia KZ, Vanhamme L, Kerkhofs M. Immune, inflammatory and cardiovascular consequences of sleep restriction and recovery. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012;16:137–49. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Aldabal L, Bahammam AS. Metabolic, endocrine, and immune consequences of sleep deprivation. Open Respir. Med. J. 2011;5:31–43. doi: 10.2174/1874306401105010031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Stones MJ. Memory performance after arousal from different sleep stages. Br. J. Psychol. 1977;68:177–181. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1977.tb01573.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes. Behav. Brain Res. 1995;69:137–45. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00024-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stenberg D. Neuroanatomy and neurochemistry of sleep. Cell Mol. Life Sci. 2007;64:1187–1204. doi: 10.1007/s00018-007-6530-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McCarley RW. Neurobiology of REM and NREM sleep. Sleep Med. 2007;8:302–30. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lesku JRN, Amlaner C. The Evolution of Sleep: A Phylogenetic Approach. In: Lee-Chiong T, editor. Sleep A Comprehensive Handbook. Vol. 1. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. p. 1038. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Elgar MA, Pagel MD, Harvey PH. Sleep in mammals. Anim Behav. 1988;36:1407–19. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rattenborg NC, Amlaner CJ, Lima SL. Behavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev . 2000;24:817–42. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00039-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McEwen BS. Sleep deprivation as a neurobiologic and physiologic stressor: Allostasis and allostatic load. Metabolism . 2006;55:S20–23. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Samkoff JS, Jacques CH. A review of studies concerning effects of sleep deprivation and fatigue on residents' performance. Acad. Med . 1991;66:687–93. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199111000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Blissitt P A. Sleep memory and learning. J. Neurosci. Nurs. 2001;33:208–15. doi: 10.1097/01376517-200108000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Peigneux P, Laureys S, Delbeuck X, Maquet P. Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory systems. Neuroreport . 2001;1218:A111–24. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112210-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.McDermott CM, LaHoste GJ, Chen C, Musto A, Bazan NG, Magee JC. sleep deprivation causes behavioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:9687–95. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-29-09687.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kim EY, Mahmoud GS, Grover LM. REM sleep deprivation inhibits LTP in vivo in area CA1 of rat hippocampus. Neurosci. Lett . 2005;388:163–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kopp C, Longordo F, Nicholson JR, Luthi A. Insufficient sleep reversibly alters bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12456–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2702-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.McDermott CM, Hardy M N, Bazan NG, Magee JC. Sleep deprivation-induced alterations in excitatory synaptic transmission in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus. J. Physiol . 2006;570(Pt 3 ):553–65. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.093781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vecsey CG, Baillie GS, Jaganath D, Havekes R, Daniels A, Wimmer M, Huang T, Brown KM, Li XY, Descalzi G, Kim SS, Chen T, Shang YZ, Zhuo M, Houslay MD, Abel T. Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;461: 1122–25. doi: 10.1038/nature08488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Smith C, Rose GM. Evidence for a paradoxical sleep window for place learning in the Morris water maze. Physiol. Behav. 1996;59:93–97. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02054-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Romcy-Pereira R, Pavlides C. Distinct modulatory effects of sleep on the maintenance of hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex LTP. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;20:3453–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03808.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Poe GR, Walsh CM, Bjorness TE. Cognitive neuroscience of sleep. Prog. Brain Res. 2010;185:1–19. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00001-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gais S, Albouy G, Boly M, Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, Desseilles M, Rauchs G, Schabus M, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Maquet P, Peigneux P. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:18778–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705454104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cai DJ, Shuman T, Gorman MR, Sage JR, Anagnostaras SG. Sleep selectively enhances hippocampus-dependent memory in mice. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;123:713–19. doi: 10.1037/a0016415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature . 2005;437:1272–78. doi: 10.1038/nature04286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6:236–49. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.6.3.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Guzman-Marin R, Ying Z, Suntsova N, Methippara M, Bashir T, Szymusiak R, Gomez-Pinilla F, McGinty D. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J. Physiol . 2006;575:807–19. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, Thomas ML, Sing HC, Redmond DP, Russo MB, Balkin TJ. The effects of sleep deprivation on information-integration categorization performance. Sleep . 2009;32:1439–48. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Guan Z, Peng X, Fang J. sleep deprivation impairs spatial memory and decreases extracellular signal-regulated kinase phosphorylation in the hippocampus. Brain Res. 2004;1018:38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Graves LA, Heller EA, Pack AI, Abel T. Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolidation for contextual fear conditioning. Learn. Mem . 2003;10:16876. doi: 10.1101/lm.48803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Belenky G, Wesensten N J, Thorne D R, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J. Sleep Res. 2003;12:1–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Suchecki D, Tiba PA, Machad RB. REM Sleep Rebound as an Adaptive Response to Stressful Situations. Front. Neurol . 2012;3:41. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Vgontzas AN, Tsigos C, Bixler EO, Stratakis CA, Zachman K, Kales A, Vela-Bueno A, Chrousos GP. Chronic insomnia and activity of the stress system a preliminary study. J. Psychosom. Res. 1998;45:21–31. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00302-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Vgontzas A N, Bixler EO, Chrousos GP. Obesity-related sleepiness and fatigue the role of the stress system and cytokines. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2006;1083:329–44. doi: 10.1196/annals.1367.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Stefurak SJ, Stefurak ML, Mendelson WB, Gillin JC, Wyatt RJ. A method for sleep depriving rats. Pharmacol. Bochem Behav. 1977;6:137–39. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(77)90169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Campbell IG, Guinan MJ, Horowitz JM. sleep deprivation impairs long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. J. Neurophysiol . 2002;88:1073–76. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Levitt RA. Sleep deprivation in the rat. Science . 1966;153:85–87. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3731.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Guzman-Marin R, Suntsova N, Stewart DR, Gong H, Szymusiak R, McGinty D. sleep deprivation reduces proliferation of cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in rats. J. Physiol . 2003;549:563–71. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.041665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Webb W B, Agnew HW., Jr Sleep deprivation, age, and exhaustion time in the rat. Science . 1962;136:1122. doi: 10.1126/science.136.3522.1122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.O'Callaghan RM, Ohle R, Kelly AM. The effects of forced exercise on hippocampal plasticity in the rat, A comparison of LTP, spatial- and non-spatial learning. Behav. Brain Res . 2007;176:362–66. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Tobler I, Scherschlicht R. Sleep and EEG slow-wave activity in the domestic cat, effect of sleep deprivation. Behav. Brain Res. 1990;37:109–18. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90086-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Tobler I, Franken P, Scherschlicht R. Sleep and EEG spectra in the rabbit under baseline conditions and following sleep deprivation. Physiol. Behav. 1990;48:121–29. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90272-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Franken P, Tobler I, Borbely AA. Effects of 12-h sleep deprivation and of 12-h cold exposure on sleep regulation and cortical temperature in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1993;54:885–94. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90297-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ocampo-Garces A, Molina E, Rodriguez A, Vivaldi EA. Homeostasis of REM sleep after total and selective sleep deprivation in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;84:2699–702. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.5.2699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vyazovskiy VV, Borbely AA, Tobler I. Interhemispheric sleep EEG asymmetry in the rat is enhanced by sleep deprivation. J. Neurophysiol . 2002;88:2280–86. doi: 10.1152/jn.00304.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Modirrousta M, Mainville L, Jones BE. Orexin and MCH neurons express c-Fos differently after sleep deprivation vs. recovery and bear different adrenergic receptors. Eur. J. Neu. 2005;21:2807–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Li S, Tian Y, Ding Y, Jin X, Yan C, Shen X. The effects of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation and recovery on spatial reference memory of young rats. Learn Behav . 2009;37:246–53. doi: 10.3758/LB.37.3.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kopp C, Longordo F, Luthi A. Experience-dependent changes in NMDA receptor composition at mature central synapses. Neuropharmacology . 2007;53:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Datta S, Mavanji V, Ulloor J, Patterson EH. Activation of phasic pontine-wave generator prevents rapid eye movement sleep deprivation-induced learning impairment in the rat, a mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci . 2004;24:1416–27. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4111-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Grahnstedt S, Ursin R. Platform sleep deprivation affects deep slow wave sleep in addition to REM sleep. Behav. Brain Res. 1985;18:233–39. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90031-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Machado RB, Hipolide DC, Benedito-Silva AA, Tufik S. sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique, quantification of sleep loss and recovery. Brain Res . 2004;1004:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jouvet D, Vimont P, Delorme F, Jouvet M. Study of Selective Deprivation of the Paradoxal Sleep Phase in the Cat. C R Seances Soc Biol. Fil . 1964;158:756–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Suchecki D, Lobo LL, Hipolide DC, Tufik S. Increased ACTH and corticosterone secretion induced by different methods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 1998;7:276–81. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Coenen A M, Van Hulzen Z J. Paradoxical sleep deprivation in animal studies, some methodological considerations. Prog. Brain Res . 1980;53:325–330. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60072-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.van Hulzen ZJ, Coenen AM. Paradoxical sleep deprivation and locomotor activity in rats. Physiol. Behav. 1981;27:741–44. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90250-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Suchecki D, Tufik S. Social stability attenuates the stress in the modified multiple platform method for paradoxical sleep deprivation in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 2000;68:309–16. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM. Sleep deprivation in the rat, an update of the 1989 paper. Sleep . 2002;25:18–24. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Medeiros R, Lenneberg-Hoshino C, Hoshino K, Tufik S. Neuroethologic differences in sleep deprivation induced by the single- and multiple-platform methods. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 1998;31:675–80. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x1998000500012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Gulyani S, Mallick BN. Effect of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation on rat brain Na-K ATPase activity. J. Sleep Res. 1993;2:45–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1993.tb00060.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bentivoglio M, Grassi-Zucconi G. The pioneering experimental studies on sleep deprivation. Sleep . 1997;20:570–76. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.7.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Bast B. Studies in experimental exhaustion due to lack of sleep. IV. Effect on the nerve cells in the medulla. Am. J. Physiol . 1927;82:140–46. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Dement W, Henry P, Cohen H, Ferguson J. Studies on the effect of REM deprivation in humans and in animals. Res. Publ. Assoc. Res. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1967;45:456–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Dement W. The effect of dream deprivation. Science . 1960;131:1705. doi: 10.1126/science.131.3415.1705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Cohen HB, Duncan RF, 2nd, Dement WC. Sleep, the effect of electroconvulsive shock in cats deprived of REM sleep. Science. 1967;156:1646–48. doi: 10.1126/science.156.3782.1646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Penev PD. Sleep deprivation and energy metabolism to sleep, perchance to eat? Curr. Opin. Endocrinol. Diabetes Obes . 2007;14:374–81. doi: 10.1097/MED.0b013e3282be9093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, Holcomb H, Mayberg H D, Annals R, Wagner H, Thorne D, Popp K, Rowland L, Welsh A, Balwinski S, Redmond D. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J. Sleep Res. 2000;9:335–52. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.West LJ, Janszen HH, Lester BK, Cornelisoon FS., Jr The psychosis of sleep deprivation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1962;96:66–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1962.tb50101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Kollar EJ, Slater GR, Palmer JO, Doctor RF, Mandell AJ. Stress in subjects undergoing sleep deprivation. Psychosom. Med. 1966;28:101–13. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196603000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Caldwell JA, Prazinko B, Caldwell JL. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol . 2003;114(1 ):23–31. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00283-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Nicholson AN, Stone BM. Influence of back angle on the quality of sleep in seats. Ergonomics . 1987;30:1033–41. doi: 10.1080/00140138708965993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Dinges DF. An overview of sleepiness and accidents. J. Sleep Res. 1995;4(S2 ):4–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Effects of total sleep loss on sleep tendency. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1979;48:495–506. doi: 10.2466/pms.1979.48.2.495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Horne JA. Sleep loss and "divergent" thinking ability. Sleep. 1988;11(6 ):528–36. doi: 10.1093/sleep/11.6.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Gillberg M, Akerstedt T. Sleep loss and performance no "safe" duration of a monotonous task. Physiol. Behav. 1998;64:599–604. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00063-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Thomas ML, Sing HC, Belenky G, Holcomb HH, Mayberg HS, Dannals RF, Wagner HN Jr, Thorne DR, Popp KA, Rowland LM, Welsh AB, Balwinski SM, Redmond DP. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. II. Effects of 48 and 72 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. Thalamus Related Syst . 2003;2:199–229. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Drummond SP, Gillin JC, Brown GG. Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 2001;10:85–92. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00245.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Jin SH, Na SH, Kim SY, Kim DJ. Effects of total sleep-deprivation on waking human EEG: functional cluster analysis. Clin. Neurophysiol . 