Table 1.
Author and reference | Study design | Target population | Breast milk exposure | Cognitive development assessment tool and participant age | Results | Confounders adjusted for | Difference in IQ after adjustment | Study quality grading |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Amanda and Singh21 | Prospective cohort. Feeding method collected retrospectively | 84 school-aged children from two schools in India | Exclusive breast milk for more than 4 months vs less than 4 months or formula fed | General mental ability test for children from 7–11 years (Srivastava and Saxena 1988–1989) | There were significantly more breastfed (>4 m) children in the higher IQ category (IQ>109) | None | Not provided | C |
Andres et al | Longitudinal study | 391 healthy infants enrolled in the Beginnings Study in Arkansas, USA | Breastfed vs soy fed vs milk-based formula fed | Assessed at ages 3, 6, 9 and 12 months using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) second edition, from which the Mental Developmental Index (MDI) and Psychomotor Development Index (PDI) were derived | BF infants scored slightly higher than formula-fed infants on the MDI score at ages 6 and 12 months (p<0.05). Confounders included in the model | Socioeconomic status, mother's age and IQ, gestational age, gender, birth weight, head circumference, race, age and diet history | BF infants scored 1–2 points higher than formula-fed infants on the MDI score at ages 6 and 12 months (p<0.05) | A |
Angelsen et al 200123 | Prospective cohort | 345 children in Scandinavia (Norway and Sweden) | <3 months 3–6 months >6 months |
BSID at 13 months. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence (WPPSI-R), and Peabody Developmental Motor Scales (PDMS) at age 5 years | Shorter duration of breastfeeding was associated with lower scores on mental developmental tests both at 13 months and at 5 years of age. Unadjusted difference of eight points | Maternal age, education, smoking and Raven score (IQ) | Maternal age, education and intelligence were significant confounders. When analysing performance IQ and verbal IQ separately, the median IQ value was not statistically different when adjusting for maternal Raven score | B |
Auestad et al24 | Prospective randomised longitudinal study comparing different formula types and non-randomised breastfeeding group | 294 children from four sites in the USA | Breastfeeding until age 12 m, vs three different types of formulas (±AA, DHA) | Bayley scale for infant development at 6 and 12 months Fagan test of infant intelligence at 6 and 9 months MacArthur communicative development inventories at 9 and 14 months |
No difference in any of the parameters checked between any study groups | None | No difference in any of the parameters checked between any of the study groups | A |
Auestad et al25 | Follow-up study of Auestad 2001: prospective randomised longitudinal study comparing different formula types and a non-randomised breastfeeding group | 157 children from the original 294 children from four sites in the USA | Breastfeeding until age 12 months, vs three different types of formulas (±AA,DHA) | At 39 months, standard tests of IQ (Stanford Binet IQ), receptive vocabulary (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised), and expressive vocabulary (mean length of utterance) | No difference in any of the parameters checked between any study groups | None | No difference in any of the parameters checked between any study groups | A |
Bartels et al26 | Retrospective cohort. Breastfeeding status prospectively assessed | 672 monozygotic male twin pairs 637 dizygotic male twin pairs 860 monozygotic female twin pairs 647 dizygotic female twin pairs 679 male–female twin pairs 598 female–male twin pairs in the Netherlands |
<2 weeks >2 weeks |
Dutch CITO-elementary test at age 12 years | Breastfed children of highly educated mothers score on average 7.6 points higher on a standardised test of cognitive abilities than formula-fed children of mothers with a low education | Maternal education, income | A significant positive effect of breastfeeding on cognitive abilities above the expected positive effect of maternal education. Exact numbers not provided | B |
Bauer et al27 | Prospective cohort | 50 children from Honolulu, Hawaii | Breastfeeding as a continuous variable over time | The McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities at age 3 years | The duration of breastfeeding was significantly correlated with scores on the scales, General, cognitive, Quantitative and Memory | Socioeconomic status, gender and pesticide exposure | Remained significant, numbers not provided | C |
Birch et al28 | Single-center, double-blind, randomised clinical trial comparing different formula types and non-randomised breastfeeding group | 52 healthy term infants enrolled for DHA and ARA supplementation and 32 breastfed infants served as controls in Dallas, Texas, USA | Assigned diets were fed exclusively through 17 weeks of age. In the breastfeeding group, the average duration of breastfeeding was 43±9 weeks | WPPSI-R was used to assess intelligence at 4 years of age | The control formula and DHA-supplemented groups had Verbal IQ scores poorer than the breastfed group. There was no difference in performance or full-scale IQ between all groups | None | No adjustment carried out | B |
Bon29 | Retrospective cohort | 954 children from France | Exclusive bottle-fed vs breastfed for <15 days vs breastfed between 15 days and 2 months; vs breastfed for more than 2 months | The PM-47 Non-Verbal test, at 6–8.5 years of age | Higher scores for girls who were breastfed vs not breastfed. No difference in boys | Could not be assessed from the text | Not performed | C |
Bouwstra et al30 | A prospective, double-blind, randomised control study comparing different formulas. Non-randomised breastfed group as control | A control formula—n=169, an LC-PUFA supplemented n=146 Breastfed group n=159 in the Netherlands |
Supplementation—2 months All formula-fed infants received control formula between 2 and 6 months |
BSID-II at 18 months | Bayley's MDI and PDI result values did not differ significantly between the three groups | Parity, HOME score, parental Education | No difference between the groups | B |
Burruchaga et al31 | Prospective cohort | 39 children born at term and from homogeneous sociocultural status in Spain | Exclusive breast milk for at least 2 months, vs exclusive bottle-fed | BSID at 22 months | Bayley's MDI and PDI result values did not differ significantly between the groups | Maternal education, head circumference, maternal occupation, birth order, smoking status | No difference between the groups | C |
Caspi et al32 | Retrospective cohort | 858 children from the New Zealand (Dunedin) birth cohort. 1848 children from the British (E-risk study) twin birth cohort |
No breastfeeding vs breastfeeding |
New Zealand cohort: IQ measured at ages 7, 9, 11 and 13 years using Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised. IQ scores combined for an overall score British cohort: revised short form of WPPSI–age 5 |
Difference in IQ test scores between breastfed children and those not breastfed was 5.6 and 6.3 IQ points in the Dunedin and E-risk cohorts, respectively Benefit mediated by a specific genotype. Only in children carrying the C allele |
Genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism (rs174575) Socioeconomic status, Maternal cognitive ability, Gestational age, birth weight |
Children not carrying the C allele did not benefit from breastfeeding | A |
Clark et al33 | Prospective cohort | 784 Chilean children | <2 months 2>8 months >8 months |
At age 5.5 years WPPSI-R. Preschool Language Scale—3rd Edition (PLS-3) the Broad Cognitive Abilities Standard Scale (BCA) of the Batería Woodcock-Muñoz-Revisada (Batería-R) |
