Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2013 Jul 4;3(7):e002863. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002863

Psychosocial and educational outcomes of weight faltering in infancy in ALSPAC

Amelia R Holme 1, Peter S Blair 1, Alan M Emond 1
PMCID: PMC3703578  PMID: 23833121

Abstract

Objectives

To investigate whether infants with weight faltering have impaired psychosocial and educational outcomes in later childhood.

Design

Follow-up of infants with weight faltering in a large UK cohort study.

Setting

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC).

Participants

11 534 term infants from ALSPAC with complete weight records. Weight gain (conditional on initial weight) was calculated for three periods: from birth to 8 weeks, 8 weeks to 9 months, and birth to 9 months. Cases of weight faltering were defined as those infants with a conditional weight gain below the 5th centile, and these were compared with the rest of the cohort as the control group.

Outcomes

Between 6 and 11 years, social, emotional and behavioural development was measured by direct assessment of the children and parental and teacher report. Educational outcomes included Standardised Assessment Test results at 7 and 11 years and Special Educational Needs status at age 11.

Results

Differences seen on univariate analysis in attention, non-verbal accuracy, educational attainment and special educational needs became non-significant after adjustment for confounding. Children with weight faltering in infancy did not differ from controls on any measures of self-esteem, peer relationships, experience of bullying, social cognition, antisocial activities, anxiety, depression or behavioural problems.

Conclusions

Weight faltering in early infancy was associated with poorer educational outcomes in later childhood, but these associations were explained by confounding. The subsequent psychosocial development of infants with slow weight gain was not different from that of their peers.

Keywords: growth faltering, behaviour, ALSPAC


Article summary.

Article focus

  • Studies of clinically derived samples of children with failure to thrive have reported problems with psychosocial development and subsequent educational attainment.

  • We have used a population-based study of weight faltering in 11 534 term infants to investigate whether children who grew slowly in the first 9 months had different psychological, behavioural and educational outcomes later in childhood compared with their peers.

Key messages

  • Using a large birth cohort, weight faltering in infancy was associated with poorer educational outcomes in later childhood, but these associations were explained by confounding factors.

  • Infants with slow weight gain who were otherwise well had similar psychosocial development to their peers at school age.

Strengths and limitations of this study

  • This research utilised a large representative population-based cohort (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, ALSPAC), on whom a wide range of psychological and educational outcomes were available.

  • The main limitation is missing data in some of the outcome measures, due to loss to follow-up.

  • ALSPAC is an observational research study, and we do not know which cases were identified by health and education services and subject to interventions which may have influenced the outcome.

Introduction

Failure to thrive (FTT) is a term describing children who fail to grow at the expected rate, with various definitions, including ‘infants and young children whose growth is substantially less than that of their peers’.1 A common factor is the identification of cases by weight gain, so the term weight faltering is preferred as it avoids the pejorative use of ‘failure’. The development of conditional growth measures,2 which take account of an infant's weight gain relative to their sex, initial weight and regression towards the mean, has enabled a standardised anthropometric approach to the identification of cases of weight faltering.

In view of the evidence for developmental delay3 4 and poorer cognitive outcomes5 6 associated with early growth faltering, one may predict that adverse effects extend to psychosocial development and educational attainment. The link between growth problems and insecure attachment7 8 adds further plausibility that early growth faltering could be causally related to disordered emotional development. Early studies on FTT based on clinically derived samples reported that growth faltering was associated with behavioural disturbances, poorer social skills, lower self-esteem, abnormal personalities, negative relationships, delayed social maturity, lower emotional stability, learning difficulties and school problems.9–19 Recently, population-based cohort studies which define growth faltering on standardised anthropometric criteria20–24 have challenged these traditional findings. Systematic reviews have highlighted the paucity of high-quality follow-up studies and recommended the investigation of a larger range of outcomes, including attention and emotional development.5 25

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)26 has been used to investigate the epidemiological factors,27 28 the anthropometric outcomes29 and the IQ6 of children with weight faltering in infancy. The analysis of IQ showed that children whose weight faltered from birth to 9 months had a total IQ that was significantly lower by an average of 2.71 points at 8 years, equivalent to 0.17 SD. Weight gain from birth to 8 weeks had a positive linear association with child IQ at 8 years, but weight gain from 8 weeks to 9 months did not show an association with IQ.

We now report on the psychological and educational outcomes in later childhood of infants in ALSPAC who had weight faltering in the first 9 months.