2004;115:2825–33. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Szelenberger W, Piotrowski T, Dabrowska AJ. Increased prefrontal event-related current density after sleep deprivation. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. Wars . 2005;65:19–28. doi: 10.55782/ane-2005-1536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Yang RH, Hu SJ, Wang Y, Zhang WB, Luo WJ, Chen JY. Paradoxical sleep deprivation impairs spatial learning and affects membrane excitability and mitochondrial protein in the hippocampus. Brain Res. 2008;1230:224–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Zagaar M, Alhaider I, Dao A, Levine A, Alkarawi A, Alzubaidy M, Alkadhi K. The beneficial effects of regular exercise on cognition in REM sleep deprivation Behavioral, electrophysiological and molecular evidence. Neurobiol. Dis. 2012;4:1153–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Hagewoud R, Havekes R, Novati A, Keijser JN, Van de Zee EA, Meerlo P. sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and reduces hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation. J. Sleep Res. 2010;19:280–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00799.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Youngblood BD, Smagin GN, Elkins PD, Ryan DH, Harris RB. The effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation and valine on spatial learning and brain 5-HT metabolism. Physiol. Behav. 1999;67:643–49. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00120-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Ruskin DN, Liu C, Dunn KE, Bazan NG, LaHoste GJ. Sleep deprivation impairs hippocampus-mediated contextual learning but not amygdala-mediated cued learning in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;19:3121–24. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03426.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Yoo S S, Hu P T, Gujar N, Jolesz F A, Walker M P. A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat. Neurosci . 2007;10:385–392. doi: 10.1038/nn1851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Yang RH, Hu SJ, Wang Y, Zhang WB, Luo WJ, Chen JY. Paradoxical sleep deprivation impairs spatial learning and affects membrane excitability and mitochondrial protein in the hippocampus. Brain Res . 2008;1230:224–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Mograss MA, Guillem F, Brazzini-Poisson V, Godbout R. The effects of total sleep deprivation on recognition memory processes, a study of event-related potential. Neurobiol. Learn Mem . 2009;91:343–52. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Ramanathan L, Hu S, Frautschy SA, Siegel JM. Short-term total sleep deprivation in the rat increases antioxidant responses in multiple brain regions without impairing spontaneous alternation behavior. Behav. Brain Res . 2009;207:305–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Wang GP, Huang LQ, Wu HJ, Zhang L, You ZD, Zhao ZX. Calcineurin contributes to spatial memory impairment induced by rapid eye movement sleep deprivation. Neuroreport . 2009;20:1172–76. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832f0772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Alhaider IA, Aleis AM, Tran TT, Alkadhi KA. Caffeine prevents sleep loss-induced deficits in long-term potentiation and related signaling molecules in the dentate gyrus. Eur. J. Neurosci . 2010a;31:1368–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07175.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Aleisa AM, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA. Post-learning REM sleep deprivation impairs long-term memory Reversal by acute nicotine treatment. Neurosci. Lett . 2011a;499:28–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Samkoff JS, Jacques CH. A review of studies concerning effects of sleep deprivation and fatigue on residents' performance. Acad. Med . 1991;66:687–93. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199111000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Blissitt P A. Sleep, memory, and learning. J. Neurosci. Nurs . 2001;33:208–15. doi: 10.1097/01376517-200108000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Drake CL, Roehrs T, Roth T. Insomnia causes, consequences, and therapeutics an overview. Depress Anxiety . 2003;184:163–176. doi: 10.1002/da.10151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Sagaspe P, Sanchez-Ortuno M, Charles A. Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain Cogn . 2005;60:76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Uhde TW. Anxiety disorders. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Demet WC, editors. Sleep medicine. Philadelphia, W.B: Saunders Company; 1994. pp. 1123–1139. [Google Scholar]; American Psychiatry Association. 4th. Washington D.C: APA; Diagnostic statistical manual of mental disorders; pp. 1123–1139. [Google Scholar]
- 115.Wyatt RJ, Fram DH, Kupfer DJ, Snyder F. Total prolonged drug-induced REM sleep suppression in anxious-depressed patients . Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1971;24:145–55. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750080049007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Peeke SC, Callaway E, Jones RT, Stone GC, Doyle J. Combined effects of alcohol and sleep deprivation in normal young adults. Psychopharmacology. 1980;67:279–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00431270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams D, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbances, and psychomotor vigillance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997;20:267–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Roy-Byrne PP, Uhde TW, Post RM. Effects of one night’s sleep deprivation on mood and behavior in panic disorder. Patients with panic disorder compared with depressed patients and normal controls. Arch. Gen.Psychiatry . 1986;43:895–99. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800090085011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen. Psychiatry . 2005;62(6 ):617–27. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Kessler RC, Greenberg PE. The economic burden of anxiety and stress disorders. In: Davis KL, Charney DS, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology. The Fifth Generation of Progress. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2002. pp. 981–92. [Google Scholar]
- 121.Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, Andreski P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders, A longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biol. Psychiatry . 1996;39:411–18. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Harvey AG, Hairston IS, Gruber J, Gershon A. Anxiety and sleep. In: Antony MA, Stein MB, editors. The handbook of anxiety and anxiety disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Papadimitriou GN, Linkowski P. Sleep disturbance in anxiety disorders. Intk. Rev Psychiatry . 2005;17:229–36. doi: 10.1080/09540260500104524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kumar A, Singh A, Kumar P. Possible involvement of GABAergic mechanism in protective effect of melatonin against sleep deprivation-induced behavior modification and oxidative damage in mice. Ind. J. Exp. Biol . 2011;49:211–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Kumar A, Garg R. Possible role of trazodone and imipramine in sleep deprivation-induced anxiety-like behavior and oxidative damage in mice. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol . 2009;3:383–87. doi: 10.1358/mf.2009.31.6.1386992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Kumar A, Singh A. Possible involvement of GABAergic mechanism in protective effect of melatonin against sleep deprivation-induced behaviour modification and oxidative damage in mice. Fundam. Clin. Pharmacol . 2009;23:439–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1472-8206.2009.00737.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Xu A, Sakurai E, Kuramasu A, Zhang J, Li J, Okamura N, Zhang D, Yoshikawa T, Watanabe T, Yanai K. Roles of hypothalamic subgroup histamine and orexin neurons on behavioral responses to sleep deprivation induced by the treadmill method in adolescent rats. J. Pharmacol. Sci . 2010;1144:444–53. doi: 10.1254/jphs.10177fp. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Pokk P, Zharkovsky A. The effects of drugs acting at GABA-benzodiazepine-barbiturate receptor complex on the behavior of sleep deprived mice. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 1995;76:23–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0773.1995.tb00097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Pokk P, Zharkovsky A. Small platform stress attenuates the anxiogenic effect of diazepam withdrawal in the plus-maze test. Behav. Brain Res . 1998;97:153–57. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00036-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Pokk P, Zharkovsky A. The effects of flumazenil, RO 15- 4513 and β-CCM on the behaviour of control and stressed mice in the plus-maze test. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 1997;48:253–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Pokk P, Liljequist S, Zharkovsky A. Ro 15-4513 potentiates, instead of antagonizes, ethanol-induced sleep in mice exposed to small platform stress. Eur. J. Pharmacol . 1996;317:15–20. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(96)90061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Suchecki D, Tibamm P A, Tufik S. Hormonal and behavioral responses of paradoxical sleep-deprived rats to the elevated plus maze. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14:549–54. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2002.00812.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Gessa GL, Pani L, Fadda P, Fratta W. Sleep deprivation in the rat an animal model of mania. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol . 1995;5:89–93. doi: 10.1016/0924-977x(95)00023-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Silva RH, Kameda SR, Carvalho RC. Anxiogenic effect of sleep deprivation in the elevated plus-maze test in mice. Psychopharmacology . 2004;176:115–22. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1873-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Pokk P, Väli M. Small platform stress increases exploratory activity of mice in staircase test. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry . 2001;25:1435–44. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(01)00195-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Martinez-Gonzalez D, Obermeyer W, Fahy JL, Riboh M, Kalin NH, Benca RM. REM sleep deprivation induces changes in coping responses that are not reversed by amphetamine. Sleep . 2004;27:609–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Mogilnicka E, Boissard CG, Hunn C, Delini-Stula A. Suppressant effect of REM sleep deprivation on neophobia in normal rats and in rats with selective DSP-4 induced damage of locus coeruleus neurons. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav . 1985;23:93–97. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(85)90136-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Meerlo P, Mistlberger R, Jacobs B, Heller H, McGinty D. New neurons in the adult brain, the role of sleep and consequences of sleep loss. Sleep Med. Rev . 2009;13:187–94. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Vollert C, Zagaar M, Hovatta I, Taneja M, Vu A, Dao A, Levine A, Alkadhi K, Salim S. Exercise prevents sleep deprivation-associated anxiety-like behavior in rats, Potential role of oxidative stress mechanisms. Behav. Brain Res . 2011;224:233–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Gopalakrishnan A, Ji LL, Cirelli C. Sleep deprivation and cellular responses to oxidative stress. Sleep . 2004;27:27–35. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Kumar A, Kalonia H. Protective effect of Withania somnifera Dual on the behavioral and biochemical alterations in sleep disturbed mice grid over water suspended method. Ind. J. Exp. Biol. 200;45:524–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Reimund E. The free radical theory of sleep. Med. Hypotheses . 1994;43:231–33. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(94)90071-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Ikeda M, Ikeda-Sagara M, Okada T. Brain oxidation is an initial process in sleep induction. Neuroscience . 2005;130:1029–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Honda K, Kamoda Y, Inoue S. Oxidized glutathione regulates physiological sleep in unrestrained rats. Brain Res . 1994;636:253–58. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91024-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Horne JA. A review of the biological effects of total sleep deprivation in man. Biol. Psychol. 1978;7:55–102. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(78)90042-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Taylor AH. Physical activity, anxiety, and stress. In: Biddle SJH, Fox KR, Boutcher CH, editors. Physical activity and psychological well-being. New York: Routledge; 2000. pp. 10–45. [Google Scholar]
- 147.Salmon P. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress, a unifying theory. Clin. Psychol. Rev . 2001;21:33–61. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00032-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Fasting K, Gronningsaeter H. Unemployment, trait-anxiety and physical exercise. Scandinavian J. Sports Sci . 1986;8:99–103. [Google Scholar]
- 149.Roth DL, Holmes DS. Influence of aerobic exercise training and relaxation on physical and psychologic health following stressful life events. Psychosomatic Med. 1987;49:355–65. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198707000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Simons CW, Birkimer JC. An exploration of factors predicting the effects of aerobic conditioning on mood state. J. Psychosomatic Res . 1988;32:63–75. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(88)90089-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Williams P, Lord SR. Effects of group exercise on cognitive functioning and mood in older women. Australian and New Zealand J. Public Health . 1997;21:45–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.1997.tb01653.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moa l M. Adult neurogenesis from precursors to network and physiology. Physiol. Rev . 2005;85:523–69. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00055.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Ming GL, Song H. Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian central nervous system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;28:223–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.051804.101459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Lledo PM, Alonso M, Grubb MS. Adult neurogenesis and functional plasticity in neuronal circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:179–93. doi: 10.1038/nrn1867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Kaplan MS, Hinds JW. Neurogenesis in the adult rat, electron microscopic analysis of light radioautographs. Science . 1977;197:1092–94. doi: 10.1126/science.887941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Bjork-Eriksson T, Alborn AM, Nordborg C, Peterson DA, Gage H. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat. Med. 1998;4:1313–17. doi: 10.1038/3305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Kornack DR, Rakic P. Continuation of neurogenesis in the hippocampus of the adult macaque monkey. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:5768–73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.10.5768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Becker S, Wojtowicz JM. A model of hippocampal neurogenesis in memory and mood disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Sahay A, Hen R. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1110–15. doi: 10.1038/nn1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Samuels BA, Hen R. Neurogenesis and affective disorders. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;33:1152–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Winner B, Kohl Z, Gage FH. Neurodegenerative disease and adult neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci . 2011;33:1139–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07613.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage FH. More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature . 1997;386:493–95. doi: 10.1038/386493a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci . 1999;2:266–70. doi: 10.1038/6368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.van Praag H, Schinder AF, Christie BR, Toni N, Palmer TD, Gage FH. Functional neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Nature . 2002;415:1030–34. doi: 10.1038/4151030a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Nakashiba T, Cushman JD, Pelkey KA, Renaudineau S, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ, Rodriguez B V, Chittajallu R, Iwamoto KS, McBain CJ, Fanselow MS, Tonegawa S. Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell . 2012;149(1 ):188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Hairston IS, Little MT, Scanlon MD, Barakat MT, Palmer TD, Sapolsky RM, Heller HC. Sleep restriction suppresses neurogenesis induced by hippocampus-dependent learning. J. Neurophysiol . 