Table 7.
Predictor | b | t | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Reading comprehension | ||||
1. Direct effect of late talking (model fit) | 12.8 | .000 | ||
Late talking statusa | -15.52 | 3.7 | .000 | |
2. Mediator (oral language at 8) added as | ||||
predictor (model fit) | 105.5 | .000 | ||
Oral language at 8 | 3.92 | 13.4 | .000 | |
Late talking statusa | -6.35 | 2.2 | .031 | |
3. Late talking status as predictor of oral language at | ||||
8 (model fit) | 8.4 | .004 | ||
Late talking statusa | -2.34 | 2.9 | .004 | |
Broad math | ||||
1. Direct effect of late talking (model fit) | 10.6 | .001 | ||
Late talking statusa | -13.38 | 3.3 | .001 | |
2. Mediator (oral language at 8) added as | ||||
predictor (model fit) | 69.0 | .000 | ||
Oral language at 8 | 4.10 | 11.0 | .000 | |
Late talking statusa | -4.33 | 1.3 | .194 | |
3. Late talking as a predictor of oral language at 8 | ||||
(model fit) | 12.8 | .000 | ||
Late talking statusa | -2.21 | 3.5 | .000 |
Notes. The reading comprehension measure was the Passage Comprehension subtest standard score, and the math ability measure was the Broad Math subtest standard score, both of the Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery, Revised (WJ-R). Oral language ability at age 8 was the sum of z-scores for Picture Vocabulary and Memory for Sentences subtests of the WJ-R, the Word Definitions subtest of the WASI, and the narrative total score from the SECCYD.
Late talking defined by an indicator variable for no word combinations at age 2 versus combining words at age 2.