Methods

ALSPAC is a UK-based birth cohort study designed to examine the genetic and environmental determinants of child health and development. The study recruited 15 247 pregnant women resident in the former Avon Health Authority area with an expected date of delivery between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992, resulting in a total cohort of 14 775 live-births. Avon has a mixture of urban and rural communities with sociodemographic characteristics similar to the rest of the UK at the 1991 census. Methodological details of the study have been published elsewhere26 and details on questionnaires and clinics can be found on the ALSPAC website.30

Definition of cases

Weight data collected by health professionals as part of routine child health surveillance were obtained from the Avon Child Health Computer system. Weights were taken at birth, 8 weeks (range 1–3 months), and 9 months (range 6–12 months) and converted to z scores adjusted for gender and age using the British 1990 Growth Reference.31 If data were missing for any of these measures, children were not included in further analysis (n=1292). Infants were also excluded if they had a major congenital abnormality likely to affect growth (eg, Down syndrome; n=89), were non-singleton births (n=184) or were born preterm (<37 weeks) or post-term (>42 weeks; n=871). The number of children with a complete weight dataset available for this analysis was 11 534.

Growth was measured by calculating differences in z scores between two time points and adjusted for regression towards the mean using Cole's equation,2 utilising regression coefficients derived from within the cohort. The resulting weight gain was ‘conditional’ on gender, age and initial weight. As in previous studies investigating weight faltering in ALSPAC, ‘cases’ were identified as infants with conditional weight gain below the 5th centile in the cohort (z score below −1.645). All other infants in the cohort with weight gain above the 5th centile comprised the control group. Three periods of weight faltering were compared: birth to 8 weeks (early group), 8 weeks to 9 months (late group) and birth to 9 months.

Outcomes

Between 6 and 11 years of age, psychosocial development was measured by direct assessment of the child at research clinics, and by parental and teacher report in questionnaires. The measures used are detailed in table 1. Childhood educational outcomes include Standardised Assessment Test (SAT) results for Key Stages 1 (KS1) and 2 (KS2), obtained from Local Education Authorities in England. These tests are compulsory for children in state-funded schools in England, but are optional for independent private schools. KS1 comprises years 1–2 at primary school (ages 5–7 years) and includes compulsory national tests at 7 years of age in Reading, Writing and Mathematics. KS2 comprises years 3–6 at primary school (ages 8–11 years) and includes compulsory national tests at 11 years of age in English, Mathematics and Science. Educational attainment is categorised into National Curriculum Levels 1–8. In this analysis, KS1 and KS2 summary scores were computed by adding together the National Curriculum Levels the child achieved in each component. Information on children receiving Special Educational Needs (SENs) support was collected from the Pupil Level Annual School Census, a survey which is completed annually by all state schools in England.

Table 1.

Summary of measures used for psychosocial and educational outcomes

Age of child (years) Measure Derived from Comments
6 Behaviour (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, SDQ41) Parent report Total Behavioural Difficulties score (/40) comprises sum of 4 prorated component scores (hyperactivity, emotional symptoms, conduct and peer problems)
7 Behaviour (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, SDQ41) Teacher report As above
Mental health (Development and Well-Being Assessment, DAWBA42) Parent and teacher report Various prorated scores used (anxiety, mood, attention/activity symptoms, awkward and troublesome behaviours)
Social cognition (Social Communication Disorders Checklist, SCDC43) Parent report Prorated score summarises 12 questions on the main features of social cognition behaviour (/24)
SATs—Key Stage One (Reading/Writing/Mathematics, KS1) School/LEA records Children are expected to achieve ≥ National Curriculum Level 2 in each subject at Key Stage 1
8 Attention (Tests of Everyday Attention for Children, TEACh44) Assessment of child Time-dependent tasks measure selective attention, ability to divide attention between 2 tasks and attentional control
Friends (Cambridge Hormones and Moods project Friendship questionnaire45) Assessment of child Summary score (/15) comprising child's satisfaction with friends, frequency they see them outside school and if they can talk to friends about problems and they are understanding
Bullying (Bullying and Friendship Interview Schedule46) Assessment of child Summary measure comprises proportion of children receiving direct or indirect bullying, frequently or very frequently in the previous 6 months
Antisocial activities (Self-reported antisocial behaviour for young children questionnaire47) Assessment of child Summary measure comprises proportion of children who report ever undertaking ≥2 of 11 antisocial activities.
Self-esteem (Harter's Self Perception Profile for Children48) Assessment of child Scores generated for global self-worth and scholastic competence subscales (both/24)
Non-verbal accuracy (Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy, DANVA49) Assessment of child Summary measure comprises proportion of children with ≥7 errors on facial expressions recognition task (Faces subtest)
9 Depression (Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire, MFQ50 51) Parent report Depression score (/26) comprises sum of 13 questions on the child's moods and feelings in the past 2 weeks
Special Educational Needs (SEN) School/PLASC records Summary measure comprises proportion of children with SEN±statement of SEN
10 Depression (Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire, MFQ50 51) Assessment of child Summary measure comprises proportion of children with SEN±statement of SEN
SEN School/PLASC records Summary measure comprises proportion of children with SEN±statement of SEN
11 Attention (Tests of Everyday Attention for Children, TEACh44) Assessment of child Summary measure comprises proportion of children with SEN±statement of SEN
SATs—Key Stage Two (English/Mathematics/Science, KS2) School/LEA records Children are expected to achieve ≥ National Curriculum Level 4 in each subject at Key Stage 2

In all of the numerical outcomes, a higher score equates to a worse outcome except for the Harter's Self Perception Profile for Children where a higher score indicates better self-esteem. Full details on the measures can be found using the respective references.