2005;94:4224–33. doi: 10.1152/jn.00218.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Lucassen PJ, Meerlo P, Naylor AS, van Dam AM, Dayer AG, Fuchs E, Oomen CA, Czéh B. Regulation of adult neurogenesis by stress, sleep disruption, exercise and inflammation, implications for depression and antidepressant action. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol . 2010;20:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Guzman-Marin R, Suntsova N, Methippara M, Greiffenstein R, Szymusiak R, McGinty D. Sleep deprivation suppresses neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus of rats. Eur. J. Neurosci . 2005;22:2111–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04376.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Kitabatake Y, Sailor KA, Ming GL, Song H. Adult neurogenesis and hippocampal memory function, new cells, more plasticity new memories? Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am . 2007;18:105–13. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2006.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Mueller A, Pollock MS, Lieblich SE, Epp JR, Galea LA, Mistlberger RE. sleep deprivation can inhibit adult hippocampal neurogenesis independent of adrenal stress hormones. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2008;294:R1693–R1703. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00858.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Tung A, Takase L, Fornal C, Jacobs B. Effects of sleep deprivation and recovery sleep upon cell proliferation in adult rat dentate gyrus. Neuroscience . 2005;134:721–723. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Roman V, VanderBorght K, Leemburg SA, VanderZee EA, Meerlo P. Sleep restriction by forced activity reduces hippocampal cell proliferation. Brain. Res. 2005;1065:53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Roman V, Walstra I, Luiten PG, Meerlo P. Too little sleep gradually desensitizes the serotonin 1A receptor system. Sleep . 2005b;28:1505–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Mirescu C, Gould E. Stress and adult neurogenesis. Hippocampus. 006a;16:233–38. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Mirescu C, Peters JD, Noiman L, Gould E. sleep deprivation inhibits adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus by elevating glucocorticoids. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA . 2006b;103:19170–75. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608644103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.van der Borght K, Ferrari F, Klauke K, Roman V, Havekes R, Sgoifo A, van der Zee EA, Meerlo P. Hippocampal cell proliferation across the day, increase by running wheel activity, but no effect of sleep and wakefulness. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;167:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Grassi Z G, Cipriani S, Balgkouranidou I, Scattoni R. “One night” sleep deprivation stimulates hippocampal neurogenesis. Brain Res. Bull . 2006;69:375–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Junek A, Rusak B, Semb K. Short-term sleep deprivation may alter the dynamics of hippocampal cell proliferation in adult rats. Neuroscience . 2010;170:1140–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Bain MJ, Dwyer SM, Rusak B. Restraint stress affects hippocampal cell proliferation differently in rats and mice. Neurosci. Lett . 2004;368:7–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.04.096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Wakamatsu H, Yoshinobu Y, Aida R, Moriya T, Akiyama M, Shibata S. Restricted-feeding-induced anticipatory activity rhythm is associated with a phase-shift of the expression of mPer1 and mPer2 mRNA in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus but not in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of mice. Eur. J. Neurosci . 2001;13:119096. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01483.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Amir S, Harbour VL, Robinson B. Pinealectomy does not affect diurnal PER2 expression in the rat limbic forebrain. Neurosci. Lett. 2006;399:147–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Jilg A, Lesny S, Peruzki N, Schwegler H, Selbach O, Dehghani F, Stehle JH. Temporal dynamics of mouse hippocampal clock gene expression support memory processing. Hippocampus . 2010;20:377–88. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Kang H, Schuman EM. Long-lasting neurotrophin-induced enhancement of synaptic transmission in the adult hippocampus. Science . 1995;267:1658–62. doi: 10.1126/science.7886457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Crews K, Nixon D, Kim J, Joseph B, Shukitt-Hale L, QinZou J. BHT blocks NF-kappaB activation and ethanol-induced brain damage. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2006;30:1938–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00239.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Nixon K, Crews FT. Binge ethanol exposure decreases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. J. Neurochem . 2002;83:1087–93. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.01214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Herrera DG, Yague AG, Johnsen-Soriano S, Bosch-Morell F, Collado-Morente L, Muriach M, Romero FJ, Garcia-Verdugo JM. Selective impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis by chronic alcoholism, protective effects of an antioxidant. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:7919–24. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1230907100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Salim S, Sarraj N, Taneja M, Saha K, Tejada-Simon MV, Chugh C. Moderate treadmill exercise prevents oxidative stress-induced anxiety-like behavior in rats. Behav. Brain Res . 2010;208:545–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, Lotsikas A, Zachman K, Kales A, Prolo P, Wong ML, Licinio J, Gold PW, Hermida RC, Mastorakos G, Chrousos GP. Circadian interleukin-6 secretion and quantity and depth of sleep. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1999;84:2603–7. doi: 10.1210/jcem.84.8.5894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Rock RB, Gekker G, Hu S, Sheng WS, Cheeran M, Lokensgard JR, Peterson PK. Role of Microglia in Central Nervous System Infections. Clin. Microbiol. Rev. 200;17:942–64. doi: 10.1128/CMR.17.4.942-964.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Behrens MM, Ali SS, Dugan LL. Interleukin-6 mediates the increase in NADPH-oxidase in the ketamine model of schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:13957–66. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4457-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Brown GC. Mechanisms of inflammatory neurodegeneration, iNOS and NADPH oxidase. Biochem. Soc. Trans . 2007;35:1119–21. doi: 10.1042/BST0351119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Voloboueva LA, Giffard RG. Inflammation, Mitochondria, and the Inhibition of Adult Neurogenesis. J. Neurosci. Res . 2011;89:1989–96. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Van Cauter E, Spiegel K, Tasali E, Leproult R. Metabolic consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med . 2008;9(Suppl 1 ):S23–8. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70013-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Mullington JM, Simpson NS, Meier-Ewert HK, Haack M. Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2010;24:775–84. doi: 10.1016/j.beem.2010.08.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Opp MR. Cytokines and sleep. Sleep Med. Rev . 2005;9:355–64. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Motivala SJ, Irwin MR. Sleep and immunity, cytokine pathways linking sleep and health outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16:21–26. [Google Scholar]