Poorer outcomes on the cognitive and language assessments were found for both the short and long extremes of breastfeeding as the sole milk source. The highest scores were observed in children who received breast milk as the sole milk source for 2–8 months | Gender, birth weight, child's age at testing, maternal education, IQ, depression, age, father's absence, paternal education, HOME score, socioeconomic status, nutritional status, iron deficiency anaemia at 12 months, and iron supplementation | Difference in IQ not significant | B |
Daniels and Adair34 | Prospective cohort | 1984 Filipino children | Any breastfeeding during: 0–6 months 6–12 months 12–18 months 18–24 months Over 24 months |
Philippines Non-verbal Intelligence Test of fluid abilities at ages 8.5 and 11.5 years | Poor education and suboptimal living conditions among BF mothers were strong negative confounders, causing inverse associations between BF and cognitive ability. Increased duration of any BF was of small significant benefit for cognitive development at both ages | Parental education, paternal presence in home, maternal age, parity, alcohol during pregnancy, preterm status of child, mother reads, child's gender, baths (n/week), dietary variety at 2 years, household income, non-income-producing assets, electricity in home, and environmental hygiene score | 1.6 points among normal birth weight breastfed for 12–18 months vs less than 6 months | A |
De Andraca et al35 | Prospective cohort. Subanalysis of an RCT concerning iron fortified formulas | 788 infants, 4–6 months of age in Chile | <30 days vs >30 days. All children in the study were breastfed for an average of 75 days | BSID—MDI and PDI at 12 months of age | Breastfeeding for more than 30 days was associated with significantly lower scores (2.5 points less in MDI and 2.3 in PDI). Probably due to low SES | SES, education and occupation of parents, alcohol abuse, HOME, maternal intelligence by WAIS, stressful events | No adjustment was carried out for the specific association of breastfeeding and Bayley scale | C |
De Andraca et al36 | Prospective cohort | 138 mother-infant dyad in Chile | Exclusive breastfeeding for 6 months or more vs weaning before 45 days of age | BSID at 12 months of age | No difference between the groups in MDI and PDI | None; study reports similar family characteristics in each group | No difference | C |
Der et al13 | Database analysis of a prospective study, sibling pairs analysis and metaanalysis | 5475 children 332 pairs of sibling discordant for breastfeeding status 545 discordant for duration of breastfeeding in the USA |
Breastfeeding vs no breastfeeding. Breastfeeding history obtained mostly within a year of birth | Peabody individual achievement test (PIAT) was administered to children between 5 and 14 years | Unadjusted effect of breastfeeding +4.7 compared with non-breastfeeding | Maternal IQ, education, age, family poverty, HOME stimulation score and birth order | After adjustment, the difference became non-significant | A |
Di girolamo et al37 | Prospective cohort. Breastfeeding data collected retrospectively | 80 children in Spain | Full breastfeeding for at least 4 months vs no breastfeeding since 2 weeks of age | BSID at 8–30 months | Breastfeeding group had a higher average MDI (of six points) compared with the bottle-fed group, but no difference in PDI | Parental education and age, gender, birth order | Remained significant. Data not provided | C |
Eickmann et al38 | Prospective cohort | 191 Brazilian infants | ‘Predominantly breastfed’ ‘partially breast fed’ and ‘non- breast fed’ | BSID-II at 12 months of age | Full breastfeeding at 1 month was associated with a small significant benefit in mental development compared with partial or no breastfeeding. No additional advantage in mental development was found with longer durations of full breastfeeding | Adjusted for family income, possession of TV and fridge, flush toilet, maternal work and years of schooling, number of children under 5 years, home stimulation index, smoking during pregnancy, birth weight, infant's sex, haemoglobin, weight-for-age | Full breastfeeding at 1 month was associated with +3.0 points, p=0.02 compared with partial or no breastfeeding | C |
Elwood et al39 | Prospective cohort. Breastfeeding data collected retrospectively | 779 men from Caerphilly, South Wales, UK | Artificially fed vs breastfed, duration unknown vs breastfed <3 months vs breastfed >3 months | Men aged 60–74 years The national adult reading test (NART) The AH4 13 verbal and mathematical reasoning 3. The choice reaction time (CRT) for hand-brain reaction speed |
In the normal birth weight group, the mean cognitive function was similar in both groups. In the men whose birth weight had been below the median, having been artificially fed was associated with significantly lower results in two of the three tests |
Age, social class, education, birth order and family size, father's social class, father's unemployment | In the normal birth weight group, the adjusted mean cognitive function was similar in both groups | B |
Evenhouse and Reilly40 | Database analysis of a prospective study, sibling pairs analysis. Data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health | 2734 sibling pairs in the USA | No breastfeeding <3 months 3–6 months 6–9 months 9–12 months 12–24 months >24 months | Add Health's abbreviated version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PVT), normed for age and sex | Persistent positive correlation between breastfeeding and cognitive ability Unadjusted Difference in PVT 4.9 centile points when comparing never breastfed to any breastfeeding in the full sample |
Birth weight, gender, birth order, parental investment, environmental characteristics | 1.68 centile points higher for ever breastfed to never breastfed | A |
Fergusson et al41 | Prospective cohort | Birth cohort of children from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development Study: 1037 children assessed at age 3 years 991 at age 5 years 954 at age 7 years |
Child bottle-fed; Breast-fed for up to 4 months; Breastfed for 4 months or longer. | Measures of intelligence at 3, 5 and 7 years. 3 years-Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test 5 years-Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale 7 years-Weschler Child Intelligence Scale |
Children who were breastfed for 4 months or longer had scores which were 3.84 (average) points higher than bottle-fed infants (on scales with an SD of 10) | Maternal intelligence (SRA verbal scale), maternal education, childhood experiences. Maternal training in child-rearing, family socioeconomic status. Birth weight and gestational age | A small but statistically significant benefit in the test scores of breastfed vs Bottle-fed infants (mean=1.89) | A |
Florey et al42 | Retrospective cohort | 592 firstborn singletons in Dundee | Breastfed on discharge from the hospital vs Bottle-fed | Age 18 months. Bayley Scales of Infant Mental and Motor Development | Higher mental development was significantly related to breastfeeding on discharge from hospital | Partner's social class, mother's education, height, alcohol and cigarette consumption, placental weight and the child's sex, birth weight and gestational age at birth | After adjustment, the difference in the Bayley mental development index between the groups was between 3.7 and 5.7 units | C |
Foroushani et al43 | Longitudinal cohort. Breastfeeding data collected retrospectively at age 2 years | 5362 singletons born in 1946 in England, Wales and Scotland | No breastfeeding 1–3 months 4+ months |
Age 8—sentence completion, reading and vocabulary Age 11—Verbal, reading and vocabulary. Age 15—Verbal, reading and vocabulary. Age 26—reading Age 43—visual and memory |
Children who were breastfed longer scored higher on verbal tests. Tests at older age (26, 43) were not significantly different | Birth weight, childhood illness, home conditions, parents’ age and education, child's behavioural scores, parents interest in the child's development, school type | Mean score 1.5 points higher for ages 8, 11, 15. Not significant at older ages | C |