PLASC, Pupil Level Annual School Census.

Confounders

Background sociodemographic and health data were obtained from postal questionnaires completed by the study mother and partner at various stages through pregnancy, infancy and childhood. Factors predictive of weight faltering identified in previous ALSPAC studies27 28 were used as covariates for infant growth in the regression analyses. Covariates used in a previous ALSPAC study on weight faltering and cognitive development6 were entered into regression analyses of educational outcomes, and covariates for psychosocial outcomes were the same as used in a previous ALSPAC study on child behaviour.32 These include: gender, the child's IQ, birthweight, breastfeeding, maternal age and education, maternal smoking, family size, housing tenure, a home facilities score at 6 months, paternal socioeconomic status at the time of pregnancy, Family Adversity Index (a cumulative index of adversity including housing quality, financial difficulties, partner relationships, maternal mental health, education, criminality, excess alcohol/drugs) and parenting attitudes at 6 months.

Statistical methodology

Analysis was undertaken using Stata V.11.1 (StataCorp 2009). For univariate analysis, normal distributions were described using means and SD, and other distributions using medians and IQR. Simple cross-tabulations were undertaken for binary outcomes, using χ² or Fisher's exact test as appropriate. Differences in means were tested using the unpaired t test for two means or analysis of variance for several means, whereas the difference in medians was tested using the Mann-Whitney U test.

Multivariate multiple regression analyses for all three time periods of growth faltering were conducted with psychosocial and educational outcomes as dependent variables, using backwards stepwise models. Numeric scale outcomes were investigated with ordinal regression and binary outcomes with logistic regression. For all regression analyses, initial models were created excluding covariates with over 10% missing data, and then repeated with those covariates added back in to ascertain their effect and create final models. A p value of >0.01 was used for stepwise exclusion of covariates in view of the number of outcome variables investigated and the importance of a conservative approach to analysis. We took the approach of using available data for each outcome, as restricting the analysis to only those with complete data for all outcomes would reduce the numbers substantially.

Results

Ascertainment

Of the 11 534 infants with complete infant weight data, 46–62% had data available on psychosocial outcomes from child-derived, parent-derived and teacher-derived measures, while educational outcome data were available for 82–87%.

Children attending research clinics and those with returned questionnaires were more likely to come from families of higher socioeconomic class, with a higher maternal educational level and secure housing tenure (p≤0.01 for all). In contrast, the response bias for educational outcomes was in the opposite direction (p≤0.001 for socioeconomic class and maternal education), attributable to missing data from independent schools since SATs were only compulsory in the state school system.

Infant growth

Weight z scores (corrected for gender and gestational age) at birth, 8 weeks and 9 months were normally distributed with a mean just above 0 and SD of ∼1. Weight gain z scores were also normally distributed, centiles were constructed, and infants below the 5th centile were classified as cases of growth faltering. There were 517 (4.5%) cases of ‘early’ weight faltering between birth and 8 weeks, and 487 (4.2%) in the ‘late’ group between 8 weeks and 9 months. Only 30 infants were classed as cases in both the early and late periods.

The birth to 9-month weight faltering group consisted of 520 infants (4.5%) who were slow growing (<5th centile) over a longer period. Of those in the birth to 9-month group, only 97/520 (19%) were also in the birth to 8-week group, but 334/520 (64%) were also in the 8-week to 9-month group.

Psychosocial outcomes

Only 2 of the 14 psychosocial outcomes demonstrated statistically significant differences (p<0.01) on univariate analysis; at 8 years, children with a history of early weight faltering demonstrated worse attention in half of the TEACh tasks, and cases of weight faltering between birth to 6–8 weeks and birth to 9 months also made more errors on the DANVA facial emotions recognition task.

Table 2 demonstrates univariate results and regression analyses of psychosocial outcomes for early growth faltering. Differences in psychosocial outcomes present on univariate analysis became non-significant after adjustment, except for the TEACh tasks at 8 years, where cases with early growth faltering demonstrated worse divided attention than controls (OR 1.43, CI 1.13 to 1.80, p≤0.01). The regression model for this attention task showed independent effects of sex (worse in boys, OR 1.25, CI 1.14 to 1.38, p≤0.001) and IQ (worse with lower IQ, OR 0.97, CI 0.968 to 0.974, p≤0.001). No interaction effects were observed. When the attention tasks were repeated at a research clinic at 11 years, no association was observed with any period of growth faltering, even on univariate analysis, so the clinical importance of the finding at 8 years is questionable.