- 197.Mortensen RF. C-reactive protein, inflammation, and innate immunity. Immunol. Res. 2001;24:163–76. doi: 10.1385/IR:24:2:163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Zimmerman MA, Selzman CH, Cothren C, Sorensen AC, Raeburn CD, Harken AH. Diagnostic implications of C-reactive protein. Arch. Surg . 2003;138:220–24. doi: 10.1001/archsurg.138.2.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N, Price N, Dinge DF, Mullington JM. Absence of diurnal variation of C-reactive protein concentrations in healthy human subjects. Clin. Chem. 2001;47:426–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N. Effect of sleep loss on C-reactive protein, an inflammatory marker of cardiovascular risk. J Am. Coll. Cardiol . 2004;43:678–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2003.07.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Rief W, Mills PJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Ziegler MG, Pung MA, Dimsdale JE. Overnight changes of immune parameters and catecholamines are associated with mood and stress. Psychosom. Med . 2010;72(8 ):755–62. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181f367e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Salim S, Asghar M, Taneja M, Hovatta I, Chugh G, Vollert C, Vu A. Potential Contribution of Oxidative Stress and Inflammation to Anxiety and Hypertension. Brain Res . 2011;1404:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory, long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature . 1993;361(6407 ):31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Smith CT, Conway JM, Rose GM. Brief paradoxical sleep deprivation impairs reference, but not working, memory in the radial arm maze task. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 1998;69:211–17. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Davis CJ, Harding JW, Wright JW. REM sleep deprivation-induced deficits in the latency-to-peak induction and maintenance of long-term potentiation within the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Brain Res . 2003;973:293–97. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)02508-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Tiba A, Oliveira MG, Rossi VC, Tufik S, Suchecki D. Glucocorticoids are not responsible for paradoxical sleep deprivation-induced memory impairments. Sleep . 2008;31:505–15. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.4.505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Ruskin DN, Dunn KE, Billiot I, Bazan NG, LaHoste GJ. Eliminating the adrenal stress response does not affect sleep deprivation-induced acquisition deficits in the water maze. Life Sci . 2006;78:2833–38. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Alhaider IA, Aleisa AM, Tran TT, Alkadhi KA. Sleep deprivation prevents stimulation-induced increases of levels of P-CREB and BDNF: protection by caffeine. Mol. Cell. Neurosci . 2010b;46:742–51. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2011.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Alhaider IA, Aleisa AM, Tran TT, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA. Chronic caffeine treatment prevents sleep deprivation-induced impairment of cognitive function and synaptic plasticity. Sleep . 2011;33:437–444. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.4.437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Aleisa AM, Helal G, Alhaider IA, Alzoubi KH, Srivareerat M, Tran TT, Al-Rejaie SS, Alkadhi KA. Acute nicotine treatment prevents REM sleep deprivation-induced learning and memory impairment in rat. Hippocampus. 2011b;21:899–909. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Yang Z, Zhang W, Wang M, Ruan D, Chen J. Effects of daytime, night and sleep pressure on long-term depression in the hippocampus in vivo. Neurosci. Lett. 2012;5112:106–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Tadavarty R, Rajput PS, Wong JM, Kumar U, Sastry BR. Sleep-deprivation induces changes in GABAB and mGlu receptor expression and has consequences for synaptic long-term depression. PLoS One . 2011;6:6 e24933. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Tadavarty R, Kaan TK, Sastry BR. Long-term depression of excitatory synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal CA1 neurons following sleep-deprivation. Exp. Neurol . 2009;216:239–42. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 214.Tartar JL, Ward CP, McKenna JT, et al. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;23:2739–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04808.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Marks CA, Wayner MJ. Effects of sleep disruption on rat dentate granule cell LTP in vivo. Brain Res Bull. 2005;66:114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 216.Ishikawa A, Kanayama Y, Matsumura H, Tsuchimochi H, Ishida Y, Nakamura S. Selective rapid eye movement sleep deprivation impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Eur. J. Neurosci . 2006;24:243–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 217.Park HJ, Kang WS, Paik JW, Kim JW. Effect of Valproic Acid Through Regulation of NMDA Receptor-ERK Signaling in Sleep Deprivation Rats. J. Mol. Neurosci. 2011 Nov 19; doi: 10.1007/s12031-011-9673-5. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 218.Novati A, Hulshof HJ, Granic I, Meerlo P. Chronic partial sleep deprivation reduces brain sensitivity to glutamateN-methyl-d-aspartate receptor-mediated neurotoxicity. J. Sleep Res. 2012;21:3–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00932.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 219.Hagewoud R, Havekes R, Novati A, Keijser JN, Van der Zee EA, Meerlo P. Sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and reduces hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation. J. Sleep Res. 2010;19:280–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00799.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 220.Chen C, Hardy M, Zhang J, LaHoste GJ, Bazan NG. Altered NMDA receptor trafficking contributes to sleep deprivation-induced hippocampal synaptic and cognitive impairments. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2006;340:435–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2005.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 221.Ravassard P, Pachoud B, Comte JC, et al. Paradoxical REM sleep deprivation causes a large and rapidly reversible decrease in long-term potentiation, synaptic transmission, glutamate receptor protein levels, and ERK/MAPK activation in the dorsal hippocampus. Sleep. 2009;32:227–40. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.2.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 222.Vecsey CG, Baillie GS, Jaganath D, Havekes R, Daniels A, Wimmer M, Huang T, Brown KM, Li XY, Descalzi G, Kim SS, Chen T, Shang YZ, Zhuo M, Houslay MD, Abel T. Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature . 2009;461:1122–25. doi: 10.1038/nature08488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 223.Gerges NZ, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA. Role of phosphorylated CaMKII and calcineurin in the differential effect of hypothyroidism on LTP of CA1 and dentate gyrus. Hippocampus . 2005;15:480–490. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 224.Aleisa AM, Alzoubi KH, Gerges NZ, Alkadhi KA. Nicotine blocks stress-induced impairment of spatial memory and LTP of the CA1 region. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol . 2006;9:417–26. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 225.Aleisa AM, Alzoubi KH, Gerges NZ, Alkadhi KA. Chronic psychosocial stress-induced impairment of hippocampal LTP: possible role of BDNF. Neurobiol. Dis . 2006b;22:453–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2005.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 226.Srivareerat M, Tran TT, Alzoubi KH, Alkadhi KA. Chronic psychosocial stress exacerbates impairment of cognition and LTP in a Aβ rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry . 2009;65(11 ):918–926. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 227.Malenka RC, Kauer JA, Perkel DJ, Mauk MD, Kelly PT, Nicoll RA, Waxham MN. An essential role for postsynaptic calmodulin and protein kinase activity in long-term potentiation. Nature . 1989;340:554–57. doi: 10.1038/340554a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 228.Pettit DL, Perlman S, Malinow R. Potentiated transmission and prevention of further LTP by increased CaMKII activity in postsynaptic hippocampal slice neurons. Sci. N.Y. 1994;266:1881–1885. doi: 10.1126/science.7997883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 229.Lledo PM, Hjelmstad GO, Mukherji S, Soderling TR, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA. Calcium/calmodulin-dependent kinase II and long-term potentiation enhance synaptic transmission by the same mechanism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1995;92:11175–79. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.24.11175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 230.Giese KP, Fedorov NB, Filipkowski RK, Silva AJ. Autophosphorylation at Thr286 of the alpha calcium-calmodulin kinase II in LTP and learning. Science. 1998;279:870–73. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5352.870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 231.Mulkey RM, Herron CE, Malenka RC. An essential role for protein phosphatases in hippocampal long-term depression. Science . 