Gale and Martyn44 | Prospective cohort | 994 men and women, born between 1920 and 1930 in Hertfordshire, UK | Exclusive breastfeeding, exclusive bottle feeding, mixed feeding | 65–75 years old. AH4 IQ test, taken on a computer | Participants who had been exclusively breastfed had slightly higher IQ scores compared with the two other groups | The use of a dummy in infancy, number of older siblings, father's occupational class, and mother's age at the participant's birth | After adjustment, no association was found between adult intelligence and method of infant feeding | B |
Gale et al45 | Prospective cohort | 241 children born to the Southampton Women's Survey, UK | Breastfeeding Fortified formula feeding Unfortified formula feeding |
Age 4—WPPSI (3rd edn.), sentence repetition and verbal fluency measured by NEPSY | In unadjusted analysis, children who were breastfed or fed with a fortified formula had significantly higher scores | Maternal IQ and education, social class, on benefits, age at birth, birth weight | After adjustment, the differences in IQ between groups became non-significant | A |
Ghys et al46 | Prospective cohort. Data on breastfeeding collected retrospectively | 128 full-term children in the Netherlands | Breastfeeding was expressed as the number of months of breastfeeding regardless of possible additional formula feeding | At 4 years: Dutch adaptation of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC), the Groningen Developmental Scale (GOS). And items of the motor scale of the McCarthy Scales of Children's Mental Abilities | Duration of breastfeeding showed significant correlations with cognitive development (Pearson correlation coefficient 0.26) | Plasma and RBC DHA and AA, maternal intelligence, birth weight, duration of breastfeeding and paternal educational attainment, smoking during pregnancy | In the regression analysis, the correlation disappeared | B |
Gibson-Davis and Brooks-Gunn 47 | Longitudinal birth cohort study. Breastfeeding information collected retrospectively at 1 year | 1645 American-born mothers and their babies | Breastfeeding for at least 1 month vs none | At 3 years of age: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition | In unadjusted mean comparisons, breastfed children had Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test scores that were 6.6 points higher than children who were not breastfed | An extensive set of demographic characteristics, including mother's Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test and the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment score. Mothers were categorised into one of three educational-status groups | After adjusting for demographic characteristics and maternal verbal ability, the coefficient dropped to 1.72 | A |
Gomez-Sanchiz et al48 | Prospective cohort, information on feeding collected retrospectively | 238 healthy babies born at term, non-IUGR in Spain | Formula fed, Breastfed up to 4 months, Breastfed for more than 4 months | Bayley Infant Development Scale at 24 months of age | Infants breastfed for longer than 4 months scored higher on the mental development scale than those breastfed for less time | Sociodemographic and neonatal variables including parental IQ score | The results of multiple linear regression analysis showed that infants breastfed for longer than 4 months scored 4.3 points more than those breastfed for less time | A |
Greene et al49 | Retrospective cohort | 432 participants. 208 males, 224 females in Ireland | Breastfed vs non-breastfed And Breastfed for up to 12 weeks vs more than 12 weeks |
Age: 11–16 years Raven's Standard progressive matrices test and subsets of the Primary Mental Ability Test namely verbal meaning, reasoning and number facility |
The breastfed children showed a highly significant advantage over the non-breastfed children for all measures of IQ assessed, ranging from a 4.3 point advantage in Raven's IQ to a 6.0 point advantage in Primary mental abilities IQ | Birth weight, gestational age, birth rank, child's sex, social class, mother's age and mother's educational level | Following adjustment, the beneficial effect of breastfeeding (yes vs no) was statistically non- significant. A six point advantage in verbal IQ and 5.4 point advantage in reasoning IQ were observed for participants breastfed for >I 2 weeks, compared with less | C |
Gurka et al abstract50 | Prospective cohort | 1050 children, from the National Institute of Child Health and Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development in the USA |
Never; 0–6 months; longer than 6 months |
Age: 4 years old Standardised (mean.100; SD.15) cognitive outcomes |
Significant positive associations were observed between breastfeeding and cognitive outcomes before adjusting for other factors | Maternal age, education, observed quality of the home environment, mother's attitude regarding modernity of parenting, and maternal verbal IQ | No difference between the groups | A |
Hart et al51 | Prospective cohort | 83 healthy full-term infants in Texas, USA | Exclusively breastfed vs exclusively non-breastfed | Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (BNBAS) at a mean age of 8.95 days | Breastfed infants surpassed formula-fed infants on items of orientation, motor, range of state, and state regulation dimensions of the BNBAS. Breastfed infants also exhibited fewer abnormal reflexes, signs of depression and withdrawal | Socioeconomic status | After adjustment for SES only, the differences remained significant | B |
Hoefer and Hardy9 | Retrospective cohort | 383 children in Illinois, USA | Artificially fed, breastfed for 3 months or less, from 4 to 9 months and from 10 to 20 months | Age—7–13 years. Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon intelligence test and the Pintner-Patterson performance scale (a nonverbal intelligence test), and by a group educational test, the Stanford achievements test | Infants artificially fed were inferior in all standardised measurements to those breastfed from 4 to 9 months, and, with one exception, to those breastfed 3 months or less. Those artificially fed equalled or excelled those breastfed from 10 to 20 months | none; similar paternal IQ | Adjustment was not carried out, although they mention that paternal IQ was similar between the groups | C |
Holme et al52 | A secondary analysis of data from a follow-up study of an RCT of an intervention to reduce smoking in pregnancy | 1218 children in Birmingham, UK | Not breastfed, Breastfed up to 2 months, 2–4 months, over 4 months. Also, any breastfeeding vs none |
British Ability Scales (Total IQ, Visual IQ, and verbal IQ), and Quick Neurological Screening Test (QNST) at age 9 years | Before adjustment, breastfeeding was significantly associated with higher total, verbal and visual IQ scores, and increasing duration was significantly correlated with IQ scoresBreastfeeding was associated with a crude total IQ increase of 5.49 points, which was reduced to 1.78 points on analysis with breastfeeding as a binary variable (yes/no—still significant) | Maternal demographics (including education, race and age), smoking history, work patterns, depression, social support, neonatal details and ill-health in the child | Total IQ became non-significant after adjustment | A |
Horwood and Fergusson53 | Longitudinal study—children studied at birth, 4 months, 1 year, at annual intervals to age 16, and 18 years | 1064 children born in New Zealand in 1977 | Not breastfed, breastfed for <4 months, breastfed for 4–7 months, breastfed for ≥8 months | Revised Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (total IQ)—age 8 and 9 Teachers’ rating of school performance (reading and mathematics)—age 8 and 12 Progressive Achievement Test of Reading Comprehension—ages 10 and 12 Progressive Achievement Test of Mathematics—age 11 Tests of scholastic abilities—age 13 High School Outcomes |