Table 2.

Psychosocial outcomes in later childhood of weight faltering between birth to 8 weeks

Age (years) Psychosocial outcome Weight faltering between birth to 8 weeks
Adjusted OR† (95% CI's)
Case
Control
N Median/proportion IQR/% N Median/ proportion IQR/%
6 Behavioural difficulties score/40 268 7 4–11 5874 7 4–10 0.87 (0.67 to 1.12)
7 Behavioural difficulties score/40 237 4 2–9 5062 4 2–9 0.92 (0.68 to 1.24)
Mental health (teacher report)
 Attention/activity symptoms score/38 239 3 1–10 5062 3 0–10 1.01 (0.75 to 1.35)
 Awkward/troublesome behaviour score/40 239 0 0–1 5058 0 0–2 0.82 (0.55 to 1.22)
Mental health (parent report)
 General anxieties score/14 300 1 0–3 6866 0 0–2 1.11 (0.89 to 1.38)
 General anxiety symptoms score/12 300 0 0–2 6835 0 0–1 1.18 (0.92 to 1.51)
 Mood symptoms score/12 295 0 0–0 6758 0 0–0 1.19 (0.84 to 1.69)
 Attention/activity symptoms score/36 298 2 0–7 6859 2 0–7 0.93 (0.72 to 1.20)
 Awkward behaviours score/18 296 0 0–1 6820 0 0–1 0.84 (0.62 to 1.13)
 Troublesome behaviours score/14 299 0 0–1 6834 0 0–1 1.18 (0.88 to 1.58)
 Social cognition score/24 299 2 0–4 6820 2 0–4 0.87 (0.67 to 1.12)
8 Attention
 Selective attention score (range −0.4 to 46.6) 273 4.9 4.1–6.1 5759 4.9 4.1–5.8 1.06 (0.83 to 1.35)
 Dividing attention score (range −1.0 to 270.9) 242 2.6* 1.0–7.4 5264 2.0 0.4–5.5 1.43 (1.13 to 1.80)**
 Attentional control-same worlds task (seconds) 274 13.0 11.5–14.5 5779 12.5 11.0–14.5 0.98 (0.79 to 1.22)
 Attentional control-opposite worlds task (seconds) 274 17.5* 15.0–20.0 5775 16.5 14.5–19.5 1.15 (0.93 to 1.43)
 Friends score/15 267 3 2–5 5721 3 2–5 1.26 (1.01 to 1.56)
Bullying status
 Victim of direct bullying 366 102/366 38% 5703 1989/5703 34% 1.17 (0.89 to 1.53)
 Victim of indirect bullying 252 43/252 17% 5576 915/5576 16% 0.97 (0.67 to 1.39)
 ≥2 anti-social activities 269 20/269 7% 5716 426/5716 7% 1.08 (0.67 to 1.77)
 ≥7 errors on non-verbal accuracy task 252 76/253* 30% 5456 1202/5456 22% 1.36 (1.00 to 1.83)
Self-esteem
 Scholastic competence score/24 259 17 15–19 5573 17 15–20 0.96 (0.77 to 1.20)
 Global self-worth score/24 261 19 17–21 5565 20 17–22 0.81 (0.65 to 1.01)
9 Depression score/26 304 2 0–4 6507 2 0–4 1.05 (0.83 to 1.34)
10 Depression score/26 273 3 1–6 5857 3 1–6 1.01 (0.79 to 1.30)
11 Attention
 Selective attention score (range −1.9 to 25.7) 263 3.4 2.8–4.0 5644 3.4 2.8–4.1 1.11 (0.86 to 1.44)
 Dividing attention score (range −7.2 to 186.5) 258 0.4 −0.2–1.9 5593 0.4 −0.3–1.5 1.11 (0.87 to 1.41)
 Attentional control-same worlds task (seconds) 255 10 9–11 5443 10 9–11 1.12 (0.88 to 1.42)
 Attentional control-opposite worlds task (seconds) 255 12.5 11.5–14 5441 12.5 11–14 1.05 (0.82 to 1.33)

In all the numerical outcomes, a higher score equates to a worse outcome except for self-esteem scores where a higher score indicates better self-esteem.

*p<0.01 on univariate analysis.