1993;261:1051–55. doi: 10.1126/science.8394601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 232.Mulkey RM, Endo S, Shenolikar S, Malenka RC. Involvement of a calcineurin/inhibitor-1 phosphatase cascade in hippocampal long-term depression. Nature . 1994;369:486–88. doi: 10.1038/369486a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 233.Wang JH, Kelly PT. The balance between postsynaptic Ca2+-dependent protein kinase and phosphatase activities controlling synaptic strength. Learn. Mem. 1996;3:70–81. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.2-3.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 234.Winder DG, Mansuy IM, Osman M, Moallem TM, Kandel ER. Genetic and pharmacological evidence for a novel, intermediate phase of long-term potentiation suppressed by calcineurin. Cell. 1998;92:25–37. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80896-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 235.Linnarsson S, Bjorklund A, Ernfors P. Learning deficit in BDNF mutant mice. Eur. J. Neurosci . 1997;9:2581–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01687.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 236.Korte M, Carroll P, Wolf E, Brem G, Thoenen H, Bonhoeffer T. Hippocampal long-term potentiation is impaired in mice lacking brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1995;92:8856–60. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.19.8856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 237.Patterson SL, Abel T, Deuel TA, Martin KC, Rose JC, Kandel ER. Recombinant BDNF rescues deficits in basal synaptic transmission and hippocampal LTP in BDNF knockout mice. Neuron . 1996;16:1137–45. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80140-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 238.Mizuno Yamada K, Olariu A, Nawa H, Nabeshima T. Involvement of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in spatial memory formation and maintenance in a radial arm maze test in rats. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:7116–21. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-07116.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 239.Gooney M, Shaw K, Kelly A, O'Mara SM, Lynch MA. Long-term potentiation and spatial learning are associated with increased phosphorylation of TrkB and extracellular signal-regulated kinase ERK in the dentate gyrus, evidence for a role for brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Behav. Neurosci . 2002;116:455–63. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.3.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 240.Patterson S L, Grover LM, Schwartzkroin PA, Bothwell M. Neurotrophin expression in rat hippocampal slices, a stimulus paradigm inducing LTP in CA1 evokes increases in BDNF and NT-3 mRNAs. Neuron . 1992;9:1081–88. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90067-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 241.Castren E, Pitkanen M, Sirvio J, et al. The induction of LTP increases BDNF and NGF mRNA but decreases NT-3 mRNA in the dentate gyrus. Neuroreport . 1993;4:895–98. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199307000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 242.Dragunow M, Beilharz E, Mason B, Lawlor P, Abraham W, Gluckman P. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression after long-term potentiation. Neurosci. Lett . 1993;160:232–36. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90420-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 243.Springer JE, Gwag BJ, Sessler FM. Neurotrophic factor mRNA expression in dentate gyrus is increased following in vivo stimulation of the angular bundle. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 1994;23:135–143. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(94)90219-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 244.Morimoto K, Sato K, Sato S, Yamada N, Hayabara T. Time-dependent changes in neurotrophic factor mRNA expression after kindling and long-term potentiation in rats. Brain Res. Bul. 1998;45:599–605. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00459-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 245.Finkbeiner S, Tavazoie SF, Maloratsky A, Jacobs KM, Harris KM, Greenberg ME. CREB, a major mediator of neuronal neurotrophin responses. Neuron. 1997;19:1031–1047. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80395-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 246.Boulanger L, Poo MM. Gating of BDNF-induced synaptic potentiation by cAMP. Science . 1999;284:1982–1984. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5422.1982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 247.Blanquet PR, Lamour Y. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor increases Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase 2 activity in hippocampus. J. Biol. Chem. 1997;272:24133–36. doi: 10.1074/jbc.272.39.24133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 248.Minichiello L, Calella AM, Medina DL, Bonhoeffer T, Klein R, Korte M. Mechanism of TrkB-mediated hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuron . 2002;36:121–137. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00942-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 249.Yin JC, Wallach JS, Del Vecchio M, et al. Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell . 1994;79:49–58. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90399-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 250.Tully T. Regulation of gene expression and its role in long-term memory and synaptic plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1997;94:4239–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.9.4239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 251.Abel T, Kandel E. Positive and negative regulatory mechanisms that mediate long-term memory storage. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1998;26:360–378. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00050-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 252.Abel T, Martin KC, Bartsch D, Kandel ER. Memory suppressor genes inhibitory constraints on the storage of long-term memory. Science . 1998;279:338–341. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5349.338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 253.Yin JC, Del Vecchio M, Zhou H, Tully T. CREB as a memory modulator, induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell . 1995;81:107–115. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90375-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 254.Jensen KF, Ohmstede CA, Fisher RS, Sahyoun N. Nuclear and axonal localization of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase type Gr in rat cerebellar cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA . 991a;88:2850–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.7.2850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 255.Jensen KF, Ohmstede CA, Fisher RS, Olin JK, Sahyoun N. Acquisition and loss of a neuronal Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase during neuronal differentiation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA . 991b;88:4050–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.9.4050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 256.Bito H, Deisseroth K, Tsien RW. CREB phosphorylation and dephosphorylation a Ca2+- and stimulus duration-dependent switch for hippocampal gene expression. Cell . 1996;87:1203–14. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81816-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 257.Tokuda M, Ahmed BY, Lu YF, Matsui H, Miyamoto O, Yamaguchi F, Konishi R, Hatase O. Involvement of calmodulin-dependent protein kinases-I and -IV in long-term potentiation. Brain Res. 1997;755:162–66. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00189-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 258.Ho N, Liauw JA, Blaeser F, Wei F, Hanissian S, Muglia LM, Wozniak DF, Nardi A, Arvin KL, Holtzman DM, Linden DJ, Zhuo M, Muglia LJ, Chatila TA. Impaired synaptic plasticity and cAMP response element-binding protein activation in Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase type IV/Gr-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:6459–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06459.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 259.Kang H, Sun LD, Atkins CM, Soderling TR, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. An important role of neural activity-dependent CaMKIV signaling in the consolidation of long-term memory. Cell . 2001;106:771–83. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(01)00497-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 260.Srivareerat M, Tran TT, Salim S, Aleisa AM, Alkadhi KA. Chronic nicotine restores normal Aβ levels and prevents short-term memory and E-LTP impairment in aβ rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging . 2011;32:834–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 261.Mansvelder HD, McGehee DS. Long-term potentiation of excitatory inputs to brain reward areas by nicotine. Neuron . 2000;27:349–57. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00042-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 262.Ji D, Lape R, Dani JA. Timing and location of nicotinic activity enhances or depresses hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuron . 2001;31:131–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00332-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 263.Alkondon M, Braga MF, Pereira EF, Maelicke A, Albuquerque EX. alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors and modulation of gabaergic synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Eur. J. Pharmacol . 2000;393:59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(00)00006-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 264.Fujii S, Ji Z, Morita N, Sumikawa K. Acute and chronic nicotine exposure differentially facilitate the induction of LTP. Brain Res. 1999;846:137–43. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01982-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 265.French SJ, Humby T, Horner CH, Sofroniew MV, Rattray M. Hippocampal neurotrophin and trk receptor mRNA levels are altered by local administration of nicotine, carbachol and pilocarpine. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 1999;67:124–36. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(99)00048-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 266.Nehlig A, Daval JL, Debry G. Caffeine and the central nervous system, mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychostimulant effects. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1992;17:139–70. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(92)90012-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 267.Angelucci ME, Cesario C, Hiroi RH, Rosalen PL, Da Cunha C. Effects of caffeine on learning and memory in rats tested in the Morris water maze. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Rest . 2002;35:1201–08. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2002001000013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 268.Dall’Igna OP, Fett P, Gomes MW, Souza DO, Cunha RA, Lara DR. Caffeine and adenosine A2a receptor antagonists prevent beta-amyloid 25-35-induced cognitive deficits in mice. Exp. Neurol. 2007;203:241–45. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 269.Gevaerd MS, Takahashi RN, Silveira R, Da Cunha C. Caffeine reverses the memory disruption induced by intra-nigral MPTP-injection in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 2001;55:101–6. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00501-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 270.Prediger RD, Pamplona FA, Fernandes D, Takahashi RN. Caffeine improves spatial learning deficits in an animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ADHD--the spontaneously hypertensive rat SHR. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:583–94. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 271.Riedel WJ, Jolles J. Cognition enhancers in age-related cognitive decline. Drugs Aging . 1996;8:245–74. doi: 10.2165/00002512-199608040-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 272.Riedel W, Hogervorst E, Leboux R, Verhey F, van Praag H, Jolles J. Caffeine attenuates scopolamine-induced memory impairment in humans. Psychopharmacol. Berl. 1995;122:158–68. doi: 10.1007/BF02246090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 273.Fredholm BB. Astra Award Lecture. Adenosine, adenosine receptors and the actions of caffeine. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 1995;76:93–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0773.1995.tb00111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 274.de Mendonca A, Ribeiro JA. Endogenous adenosine modulates long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Neuroscience . 1994;62:385–90. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90373-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 275.de Mendonca A, Sebastiao AM, Ribeiro JA. Inhibition of NMDA receptor-mediated currents in isolated rat hippocampal neurons by adenosine A1 receptor activation. Neuroreport . 1995;6:1097–1100. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199505300-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 276.Dunwiddie TV, Masino SA. The role and regulation of adenosine in the central nervous system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci . 2001;24:31–55. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 277.Kalinchuk AV, McCarley RW, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Basheer R. The time course of adenosine, nitric oxide NO and inducible NO synthase changes in the brain with sleep loss and their role in the non-rapid eye movement sleep homeostatic cascade. Neurochemistry. 2011;116:260–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07100.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 278.Ding YH, Luan XD, Li J, Rafols JA, Guthinkonda M, Diaz FG, Ding Y. Exercise-induced overexpression of angiogenic factors and reduction of ischemia/reperfusion injury in stroke. Curr. Neurovasc. Res. 2004;1:411–20. doi: 10.2174/1567202043361875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 279.Sim YJ, Kim SS, Kim JY, Shin MS, Kim CJ. Treadmill exercise improves short-term memory by suppressing ischemia-induced apoptosis of neuronal cells in gerbils. Neurosci Lett. 2004;372:256–261. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 280.Toldy A, Atalay M, Stadler K, Sasvári M, Jakus J, Jung KJ, Chung HY, Nyakas C, Radák Z. The beneficial effects of nettle supplementation and exercise on brain lesion and memory in rat. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2009;20:974–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2008.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 281.Garcia-Capdevila S, Portell-Cortes I, Torras-Garcia M, Coll-Andreu M, Costa-Miserachs D. Effects of long-term voluntary exercise on learning and memory processes, dependency of the task and level of exercise. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;202:162–170. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 282.Nichol K E, Parachikova A I, Cotman C W. Three weeks of running wheel exposure improves cognitive performance in the aged Tg2576 mouse. Behav. Brain Res. 2007;184:124–132. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 283.Kim SE, Ko IG, Kim BK, Shin MS, Cho S, Kim CJ, Kim SH, Baek SS, Lee EK, Jee YS. Treadmill exercise prevents aging-induced failure of memory through an increase in neurogenesis and suppression of apoptosis in rat hippocampus. Exp. Gerontol . 2010;45:357–365. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 284.Radak Z, Toldy A, Szabo Z, Siamilis S, Nyakas C, Silye G, Jakus J, Goto S. The effects of training and detraining on memory, neurotrophins and oxidative stress markers in rat brain. Neurochem. Int . 2006;49:387–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 285.Radak Z, Chung HY, Koltai E, Taylor AW, Goto S. Exercise, oxidative stress and hormesis. Ageing Res. Rev. 2008;7:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2007.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 286.Akaneya Y, Tsumoto T, Kinoshita S, Hatanaka H. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor enhances long-term potentiation in rat visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:6707–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-17-06707.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 287.Gomez-Pinilla F, Zhuang Y, Feng J, Ying Z, Fan G. Exercise impacts brain-derivedneurotrophic factor plasticity by engaging mechanisms of epigenetic regulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010;33:383–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07508.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 288.Gerges NZ, Alkadhi KA. Hypothyroidism impairs late LTP in CA1 region but not in dentate gyrus of the intact rat hippocampus: MAPK involvement. Hippocampus. 2004;14:40–45. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 289.Gerges NZ, Stringer JL, Alkadhi KA. Combination of hypothyroidism and stress abolishes early LTP in the CA1 but not dentate gyrus of hippocampus of adult rats. Brain Res. 2001;922:250–26. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)03181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]