Increasing duration of breastfeeding was associated with consistent and statistically significant increases in cognitive abilities, and children who were breastfed for ≥8 months had mean test scores that were between 0.35 and 0.59 SD units higher (more than 5 points) than children who were bottle-fed | Maternal age, maternal education, family socioeconomic status, averaged standard of living, averaged family income, maternal smoking during pregnancy, gender, birth order, birth weight | Upon adjustment, associations were reduced and children who were breastfed for ≥8 months had scores that were 0.11–0.3 SD units (less than three points) higher than children who were bottle-fed | A |
Innis et al54 | Retrospective cohort | 433 full-term infants born in 1994 in Vancouver | Never breastfed, breastfed less than 1 month, 1–3 months, 3–6 months, 6–8 months, more than 8 months, and mixed feeding (breast and formula milk) | Visual acuity measured using acuity card procedure with Teller Acuity Cards. Cognitive development measured using the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence, (V.4.1) at 39 ±1 weeks of age | There were no differences in visual acuity or novelty preference among the infants when they were stratified by incidence or duration of breastfeeding | None | No difference | B |
Jacobson et al55 | Prospective Longitudinal study | 323 predominantly white, middle-class children born from 1980 to 1981, at age 4, and 280 children at age 11, from two cohorts of similar demographic information in Michigan, USA *Most exposed to PCB during pregnancy |
Breastfed vs not breastfed | McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised at the age of 4 Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III, Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised, and the Woodcock Word, Passage, and Reading Comprehension test at the age of 11 |
At the ages of 4 and 11, breastfeeding was significantly related to higher IQ scores | Social class, education, maternal IQ, parenting skills (Home observation for measurement of the environment (HOME)) | The relationship was reduced to non-significance after adjustment for maternal IQ and parenting skills (HOME) | B |
James56 | Prospective cohort | 38 full-term children (taken as a sample from an extended Pembrokeshire farming family, UK) | Bottle-fed babies vs breastfed babies | IQ test At age 16 (no details) | No difference in IQ | No adjustment | No difference | C |
Jedrychowski et al57 | Longitudinal study | 468 term babies in Krakow, Poland | Complementary breastfeeding (including none), vs exclusive breastfeeding up to 3 months, 4–6 months, or longer than 6 months | In the first 3 years of the follow-up, the Bayley Mental Scales of Infant Development-second edition (BSID-II) were used. At the age of 6 and 7, the Wechsler intelligence test for children (WISC-R) was administered |
Children on mixed breastfeeding achieved lower total IQ scores at each of the IQ check-ups compared with those who were exclusively breastfed | Maternal education, baby's gender, parity, and weight gain in pregnancy | Children breastfed exclusively for >6 months increased by 3.8 points (95% CI 2.11 to 5.45) | B |
Jiang et al58 | Longitudinal study | 3271 children and their mothers from the USA participating in the Child Development Supplement of the Panel Study of Income Dynamics | Yes or no ever breastfeeding and, never breastfed; less than 6 months, 7–12 months, and more than 12 months | Woodcock Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised (WJ-R) test score and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) test score at 3 and 6 years of age | Breastfed children had higher scores on WJ-R and WISC-R tests | Child's age, race and ethnicity, sex, number of siblings, whether the child was first born to the mother, whether the child was born preterm, whether the child was born SGA, HOME scale, maternal IQ, age, education, health status, insurance, marital status, working, income | Three out of the five effects remain significant; the effect sizes are smaller, with only one effect size being larger than one-fifth of the SD. Longer spells of breastfeeding are uncorrelated with increases in the measures of achievement | A |
Johnson et al59 | Longitudinal study | 204 Euro-American full-term infants were followed up to the age of three from the Galveston (Texas) area | Breastfed vs not breastfed AND Duration of breastfeeding |
At age 3 years; Stanford-Binet Fourth Edition and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised | Breastfeeding added significantly to the prediction of the Composite IQ Comprehension factor, Vocabulary, Absurdities, Memory for Sentences, and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Duration of breastfeeding only added to the prediction of Vocabulary scores | Socioeconomic status, HOME scores (parenting skills), mother's intelligence, mother's smoking behaviour, gender and birth order of the child | Breastfeeding was associated with a 4.6-point higher mean in the children's Intelligence |
A |
Keim et al- abstract60 | Prospective cohort | 347 children in the USA | Exclusively breastfed vs formula fed | Mullen Scales of Early Learning at 1 year of age | Infants exclusively breastfed demonstrated better visual reception, fine motor and overall cognitive development at 12 months than formula fed infants (4–6 points) | Preterm birth, smoking, race/ethnicity, education | Differences were weakened after adjustment. No numbers provided in the abstract | C |
Kramer et al15 | Cluster-randomised trial. | 13 889 Belarussian children born between June 1996 and December 1997 | Experimental group vs control group: Experimental group was encouraged to breastfeed and had greater levels of breastfeeding at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months (exclusive breastfeeding also sevenfold higher at 3 months) | Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence and teacher evaluations of academic performance in reading, writing, mathematics and other participants at 6.5 years of age | The experimental group had higher means on all of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence measures, with cluster-adjusted mean differences of 7.5 points for verbal IQ, 2.9 points for performance IQ, and 5.9 points for full-scale IQ. Teachers’ academic ratings were significantly higher in the experimental group for reading and writing | Maternal (and paternal) IQ, as well as all other demographic and confounding variables, should be distributed randomly between the treatment groups and should not confound the treatment effect | Cluster-adjusted mean differences of 7.5 points for verbal IQ, 2.9 points for performance IQ, and 5.9 points for full-scale IQ | A |
Lawlor et al61 | Prospective cohort | 3794 woman who delivered a singleton baby between 1981 and 1984 in Brisbane, Australia | Never, <4 months, ≥4 months | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test at the age of 5 Raven's standard progressive matrices (Raven's SPM) and the Wide Range Achievements Test V.3 (WRAT3) at 14 years | Univariate analysis of breastfeeding vs IQ showed a significant difference between the breastfeeding groups (higher scores associated with longer breastfeeding up to 8.6 points difference) | Gender, maternal age, maternal ethnicity, maternal education, paternal education, family income, gravidity, maternal smoking, fetal distress, duration of the first and second stages of labour, mode of delivery, apgar scores at 1 and 5 min, birth weight for sex and gestational age (z score), height for age and sex (z score), BMI for age and sex (z score) | Significance remained with a mean difference in IQ of 6.8 between never and over 4 months of breastfeeding | A |
Lucas et al62 | A prospective, double-blind RCT (different formulas, breastfed control group not randomised) | 447 healthy full-term children born in the UK between 1993 and 1995 | Breastfed for at least 6 weeks vs formula fed | BSID-II at 18 months | No differences in overall developmental scores at 9 months or 18 months or in any subscale quotient at 9 months were found | Sex, centre, maternal age, maternal education, maternal marital status, and social class | No difference with adjustment for potential confounding factors | A |
Maimaitiming and Wang63 | Retrospective cohort | 442 infants and children inhabited by Uygur, Han or Kazak nationality in West China | Breastfed vs mixed feeding | Denver Developmental Screening Test at age up to 3 | There were no differences in scores between breastfeeding vs mixed feeding groups | None | No difference | C |
Makrides et al64 | A prospective, randomised, double-blind controlled. Different formulas. Breastfed group was used as control, but not randomised | 68 formula-fed infants and 46 breastfed infants born at term of appropriate weight in Australia | Formula fed vs breastfed | Infant VEP acuity at 16 and 34 weeks Bayley's Scales of Development at 1 and 2 years |
At 1 year of age, MDI scores of breastfed and formula-fed infants were not different, but at 2 years of age, MDI scores of breastfed infants were higher than those of formula fed infants. PDI scores were similar | Home screening questionnaire scores, occupational prestige, parental education, gender, birth weight, maternal smoking, birth order, feeding mode | MDI score was higher at age 2 in the breastfed group. 95% CI 3 to 16.8 | A |
Malloy and Berendes65 | Retrospective cohort | 518 children born in 1978–1979 in Washington DC who were exposed to chloride-deficient formulas | No breastfeeding (176) vs any time length of breastfeeding (342) | Weschler Intelligence Scale-Revised at 9 or 10 years of age | Breastfed scored significantly higher on full-scale IQ; Further analyses limited to those exclusively breastfed for the first 60 days failed to demonstrate any significant relationship | Maternal education, paternal education and annual income | Following adjustment, the difference was not significant | B |
Martin et al66 | Retrospective cohort | 1431 children (twin siblings) from the greater Brisbane area | Exclusively formula fed, any breastfeeding between birth and 3 months, exclusively breastfed for 3–6 months, exclusively breastfed for 6 months or more | FSIQ (full-scale IQ) assessed using the Multidimensional Aptitude Battery (MAB) at 16 years of age | Breastfeeding was significantly associated with FSIQ scores. No effect of duration of breastfeeding on FSIQ was found | Socioeconomic status, paternal education, maternal education, gestational age and birth weight | The effect was no longer significant after adjustment | A |
McCrory and Layte67 | Retrospective cross-sectional study | 8568 school children in Ireland born between 1997 and 1998 | Breastfed vs not breastfed AND Never breastfed, ≤5 weeks, 6–15 weeks, 16–25 weeks, 26+ weeks |
Age 9 years. Vocabulary component of the Drumcondra Primary Reading Test-Revised and part 1 of the Drumcondra Primary Mathematics Test-Revised | In unadjusted analysis, children who were breastfed scored 8.67 percentage points higher on reading and 7.42 percentage points higher on maths. Evidence of dose–response relationship was weak | Gender, birth weight, gestation period, NICU, primary and secondary carer's social class, primary carer's education level, household income, mother's age at birth, Irish/Non-Irish, number of children's books in the home | After adjustment, remained significant but weakened: 3.24 and 2.23 percentage points in reading and maths, respectively | A |
Morales et al68 | Retrospective cohort analysing polymorphisms in genes encoding enzymes involved in LC-PUFA synthesis | Two population-based birth cohorts n=400 mother-child pairs from INMA-Sabadell; and n=340 children from INMA-Menorca in Spain | Different types of formula fed vs breastfed | Mental development was assessed at age 14 months using the BSID, first Edition—MDI only, and at age 4 years by the Spanish version of the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (MCSA) | Children with variants associated with lower synthesis of LC-PUFA had higher scores when breastfed, while those with greater capacity to synthesise these fatty acids had higher scores regardless of breastfeeding practices | Sex, child age, psychologist, quality of neuropsychological test, maternal education, breastfeeding, and use of gas stove at home | Not being breastfed conferred an 8-point to 9-point disadvantage in cognition among children with low FADS1 activity and a 5–8-point disadvantage in cognition among children with low ELOVL5 activity | A |
Morris et al69 | Prospective cohort | 102 normal birth weight children born in Brazil at term | Number of breastfeeds per day (recorded daily) | BSID at 6 months and 12 months (MDI and PDI) | Breastfeeding frequency over the first 4 weeks of life, but not later, was significantly associated with mental development (MDI) at 6 months of age | Socioeconomic data: family income, a household resources index, a housing quality index, a water and sanitation index, and maternal and paternal literacy | An average of one extra breastfeed per day resulted in an increase of approximately one-quarter of a point. The effect was no longer apparent at 12 months of age | B |
Morrow-Tlucak et al70 | Prospective cohort | 229 children born in Ohio between 1981 and 1982 | No breastfeeding, breastfeeding=<4 months, breastfeeding >4 months | BSID—MDI at 6 months, 1 year and 2 years of age | Significant differences in MDI scores between the three groups (longer breastfeeding=higher scores) at 1 and 2 years of age (no significant difference at 6 months) | Parent education (mean of both parents), maternal attitude (Authoritarian Family Ideology), maternal intelligence (PPVT-R), cigarette use, maternal age, rage, marital status, Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) at age 1, HOME at age 2, exact age at time of testing | With covariate control, a small but significant relationship between duration of breastfeeding and Bayley MDI at 1 and 2 years was detected. Infants breastfed for 4 months or more scored on average nine points higher compared with the bottle-fed infants | A |
Mortensen et al71 | Prospective longitudinal birth cohort | Mixed sample: 973 men and women All-male sample: 2280 men in Denmark |
Divided into five groups: <1 month 2–3 months 4–6 months 7–9 months >9 months | Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) at a mean age of 27.2 years in the mixed-sex sample. Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) test at a mean age of 18.7 years in the all-male sample |
Duration of breastfeeding was associated with significantly higher scores on the Verbal, Performance, Full Scale WAIS IQs and BPP test | Parental social status Parental education Single mother status Mother's height, age and weight gain during pregnancy cigarette consumption during the third trimester number of pregnancies Gestational age birth weight Birth length Indexes of pregnancy Delivery complications |
The results remained significant with 4.6 points higher IQ for those breast-fed over 9 months compared with those breastfed for less than 1 month | B |
Mukerji et al72 | Cross-sectional study | 100 children aged 0–3 years in India | No breastfeeding or mixed. Exclusive breastfeeding for 4–9 months. Exclusive breastfeeding for more than 9 months |
Developmental Screening Test (DST) | In the 4–9 months exclusive breastfed group, 100% had average or above average IQ. In the two other groups this number was significantly lower |
None | No adjustments were made | C |
Nassar et al73 | Cross-sectional study | 42 healthy infants in Cairo, Egypt | Breastfed, artificially fed, mixed feeding | Bayley scale of infant development-second edition (BSID-II) between 4 and 6 months of age | No difference in MDI or PDI. There was significant difference only in the total behaviour rating scale (TBRS) and motor quality centile rank | No significant difference between groups in terms of age, sex, and socioeconomic standard (indirectly adjusted for with multiple regression) | Significant increase in mean adjusted TBRS and motor quality centile rank | B |
Nelson et al74 | Prospective cohort | Term gestation infants in USA | Breastfed or formula fed for 3 months | Teller Acuity Cards at 14 days, 3, 4, 8, and 18 months Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence at 8, 10 and 12 months BSID at 4, 8 and 18 months |
There were no significant differences between breastfed and formula-fed infants in visual acuity at 14 days, 3, 8 or 18 months, or recognition memory or the Bayley PDI or MDI at any age | Meeting abstract only available | C | |
Niemela and Jarvenpaa 75 | Prospective follow-up | 726 children born between 1985 and 1986 in Finland | Breastfed <5 months, breastfed >5 months (matched pairwise based on maternal education and sex) | Non-verbal Columbian Mental Maturity Scale (CMM), visual integration using the Beery test and active vocabulary by naming of pictures at 56 months | Children breastfed for 5 months or more attained higher scores in developmental tests (significance difference found in relation to the general cognitive capacity and visual motor integration). No evidence of any interaction between verbal development and breastfeeding to 5 months or more | Groups matched pairwise based on sex and maternal education | Maternal education and parental status correlated with all cognitive scores. Sex and breastfeeding correlated with scores of the general cognitive capacity and visual motor integration tests in multiple linear regressions | B |
Oddy et al76 | Prospective cohort | 1401 children at first follow-up and 1283 children at second follow-up from the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort Study following 2860 children in Perth, Australia | Never breastfed, fully breastfed 0–4 months, 4–6 months, more than 6 months | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) for receptive English vocabulary—verbal intelligence at 6 years Performance subtest by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (WISC-III)—Block Design Test at age 8 years |
On average, children breastfed for more than 6 months had mean verbal IQ scores that were 644 points higher and Block Design scores that were 1.13 points higher than children never breastfed (small but significant differences) | Gestational age, maternal age, maternal education, parental smoking and older siblings (all covariates that were significantly correlated with verbal IQ and the performance subtest or breastfeeding) | Breastfeeding for >6 months was associated with an increase in verbal IQ of 3.56 points. The Performance subtest was weakened and was no longer a significant difference after adjustment | A |
Oddy et al 77 | Prospective cohort | 980 children from the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort Study following 2860 children in Perth, Australia | Breastfed less than 4 vs more than 4 months and breastfed less than 6 vs more than 6 months | Western Australian Monitoring Standards in Education (WAMSE) scores in: maths, reading, writing and spelling at age 10 years | Continuous breastfeeding was significantly associated with an increase in scores with each additional month of breastfeeding for maths, reading, writing and spelling | Gender, maternal age, maternal education, family income, marital status, parent looks at book with child at age 5, maternal country of birth | Results were attenuated when adjusted for confounders. Significant interactions were found in maths and spelling, revealing that boys were more likely to have improved academic scores if breastfed for a longer period | A |
Paine et al78 | Retrospective cohort | 96 healthy full-term Caucasian children from the Adelaide area in Australia | Duration of exclusive breastfeeding | Bayley Scales of Infant Development at age 10–14 months | Duration of exclusive breastfeeding significantly predicted mental development scores for boys, but not for girls. Duration of breast-feeding did not predict psychomotor development scores | Duration of breastfeeding, parents’ occupational prestige, parents’ education level and smoking habits, number of siblings, birth order, HSQ score, gestational age, birth weight, age of testing, maternal age and gender were considered as possible independent variables—independent variables with p<0.02 included in the model—gender, maternal age, birth weight and duration of breastfeeding | None | C |
Pollock79 | Prospective cohort | 3838 children from the 1970 British Births Survey at full term and healthy birth weight | Wholly breastfed for more than 3 months vs wholly bottle-fed | Human figure drawing score, copying design score and English Picture Vocabulary Test score at age 5. Dichotomised outcomes: Pictorial Language Test, Friendly Maths test, Edinburgh Reading test, Spelling Test, British Ability Scales (Word definitions, similarities, matrices, total) and Continuous outcomes: Pictorial Language test, Friendly Maths test, Edinburgh Reading test, Spelling test, and British Ability Scales (Word definitions, similarities, matrices, total) at age 10 | Significant difference found between groups at age 5 for the English Picture Vocabulary test. Significant difference found for dichotomised outcomes and continuous outcomes for the British Ability Scales and dichotomised outcomes for the Pictorial Language test at age 10 |
Age father left full time education, age mother left full time education, highest educational qualifications of mother, mother's smoking behaviour during pregnancy, antenatal labour preparation class attendance, infant's place of birth | All results adjusted for all other independent predictors of breastfeeding: English Picture Vocabulary test: aOR 1.50. Dichotomised outcomes: Pictorial Language test aOR 1.49 Word definitions aOR 1.55 Similarities aOR 1.64 Total aOR 1.64 Continuous outcomes: Word definitions mean diff 3.5 Matrices mean diff 2.6 Similarities mean difference 3.0 Total mean difference 3.1 |
A |
Quinn et al80 | Prospective cohort | 3880 healthy children from the Mater Hospital-University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy project | Never breastfed, <3 weeks, 3–7 weeks, 7 weeks—4 months, 4–6 months, still breast feeding at 6 months | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Revised (PPVT-R) at 5 years of age | Significantly increasing scores were found between duration of breastfeeding and the PPVT-R scores | Birth weight, poverty, maternal education, maternal age, time in daycare/preschool, number of children in household at 5 years, English speaking background for mother and father, and degree of infant stimulation | After adjusting for a wide range of biological and social factors, the adjusted mean for those breastfed for 6 months or more was 8.2 points higher for females and 5.8 higher for males when compared with those never breastfed (this was significant) | A |
Rao et al81 | Prospective cohort | 299 children born in Norway/Sweden at appropriate size for gestational age (comparison group) | <12 weeks vs >12 weeks of breastfeeding AND Duration of breastfeeding as a continuum |
BSID at 13 months of age Norwegian version of the WPPSI-R at 5 years. of age Peabody Development Motor Scale measured at 5 years of age |
There were statistically significant differences in IQ between the 2-breastfeeding groups | Site of enrolment, maternal education, maternal IQ, maternal smoking, admission to a neonatal intensive care unit, kindergarten attendance, gender and asymmetric intrauterine growth retardation | 3.7 points for total IQ and 4.1 points for performance IQ. Results remained unaltered when adjusted for confounding variables | B |
Ribas-Fito et al82 | Prospective cohort | 391 children born in Spain between 1997 and 1999 | 0–2 weeks breastfeeding 2–20 weeks breastfeeding 20+ weeks breastfeeding | Spanish version of the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (general cognitive scale, verbal scale, perceptual-performance scale, memory scale, quantitative scale, motor scale subsets) at 4 years of age | Children with longer periods of breastfeeding performed significantly better on the McCarthy cognitive scale (except motor) | Gender, academic trimester at examination, psychologist, maternal social class, maternal education, and maternal use of alcohol and tobacco during pregnancy | After adjustment for confounders, significance remained for the general cognitive scale, and a trend remained in other subsets | A |
Richards et al83 | Retrospective cohort | 1741 people from the MRC National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD), also known as the British 1946 birth cohort | Never Up to 2 months 2–7 months 7+ months |
National Adult Reading Test (NART), word-list learning task (verbal memory) and timed letter search task (mental speed and concentration) at age 53 | Educational Attainment: odds of obtaining higher qualifications by the age of 26 were statistically significant, with greater odds for longer breastfeeding Cognitive Function: regression coefficients for the unadjusted effect show that breastfeeding was significantly associated with the NART and verbal memory tests |
Early background variables: Sex, father's social class, mother's education, birth order, parental interest in education and material home conditions |
Effect of BF on education was strengthened after adjusting for the early background variables and remained significant. No longer significant with further adjustment for cognitive ability at age 15 Cognitive function: Coefficients were reduced after adjusting for the early background variables and were further reduced after adjusting for educational attainment and adult social class Only NART remained significance |
A |
Richards et al84 | Retrospective cohort | 511 first-born offspring of the British 1946 cohort and their parent | Duration of breastfeeding | Sentence completion test, reading test and vocabulary test at age 8 years | Breastfeeding was positively associated with cognitive function at age 8 in the first offspring of a national birth cohort. This association was not evident in the subsample of mothers of these offspring. Association in the offspring cohort became non-significant after adjusting for social class, maternal education or maternal cognitive performance | Social class, parental educational attainment, material home conditions, maternal age at birth, birth order, family size, maternal cigarette smoking, parental interest in education, attendance at nursery school, whether offspring cohort members had been taught cognitive skills at age 4, and cognitive test scores of the mothers | Non-significant; after adjusting for social class, maternal education or maternal cognitive performance | C |
Rodgers85 | Longitudinal study | 1464 children at age 8 and 1398 children at age 15 from the British 1946 cohort | Never bottle-fed vs never breastfed | Picture intelligence test and word reading test at 8 years of age Non-verbal ability, mathematics and sentence completion tests at age 15 years of age |
A preliminary analysis indicated that low scores were more likely for those who had been bottle-fed than breastfed. The mean sentence completion scores between the two groups are statistically significant. Every test at both ages was significant except for word reading scores after correction for background factors | Sex, social group, parental interest in education, material home conditions, sample stratification, father's education, mother's education, family size, birth rank, age at weaning | After correction for confounders, every test at both ages was significant except for word reading scores | B |
Rogan and Gladen86 | Prospective cohort | 855 newborns being followed in North Carolina were enrolled between 1978 and 1982 and followed up to 5 years old | Bottle-fed ‘short breastfeeding’ ‘medium breastfeeding’ ‘long breastfeeding’ ‘very long breastfeeding’ |
Bayley Mental and PDI at 6, 12, 18 and 24 months of age. McCarthy General Cognitive, Verbal, Quantitative, Memory, Perceptual Performance and Motor scales at 36, 48, and 60 months of age Report Card Grades from 3rd, 4th and 5th grade (averaged) | Bayley Mental and Psychomotor: After adjusting for covariables, the results at all four time points were similar and differences among the groups were only statistically significance at 24 months McCarthy: All scales showed trends towards higher scores with increasing length of breastfeeding, but the relationship was weakest for the motor scale. Differences after adjustment were only significant at 3 and 4 years (marginally at 5 years) between length of breastfeeding groups Report Cards: Showed slight increase with breastfeeding. Marginally significant after adjustment for English scores and not significant for maths |
Age, race, occupation, education, smoking, drinking, child's sex, birth weight, number of older siblings, prenatal PCB exposure and dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethene exposure, identity of the examiner | Confounders integrated into model; unadjusted results not shown | B |
Santiago Burruchaga et al87 | Prospective cohort | 39 children born at term and from homogeneous sociocultural status in Spain | Breastfed for at least 2 months vs formula fed | Bayley's scale at 2 years of age | No statistically significant differences between groups were found in cognitive function | Maternal age, level of education, occupation, number of children in the family, smoking | None; confounders integrated into model | C |
Silva et al88 | Longitudinal population-based cohort | 9367 children from the 1970 British Cohort Study comprising individuals born during April 5–11 in the UK | Never breastfed Less than 1 month 1–3 months More than 3 months |
British Ability Scale, the Shortened Edinburgh Reading Test (word recognition), the Friendly Math Tests, and the Pictorial Language Comprehension Test at the age of 10 years | Breastfeeding showed a positive association with cognition at 10 years before adjustment | Socioeconomic class, birth weight, parity, gestational age, maternal age and maternal smoking | Breastfeeding was weakly associated with cognitive function after adjustment (standardised coefficient 0.07). However, this effect was much smaller in the structural equation model adjusting for the same variables and did not reach significance level, suggesting that it is of little clinical importance | A |
Silva et al89 | Retrospective cohort | 1037 children from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development Study | <1 week 1–4 weeks 5–12 weeks 13–24 weeks 25–36 weeks 37–51 weeks 51+ weeks |
Gross motor co-ordination, fine motor co-ordination, verbal comprehension and verbal expression, intelligence, child behaviour problems at age 3 years | Comparison of the groups resulted in only one significant difference among 96 comparisons made. No significant differences in age of attainment of milestones, gross or fine motor ability scores, verbal comprehension or expression, ability, intelligence, the incidence of separation problems, hyperactivity, height, weight and head circumference | Socioeconomic class, general mental ability (IQ), level of education Pairwise comparison with group matching to account for confounders |
None; pairwise matching said to account for confounders | B |
Sloan et al90 | Cross-sectional observational. Breastfeeding data were obtained retrospectively | 137 infants and mothers in Ireland | Breastfed (defined as more than 1 month) vs not breastfed (defined as less than a month or not at all) | BSID (second edition; Bayley, 1993), between 10 and 18 months of age | Mean cognitive scores were significantly higher in breast-fed infants (110) compared with formula-fed infants (105). Breastfeeding duration had a positive linear association with cognitive scores at 1 year | Home environment (HOME score) and infant gender | The adjusted standardised β for breastfeeding was 0.285, which remained significant after adjustment | C |
Slykerman et al91 | Cross-sectional observational. Breastfeeding data were obtained retrospectively | 550 infants, 50% of which were SGA at birth and 50% AGA in New Zealand | Not at all, less than 6 months, 6–12 months, >12 months | Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition at age 3.5–4 years |
Breastfeeding was not significantly related to intelligence scores in the AGA group | Examiner administering the intelligence test, gestation, gender, maternal education, marital status, parental occupation, maternal age, parity and smoking | No difference in IQ | B |
Steer et al92 | Prospective observational. Breastfeeding data were obtained prospectively | 9656 children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children cohort, in the UK | Breastfeeding within the first month of life vs never breastfeeding | Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 8 years old | Breastfeeding showed a strong association with full-scale IQ with breastfed children scoring 8 points higher IQ on average in unadjusted analyses | Maternal education, paternal social class, low birthweight, preterm gestation, home environment, parenting and gender | The breastfeeding effect attenuated to a 3-point advantage after adjustment | B |
Taylor and Wadsworth93 | Longitudinal population-based cohort. Breastfeeding data were obtained retrospectively | 13 135 children from The Child Health and Education Study in the UK | Never breastfed Less than 1 month 1–3 months More than 3 months |
English picture vocabulary test (EPVT) adapted from the American Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, at 5 years of age | Children breast-fed for three or more months scoring over one-quarter of the SD above the norm | The age of the child at testing; the child's sex and birth weight; whether there were older or younger siblings in the home when the study child was five years old; home furnishings and equipment; maternal age at the child's birth; maternal smoking and the social index | Breastfeeding remained a significant influence on EPVT scores, but the difference between the groups was small: +0.12 in the standardised EPVT score | A |
Temboury et al94 | Prospective cohort | 229 infants in Spain | Breastfed—at least 3 months Bottle-fed—none or less than 1 month |
Bayley's scale at age 18–29 months | Bottle-fed infants had lower IMD scores (index of mental development) | Maternal age, education and social class, job, psychosocial risk, number of children, infants’ shyness, tantrum, hyperactivity, gender, birth weight, height, place of birth | The result remained significant | C |
Thorsdottir et al95 | Longitudinal cohort | 85 children in Iceland | Duration of exclusive breastfeeding | The Icelandic developmental inventory at age 6 years. Information collected from the mothers | Duration of exclusive breastfeeding, in months, was positively related to children's total developmental index | Maternal and paternal education and family income, birth weight, maternal BMI | Total developmental index (B=1.07±0.5, p=0.04) | C |
Tozzi et al96 | Prospective cohort | 1403 children originally included in an Italian clinical trial on acellular Pertussis vaccines |
Duration of exclusive breastfeeding both as a continuous variable and as a categorical variable (<6 months, >6 months) | An estimated IQ was obtained from scores of the vocabulary, similarities, block design and coding tests at 10–12 years of age An estimation of IQ was obtained from the scores of the four WISC-R subtests |
Scores of only a few neuropsychological tests were affected by exclusive breastfeeding duration: Mean scores on vocabulary, similarities, the Boston naming test and estimated IQ improved with the duration of breastfeeding, whereas performance in one of the subtests for writing praxis decreased with breastfeeding duration | Sex, birth weight, gestational age, mother's age at birth, type of delivery, family composition, parents’ education, presence of chronic diseases, current prescription of antihistamines or antiepileptic drugs, and the amount of thimerosal to which the children were exposed through vaccines | In the regression analysis, the score on one subcategory of the California verbal learning test was negatively associated with breastfeeding for longer than 6 months No difference was detected in any of the other test scores included in the analysis |
A |
Veena et al97 | Longitudinal cohort | 514 children from the Mysore Parthenon birth cohort in south India | Six categories from <3 to ≥18 months | Kaufman Assessment Battery at 9-year-old to 10-year-old children | Within this cohort, in which prolonged breastfeeding was the norm (90% breastfed ≥6 months), there were no associations between longer duration of breastfeeding and cognitive function | Age, sex, gestation, birth size, maternal age, parity, socioeconomic status, parents’ attained schooling and rural/urban residence | No difference either unadjusted or after adjustment | B |
Whitehouse et al98 | Longitudinal cohort, breastfeeding data collected prospectively | 1195 live born children recruited at approximately 18 weeks’ gestation, Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study | (1) Never breastfed, (2) Breastfed predominantly for <4 months, (3) Breastfed predominantly for 4–6 months (4) Breastfed predominantly for >6 months |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised (PPVT-R) at age 10 years. Raw scores are transformed to standard scores, based around a mean of 100 and an SD of 15 | Strong positive association between the duration of predominant breastfeeding and PPVT-R at age 10 years | Maternal age at conception, maternal education, family income and the presence of the biological father in the family home, maternal smoking and alcohol consumption during pregnancy, maternal experience of stressful events during pregnancy, parity, gestational age, child's sex and proportion of optimal birth weight, a measure of the appropriateness of fetal growth, family functioning (MMFAD), Parenting Scale, language-learning environment |
Children who were predominantly breastfed for >6 months had a mean PPVT-R score that was 4.04 points higher than that of children who were never breastfed | A |
Wigg et al99 | Longitudinal cohort, breastfeeding data collected prospectively | 375 children born in Port Pirie, South Australia | At 6 month of age: breastfed vs bottle-fed Children who were breastfed for less than 6 months would have been classified in our analysis as bottle-fed |
BSID at age 2, the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities age 4 and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children at age 7, 11, 13 | Estimated unadjusted advantages for the breastfed children at ages 2, 4 and 7 years were 5.5 points, 4.6 points and 4.3 points in the MDI, GCI and IQ scores, respectively At age 11–13 years, the unadjusted advantage for the breastfed children was 3.8 points in IQ |
Gender, maternal age at delivery, socioeconomic status, HOME score, maternal IQ, parental smoking habits, birth weight, birth rank, lifetime average blood lead concentration and whether parents were living together | Adjusting for the covariates diminished the association of the feeding method in infancy with cognitive development. The covariates contributing most to this attenuating effect were the HOME scores, maternal IQ, socioeconomic status and parental smoking habits |
B |
Zaini et al100 | Retrospective cohort | 1397 children from Selangor Malaysia | <6 months >6 months |
Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices at a mean age 9.6 years | Those who were breastfed less than 6 months performed better (31.01 vs 30.63 of max 36) | None | None | C |
Zhou et al101 | Prospective cohort | 302 children born between 1998 and 1999 in Adelaide, Australia | Not breastfed, breastfed at hospital discharge, less than 6 months, more than 6 months | Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale at 4 years of age | There was no association between duration of breastfeeding and childhood IQ in this relatively well-nourished cohort from an industrialised society. Before adjustment, children who were breastfed for at least 6 months had a higher IQ than those who were breastfed for less than 6 months | Birth order and sex of the child, maternal smoking in pregnancy, parental education, parental occupation and quality of home environment | There was no association between breastfeeding and childhood IQ | B |
BF, breastfeeding; BMI, body mass index; LC-PUFA, long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids; RCT, randomised controlled trial.