**p<0.01 on multivariate analysis.

†Adjusted for (1) potential confounders for psychosocial outcomes: gender, the child's IQ, maternal education, housing tenure, maternal smoking, home facilities score, paternal socioeconomic status, family adversity index, parenting attitudes at 6 months; and (2) potential confounders for infant growth between birth and 6–8 weeks: time between weight measurements, maternal height, maternal age, use of a car, infant health before weight measurement and feeding difficulties (general and poor sucking) in the first weeks of life.

Online supplementary appendix 1 contains the univariate results and regression analyses of psychosocial outcomes for weight faltering between 8 weeks and 9 months and birth to 9 months, which did not show any consistent associations.

Although the correlation between the parent and teacher versions of the SDQ behavioural difficulties score was 0.33, (p<0.0001), univariate results showed that parents consistently rated their children as having more behavioural difficulties (median 7, IQR 4–10) than did teachers (median 4, IQR 2–9). Hyperactivity, as measured by DAWBA attention/activity scores, was significantly correlated between the parent and teacher versions (Spearman’s r=0.40, p<0.0001), and teachers consistently scored children higher on the hyperactivity scale than did parents (median 3, IQR 0–10 vs median 2, IQR 0–7 for parental report).

Educational outcomes

Unadjusted results demonstrated poorer educational attainment and higher rates of SENs among cases of weight faltering. These differences were more pronounced at KS2 and in the early weight faltering group displayed in (table 3). On multivariate analysis, all these differences became non-significant, except for isolated disparities in achieving the expected levels in KS1 Mathematics for cases of late growth faltering, and KS1 Mathematics and KS2 English for those with growth faltering between birth to 9 months. Regression models for failing to achieve the expected levels in KS1 Mathematics demonstrated independent effects of a number of factors other than weight faltering, such as the age of the child at testing, lower paternal socioeconomic class, low-maternal educational level and insecure housing tenure. The regression models of those who did not reach the expected levels for KS2 English demonstrated independent effects of age at testing, having non-breast milk at 6 months, low-paternal socioeconomic class, insecure housing tenure, low-maternal educational level and large families, with the latter four factors having a stronger effect than weight faltering between birth and 9 months.

Table 3.

Weight faltering in infancy and later educational outcomes

Educational outcome Number with outcome (N=11 534) Weight faltering in infancy
Birth to 8 weeks
8 weeks to 9 months
Birth to 9 months
Case N or median, (% or IQR) Control N or median, (% or IQR) Adjusted OR† (95% CI's) Case N or median, (% or IQR) Control N or median, (% or IQR) Adjusted OR‡ (95% CI's) Case N or median, (% or IQR) Control N or median, (% or IQR) Adjusted OR§ (95% CI's)
Key stage 1 (6–7 years)
 <Level 2 in reading 9454 84/434 (19%)* 1287/9020 (14%) 1.16 (0.81 to 1.66) 74/384 (19%)* 1297/9070 (14%) 1.39 (0.98 to 1.96) 74/417 (18%) 1297/9037 (14%) 1.08 (0.75 to 1.55)
 <Level 2 in writing 9456 76/433 (18%) 1316/9023 (15%) 0.91 (0.62 to 1.34) 75/384 (20%)* 1317/9072 (15%) 1.41 (1.01 to 1.95) 81/417 (19%)* 1311/9039 (15%) 1.34 (0.96 to 1.85)
 <Level 2 in Mathematics 9452 57/434 (13%) 900/9018 (10%) 1.10 (0.73 to 1.67) 62/417 (15%)* 895/9035 (10%) 1.74 (1.21 to 2.49)** 58/384 (15%)* 899/9068 (10%) 1.65 (1.15 to 2.37)**
 KS1 summary score 9439 9 (6–12)* 10 (7–13) 0.87 (0.70 to 1.08) 9.5 (6.5–13) 10 (7–12) 0.90 (0.72 to 1.13) 9 (6–12) 10 (7–12) 0.81 (0.65 to 1.01)
Key stage 2 (10–11 years)
 <Level 4 in English 10 081 111/441 (25%)* 1784/9640 (19%) 1.29 (0.93 to 1.79) 90/404 (22%) 1805/9677 (19%) 1.19 (0.87 to 1.63) 118/448 (26%)* 1777/9633 (18%) 1.49 (1.12 to 1.97)**
 <Level 4 in Mathematics 10 043 121/442 (27%)* 2012/9601 (21%) 1.45 (1.07 to 1.95) 95/398 (24%) 2038/9645 (21%) 1.30 (0.95 to 1.80) 124/444 (28%)* 2009/9599 (21%) 1.47 (1.09 to 1.97)
 <Level 4 in Science 10 089 59/444 (13%)* 788/9645 (8%) 1.55 (1.05 to 2.31) 42/403 (10%) 805/9686 (8%) 1.15 (0.74 to 1.80) 54/447 (12%)* 793/9642 (8%) 1.22 (0.77 to 1.94)
 KS2 summary score 9971 12 (11–14)* 13 (11–14) 0.77 (0.62 to 0.96) 13 (11–14) 13 (11–14) 0.89 (0.71 to 1.10) 12 (11–14)* 13 (11–14) 0.82 (0.66 to 1.01)
Special Educational Needs status
 SEN recorded at 9 years 9700 101/432 (23%)* 1709/9268 (18%) 1.04 (0.77 to 1.41) 84/387 (22%) 1726/9313 (19%) 0.89 (0.71 to 1.10) 102/434 (24%)* 1708/9266 (18%) 1.27 (0.94 to 1.71)
 SEN recorded at 10 years 9764 88/439 (20%) 1476/9325 (16%) 1.27 (0.94 to 1.71) 79/389 (20%) 1485/9375 (16%) 1.19 (0.87 to 1.63) 90/436 (21%)* 1474/9328 (16%) 1.18 (0.88 to 1.58)

*p<0.01 on univariate analysis.

**p<0.01 on multivariate analysis.

†Adjusted for (1) age of the child at testing (not for SEN status); (2) potential confounders for educational outcomes: maternal education, paternal socioeconomic class, housing tenure, use of a car, family size, birth-weight, breastfeeding, hospital admissions over childhood and (3) potential confounders for infant growth between birth and 6–8 weeks: time between weight measurements, maternal height, maternal age, infant health before weight measurement and feeding difficulties (general and poor sucking) in the first weeks of life.

‡Adjusted for (1) age of the child at testing (not for SEN status); (2) potential confounders for educational outcomes: maternal education, paternal socioeconomic class, housing tenure, use of a car, family size, birth-weight, breastfeeding, hospital admissions over childhood and (3) Potential confounders for infant growth between 6–8 weeks and 9 months: time between weight measurements, maternal height, infant health before weight measurement and feeding difficulties (refusing non-breast milk and taking too small a quantity) in first months of life.

§Adjusted for (1) age of the child at testing (not for SEN status) (2) potential confounders for educational outcomes: maternal education, paternal socioeconomic class, housing tenure, use of a car, family size, birth-weight, breastfeeding, hospital admissions over childhood and (3) potential confounders for infant growth between birth and 9 months: time between weight measurements, maternal height, maternal body mass index and feeding difficulties (refusing non-breast milk and taking too small a quantity) in the first months of life.

Discussion

This comprehensive, prospective study of early growth faltering investigated a wide range of psychological, social and educational outcomes in middle childhood. Although a proportionally small number of measures from the many tested appeared worse in children who had faltered in their growth in infancy, almost all became non-significant after adjustment for confounding factors, and the conclusion of this investigation is that growth faltering in infancy is not associated with poorer psychosocial and educational outcomes in later childhood.

The biggest limitation of this work, as with many large longitudinal cohort studies, is missing data, although the number of cases of weight faltering in this study is still large compared with that in the published literature. We acknowledge that response bias is present in the dataset but do not believe that it affects our conclusions, since a simulation study on behavioural disorders in ALSPAC found that the validity of regression models is only marginally affected despite the range restrictions after selective dropout.33 Furthermore, the lack of effect is consistent across the range of outcomes despite educational data exhibiting the opposite response bias to clinic-derived and questionnaire-derived measures.

ALSPAC is an observational research study, and we do not know which cases were identified by health and education services and subject to interventions, which could potentially have attenuated the effects of growth faltering on later psychosocial and educational outcomes. However, considering that randomised trials of interventions for FTT have failed to demonstrate convincing effects in these areas,34 35 it seems unlikely that clinical interventions have masked the impact on psychological development and educational attainment.

Despite these limitations, the study represents a comprehensive investigation into the later childhood outcomes of growth faltering in infancy, utilising a large population-based sample. This dataset benefits from extensive, prospectively collected information on relevant confounders, and the covariates entered into regression models were chosen on the basis of literature review and previous work on infant growth, cognitive and behavioural development in ALSPAC. Assessing the child's development directly in research clinics and from parental and teacher's perspectives provides triangulation of our findings and adds confidence to our conclusions. We have not been able to investigate outcomes against the severity of the infant growth faltering, but have looked at chronicity—and were able to compare early weight faltering over the first 8 weeks with longer faltering over 9 months. However, in view of the large number of outcomes investigated, a conservative approach to analysis was appropriate and we have been careful not to exaggerate our univariate findings.

The unadjusted educational outcomes are consistent with previously reported IQ data from this cohort,6 in that poorer achievement was more evident in children with early rather than late growth faltering in infancy. However, the great majority of differences become non-significant after adjustment for confounding, suggesting that childhood educational attainment is more strongly influenced by other sociodemographic factors, especially maternal education and the quality of the home environment. Differences persisting after adjustment (eg, poorer ability at 8 years in divided attention) must be interpreted with caution in view of the small effect sizes and the large number of non-significant outcomes. The lack of adverse effects is in agreement with UK population-based studies where growth faltering was defined using similar conditional anthropometric criteria—for example, Drewett et al,24 36 but differs from studies based on families from deprived backgrounds.23 This study builds on those reports with a population-based sample, a greater number of cases and a wider range of outcomes, and provides clear messages for clinical practice.

Clinical implications

The message that children who grow slowly in infancy are unlikely to have adverse psychosocial and educational consequences will reassure parents and clinicians. A large proportion of weight faltering infants who are otherwise well will be ‘slow normal’, growing according to their genetic potential (hence, the strong influence of parental height27 28). The majority of these infants warrant a conservative and supportive approach unless additional symptoms or signs of illness are present. Recommendations for the management of weight faltering in developed countries should balance this finding with the increasing evidence for adverse consequences of rapid weight gain in infancy,37 which implies that a policy of augmenting ‘catch up’ growth by giving extra calories is undesirable. In low-income settings, however, weight faltering is more often a consequence of undernutrition, with good evidence to suggest poorer psychological and cognitive outcomes and a greater need for intervention.38 39

Conclusions

Clinicians looking after infants showing weight faltering can have difficulty in containing parents’ anxiety, even when the child is otherwise well, since parents often fear slow growth is indicative of an underlying problem.40 This study provides good evidence to reassure clinicians and families that, in a resource-rich society with a good educational system, slow weight gain is not associated with later disadvantages in psychosocial development or achievement at school.

Supplementary Material

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council (Grant ref: 74882), the Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: GR076467) and the University of Bristol provided core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors who will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper.

Footnotes

Contributors: ARH undertook the analysis, initially drafted the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript submitted. PSB supervised ARH in the statistical analysis and approved the final version of the manuscript submitted. AME designed the study, interpreted the results, redrafted the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript submitted.

Funding: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests: None.

Ethics approval: ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: The ALSPAC policy on data sharing is available on the website: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac. To discuss access to ALSPAC data, please contact the ALSPAC executive team on alspac-exec@bristol.ac.uk.

References

  • 1.Wilcox WD, Nieburg P, Miller DS. Failure to thrive—a continuing problem of definition. Clin Pediatr 1989;28:391–4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cole TJ. Conditional reference charts to assess weight gain in British infants. Arch Dis Child 1995;73:8–16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Drewett R, Emond A, Blair P, et al. The importance of slow weight gain in the first 2 months in identifying children who fail to thrive. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2005;23:309–17 [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wilensky DS, Ginsberg G, Altman M, et al. A community based study of failure to thrive in Israel. Arch Dis Child 1996;75:145–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Corbett SS, Drewett RF. To what extent is failure to thrive in infancy associated with poorer cognitive development? A review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45:641–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Emond AM, Blair PS, Emmett PM, et al. Weight faltering in infancy and IQ levels at 8 years in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics 2007;120:e1051–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ward MJ, Kessler DB, Altman SC. Infant-mother attachment in children with failure-to-thrive. Infant Ment Health J 1993;14:208–20 [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ward MJ, Lee SS, Lipper EG. Failure-to-thrive is associated with disorganized infant-mother attachment and unresolved maternal attachment. Infant Ment Health J 2000;21:428–42 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glaser HH, Heagarty MC, Bullard DM, et al. Physical and psychological development of children with early failure to thrive. J Pediatr 1968;73:690–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Elmer E, Gregg GS, Ellison P. Late results of the ‘failure to thrive’ syndrome. Clin Pediatr 1969;8:584–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hufton IW, Oates RK. Nonorganic failure to thrive: a long-term follow-up. Pediatrics 1977;59:73–7 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.White JL, Malcolm R, Roper K. Psychosocial and developmental factors in failure to thrive: one- to three-year follow-up. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1981;2:112–14 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Iwaniec D, Sneddon H, Allen S. The outcomes of a longitudinal study of non-organic failure-to-thrive. Child Abuse Rev 2003;12:216–26 [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pollitt E, Eichler A. Behavioral disturbances among failure-to-thrive children. Am J Dis Child 1976;130:24–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Oates RK, Peacock A, Forrest D. Development in children following abuse and nonorganic failure to thrive. Am J Dis Child 1984;138:764–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Oates RK, Peacock A, Forrest D. Long-term effects of nonorganic failure to thrive. Pediatrics 1985;75:36–40 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Drotar D, Sturm L. Personality-development, problem-solving, and behavior problems among preschool-children with early histories of nonorganic failure-to-thrive—a controlled-study. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1992;13:266–73 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Reif SM, Beler B, Villa Y, et al. Long-term follow-up and outcome of infants with nonorganic failure-to-thrive. Israel J Med Sci 1995;31:483–9 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dykman RA, Casey PH, Ackerman PT, et al. Behavioral and cognitive status in school-aged children with a history of failure to thrive during early childhood. Clin Pediatr 2001;40:63–70 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mitchell WG, Gorrell RW, Greenberg RA. Failure-to-thrive—a study in a primary care setting—epidemiology and follow-up. Pediatrics 1980;65:971–7 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Puckering C, Pickles A, Skuse D, et al. Mother-child interaction and the cognitive and behavioral-development of 4-year-old children with poor growth. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 1995;36:573–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Corbett SS, Drewett RF, Wright CM. Does a fall down a centile chart matter? The growth and developmental sequelae of mild failure to thrive. Acta Paediatrica 1996;85:1278–83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kerr MA, Black MM. Failure-to-thrive, maltreatment and the behavior and development of 6-year-old children from low-income, urban families: a cumulative risk model. Child Abuse Neglect 2000;24:587–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Drewett RF, Corbett SS, Wright CM. Physical and emotional development, appetite and body image in adolescents who failed to thrive as infants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006;47:524–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rudolf MCJ, Logan S. What is the long term outcome for children who fail to thrive? A systematic review. Arch Dis Child 2005;90:925–31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, et al. Cohort profile: The ‘Children of the Nineties’- the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2012;42:111–27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Blair PS, Drewett RF, Emmett PM, et al. Family, socioeconomic and prenatal factors associated with failure to thrive in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Int J Epidemiol 2004;33:839–47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Emond A, Drewett R, Blair P, et al. Postnatal factors associated with failure to thrive in term infants in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Arch Dis Child 2007;92:115–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.ud Din Z, Emmett P, Steer C, et al. Growth outcomes of weight faltering in infancy in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Pediatrics 2013;131:e843–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children website http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/ 2013
  • 31.Freeman JV, Cole TJ, Chinn S, et al. Cross sectional stature and weight reference curves for the UK, 1990. Arch Dis Child 1995;73:17–24 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Chandramouli K, Steer CD, Ellis M, et al. Effects of early childhood lead exposure on academic performance and behaviour of school age children. Arch Dis Child 2009;94:844–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wolke D, Waylen A, Samara M, et al. Selective drop-out in longitudinal studies and non-biased prediction of behaviour disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2009;195:249–56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Black MM, Dubowitz H, Krishnakumar A, et al. Early intervention and recovery among children with failure to thrive: follow-up at age 8. Pediatrics 2007;120:59–69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Raynor P, Rudolf MC, Cooper K, et al. A randomised controlled trial of specialist health visitor intervention for failure to thrive. Arch Dis Child 1999(6):500–6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Drewett RF, Corbett SS, Wright CM. Cognitive and educational attainments at school age of children who failed to thrive in infancy: a population-based study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40:551–61 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ong KK, Loos RJF. Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta Paediatrica 2006;95:904–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Galler JR, Ramsey F. A follow-up study of the influence of early malnutrition on development: behavior at home and at school. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1989;28:254–61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, et al. Early childhood stunting is associated with poor psychological functioning in late adolescence and effects are reduced by psychosocial stimulation. J Nutr 2007;137:2464–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lucas P, Arai L, Baird J, et al. A systematic review of lay views about infant size and growth. Arch Dis Child 2007;92:120–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:581–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, et al. The development and well-being assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41:645–55 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Skuse DH, Mandy WPL, Scourfield J. Measuring autistic traits: heritability, reliability and validity of the Social and Communication Disorders Checklist. Br J Psychiatry 2005;187:568–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Robertson IH, Ward T, Ridgeway V, et al. The structure of normal human attention: the test of everyday attention. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:525–34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goodyer IM, Wright C, Altham PME. Recent friendships in anxious and depressed school age children. Psychol Med 1989;19:165–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wolke D, Woods S, Bloomfield L, et al. The association between direct and relational bullying and behaviour problems among primary school children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 2000;41:989–1002 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Kammen Wv, et al. Development of a new measure of self-reported antisocial behavior for young children: prevalence and reliability. In: Klein MW. ed. Cross-national research in self-reported crime and delinquency. Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989, 203–25 [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harter S. Manual for the self-perception profile for children. University of Denver, 1985 [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nowicki S, Duke MP. Individual-differences in the nonverbal-communication of affect—the diagnostic-analysis of nonverbal accuracy scale. J Nonverbal Behav 1994;18:9–35 [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wood A, Kroll L, Moore A, et al. Properties of the mood and feelings questionnaire in adolescent psychiatric outpatients: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1995;36:327–34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, et al. The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995;5:237–49 [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Author's manuscript
Reviewer comments

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES