Abstract
The negative impact of consuming sugar-sweetened beverages on weight and other health outcomes has been increasingly recognized; therefore, many people have turned to high-intensity sweeteners like aspartame, sucralose, and saccharin as a way to reduce the risk of these consequences. However, accumulating evidence suggests that frequent consumers of these sugar substitutes may also be at increased risk of excessive weight gain, metabolic syndrome, type 2 diabetes, and cardiovascular disease. This paper discusses these findings and considers the hypothesis that consuming sweet-tasting but noncaloric or reduced-calorie food and beverages interferes with learned responses that normally contribute to glucose and energy homeostasis. Because of this interference, frequent consumption of high-intensity sweeteners may have the counterintuitive effect of inducing metabolic derangements.
Keywords: obesity, diabetes, sweeteners
Sweeteners and health
Consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB; see Glossary) has been increasingly associated with negative health outcomes such as being overweight, obesity, type 2 diabetes (T2D), and metabolic syndrome, for reviews, see [1–5]. Based largely on these associations, many researchers and healthcare practitioners have proposed that non-caloric, high-intensity sweeteners provide a beneficial alternative in foods and beverages [6–10]. There is no doubt that replacing caloric with noncaloric sweeteners reduces the energy density of foods and beverages. However, whether reducing energy density in this manner always translates into reduced energy intake, lower body weight, and improved metabolic health is much less certain. Recent reviews of studies spanning at least the past 40 years have concluded that high-intensity sweeteners are potentially helpful [11], harmful [12], or have as yet unclear effects [9,13–15] with regard to regulation of energy balance or other metabolic consequences. One purpose of this opinion paper is to summarize and evaluate recent research that is consistent with the rather counterintuitive claim that consuming high-intensity sweeteners may promote excess energy intake, increased body weight, and other related co-morbidities. A second goal is to identify and examine the types of physiological mechanisms that could underlie such adverse health consequences. A third aim is to consider factors that can make studies into the effects of artificial sweeteners on energy and body weight regulation difficult to interpret.
Use of high-intensity sweeteners and artificially sweetened beverages
For the present purposes, the terms high-intensity sweeteners, low-calorie sweeteners, artificial sweeteners, and artificially sweetened beverages (ASB) have much the same meaning and are used interchangeably. Consumption and availability of artificial sweeteners have been increasing and in the USA approximately 30% of adults and 15% of children aged 2–17 years reported consumption of low-calorie sweeteners in 2007–2008 [16]. Consumption of ASB and SSB has increased between 1962 and 2000 in the USA and shows parallels with changes in the prevalence of being overweight and obesity over the same time frame (Figure 1). Consumption of ASB has also risen along with rates of obesity in Australia, whereas consumption of SSB has declined [17].
Prospective cohort studies of effects of ASB consumption
Weight gain
The San Antonio Heart Study documented weight change in men and women over a 7–8-year period. As part of that study, Fowler et al. [18] reported that, among participants who were normal weight or overweight at baseline, risk of weight gain and obesity were significantly greater in those consuming ASB compared with those who did not consume ASB [18] (Table 1). In a study of two adolescent cohorts, ASB intake was associated with increased body mass index (BMI) and increased body fat percentage in males and females at 2-year follow-up [19] when data were examined cross-sectionally, but not in a longitudinal analysis. In that study, SSB intake was associated with increased BMI in males only in the longitudinal analysis, whereas there were no increased risks for increased BMI or increased body fat percentage associated with SSB in females. Differences in outcome between these adolescents and the Fowler et al. study could reflect smaller sample sizes, younger subjects, and/or a shorter follow-up time frame. However, neither study provided evidence that ASB consumption was associated with reduced risk for either weight gain or increased body fat percentage [18,19].
Table 1.
Study | Sample | Length | Effect | ASB volume Risk estimatea | Sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) volume Risk estimatea | ASB after adjustmentb | SSB after adjustmentb | Refs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
San Antonio Heart | 1250 men and womenc | 7–8 years | BMI ≥25 | <3 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 1.56 |
N/A | [18] |
San Antonio Heart | 1250 men and womenc | 7–8 years | BMI ≥25 | 3–10 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 1.74 |
N/A | [18] |
San Antonio Heart | 1250 men and womenc | 7–8 years | BMI ≥25 | 11–21 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 1.75 |
N/A | [18] |
San Antonio Heart | 1250 men and womenc | 7–8 years | BMI ≥25 | ≥22 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 1.93 |
N/A | [18] |
San Antonio Heart | 2571 men and womend | 7–8 years | BMI ≥30 | 11–21 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 1.73 |
N/A | [18] |
San Antonio Heart | 2571 men and womend | 7–8 years | BMI ≥30 | ≥22 per week | N/A | Yes OR = 2.03 |
N/A | [18] |
IDEA/ECHO | 327 male adolescentse | 24 months | BMIf | Continuous | Continuousg | N/A | N/A | [19] |
IDEA/ECHO | 339 female adolescentse | 24 months | BMIf | Continuous | No | N/A | N/A | [19] |
CARDIA | 4161 men and women | 20 years | Metabolic syndromeh | Any | N/A | Yes HR = 1.23 |
N/A | [6] |
Framingham Offspring | 6039 men and women | 4 years | Metabolic syndrome | ≥1 per day OR = 1.53 |
≥1 per day OR = 1.62 |
N/A | N/A | [20] |
ARIC | 9514 men and women | 9 years | Metabolic syndrome | Highest tertilei HR = 1.34 |
Highest tertilei HR = 1.09 |
N/A | N/A | [21] |
MESA | 5011 men and women | 2–5 years | Metabolic syndrome | ≥1 per day HR = 1.31 |
N/A | Yesj HR = 1.17 |
N/A | [22] |
MESA | 5011 men and women | 2–5 years | T2Dh | ≥1 per day HR = 1.63 |
N/A | Yes HR = 1.38 |
N/A | [22] |
E3N | 66 118 women | 14 years | T2D | ≥603 ml per week HR = 3.50 |
≥359 ml per week HR = 1.49 |
Yes HR = 1.68 |
Yes HR = 1.30 |
[23] |
HPFS | 40 389 men | 20 years | T2D | 4.5 per week to18 per day HR = 1.91 |
4.5 per week to 7.5 per day HR = 1.25 |
No HR = 1.09 |
Yes HR = 1.24 |
[24] |
HPFS | 39 059 men | 22 years | T2D | ≥1 per day HR = 1.95l HR = 1.87m |
≥1 per day HR = 1.57l HR = 1.49m |
Nok HR = 1.15l HR = 1.06m |
Yes HR = 1.37l HR = 1.33m |
[25] |
NHS | 74 749 women | 24 years | T2D | ≥1 per day HR = 1.76l HR = 1.59m |
≥1 per day HR = 1.46l HR = 1.74m |
Yesn HR = 1.09l HR = 1.01m |
Yes HR = 1.20l HR = 1.29m |
[25] |
EPIC-InterAct | 15 374 men and women | 16 years | T2D | ≥1 per day HR = 1.84 |
≥1 per day HR = 1.68 |
No HR = 1.13 Yeso HRo = 1.43 |
Yes HR = 1.29 |
[26] |
NHS | 88 520 women | 24 years | CHDp | ≥2 per day HR = 1.28 |
≥2 per day HR = 1.93 |
Noq HR = 1.15 |
Yes HR = 1.35 |
[27] |
HPFS | 42 883 men | 22 years | CHDr | 4.5 per week to 18 per day HR = 1.04 |
4.5 per week to 7.5 per day HR = 1.21 |
No HR = 1.02 |
Yes HR = 1.20 |
[28] |
NHS-I | 88 540 women | 38 years | Hypertension | ≥1 per day HR = 1.38 |
≥1 per day HR = 1.22 |
Yes HR = 1.11 |
Yes HR = 1.12 |
[29] |
NHS-II | 97 991 women | 16 years | Hypertension | [29] | ||||
≥1 per day HR = 1.56 |
≥1 per day HR = 1.39 |
Yes HR = 1.12 |
Yes HR = 1.17 |
|||||
HPFS | 37 360 men | 22 years | Hypertension | ≥1 per day HR = 1.43 |
≥1 per day HR = 1.09 |
Yes HR = 1.20 |
No HR = 1.06 |
[29] |
Northern Manhattan | 2564 men and women | 10 years | Vascular eventss | ≥1 per day HR = 1.66 |
≥1 per day HR = 1.15 |
Yes HR = 1.44 HR = 1.59t |
No HR = 1.09 HR = 1.57t |
[30] |
Statistically significant increases in consumers relative to non-consumers. Hazard ratios (HR) and odds ratios (OR) listed in this column are from the least-adjusted models that did not include baseline body mass index (BMI) as a factor. Not all studies included result from models that did not adjust for BMI. The comparison group for the ratios is non-consumers of that beverage type.
Whether this effect was statistically significant in models that did include baseline BMI as a factor. The HR and OR listed are from models that were the most fully adjusted reported in that study for which BMI was included. Not all studies included models that adjusted for BMI.
Normal weight.
Overweight.
In grades 6–11.
Cross-sectionally but not longitudinally.
Longitudinally but not cross-sectionally.
Also jncreased waist circumference.
Not otherwise defined.
P = 0.06.
For caffeine-free ASB, P = 0.06.
Caffeine-free.
Caffeinated.
For caffeine-free ASB but not caffeinated ASB.
Among normal-weight participants but not those overweight or obese.
Nonfatal myocardial infarction (MI) or coronary heart disease death.
P = 0.07.
P = 0.05.
Stroke, MI, or vascular death.
When only those who had a baseline BMI <30 and no history of diabetes or metabolic syndrome were analyzed. Abbreviations: ARIC, Atherosclerosis Riskin Communities Study; CARDIA, Coronary Artery Risk Developmentin Young Adults; CHD, coronary heart disease; E3N, Etude Epidémiologique auprès des femmes de la Mutelle Générale de l’Education Nationale; EPIC, European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition; HPFS, Health Professionals Follow-up Study; IDEA and ECHO, Identifying Determinants of Eating and Activity and Etiology of Childhood Obesity; MESA, Multi-ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis; NHS, Nurses’ Health Study; T2D, type 2 diabetes.
Metabolic syndrome
A number of studies have reported greater risk of metabolic syndrome for consumers of ASB across a variety of cohorts [6,20–22] (Table 1). Estimates of the size of the increase in the risk of metabolic syndrome associated with consuming ASB range from approximately 17% [hazard ratios (HRs) and odds ratios (ORs) of 1.17] to over 100% (e.g., those consuming ASB had double the risk of metabolic syndrome compared with non-consumers), with the magnitude of the risk estimate also depending on which other risk factors were taken into consideration (see below). In studies that also examined the risk of metabolic syndrome associated with SSB consumption the magnitude of the increased risk was frequently similar for SSB and ASB [20,22] (Table 1).
Type 2 diabetes
In the European E3N study [23] and the Health Professionals Follow-up (HPFS) [24] risk for T2D was more than doubled for participants in the highest quartile of ASB consumption compared with non-consumers, and SSB consumption was also associated with increased risk of T2D. In both these studies [23–25], comparison of the magnitude of the risk between SSB and ASB is complicated by differences in intake of the two beverage types. Data from the Nurses’ Health Study (NHS) also indicated that risk for T2D was enhanced in those consuming at least one ASB or SSB per day [25]. Most recently, data from the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) has also indicated that risk for T2D was elevated in those consuming at least one ASB or SSB per day [26]. Importantly, a pronounced elevation of risk for T2D related to ASB in the EPIC study was seen even in participants who were normal weight at baseline [26].
Hypertension and cardiovascular disease
Risk for coronary heart disease (CHD) in the NHS was significantly elevated in women who consumed more than two ASB per day in age-adjusted models [27] or more than two SSB per day in fully adjusted models [27]. Similarly, in the HPFS risk of CHD was significantly elevated by ASB and SSB, but comparisons of magnitude of these effects are complicated by differences in intake [28]. In addition, consuming at least one ASB daily significantly elevated risk for hypertension for women in NHS-I and NHS-II, as well as in the HPFS [29], with the size of the effect similar to that observed for SSB in these samples. Finally, results from the Northern Manhattan Study (NMS) indicated that daily ASB consumption was associated with significantly increased risk of vascular events of a magnitude similar to daily SSB consumption [30].
Interventional studies
Within the past 5 years there have been fewer interventional studies that examined the effects of ASB, compared with the number of prospective studies published. In fact, only two recent papers appear to have directly manipulated exposure to ASB as a means of assessing effects on weight gain (Table 2). In the first, de Ruyter et al. [31] reported that primarily normal weight children (ages 4 to 11 years) assigned to consume a single ASB daily for 18 months gained less weight, and had smaller increases in skinfold thickness, waist-to-height ratios, and fat mass compared with children assigned to consume one SSB daily. In this study, all subjects were consumers of SSB at the start of the study, but it is not clear whether the children had experience with ASB prior to the intervention. Thus, this study suggests that among children of normal weight consuming ASB may lead to reduced weight gain relative to consuming SSB. However, whether consumption of ASB is associated with differences in weight gain compared with consumption of unsweetened beverages was not assessed. In the second study, overweight and obese adults who substituted water or ASB for SSB lost no more weightat6months than an attentional control (AC) group [10]. Replacement of SSB with water or ASB resulted in similar changes in some metabolic outcomes, such as decreased waist circumference and decreased systolic blood pressure, compared to the AC [10]. By contrast, although AC and water groups showed improvement in fasting glucose relative to baseline the ASB group did not [10]. Thus, in this interventional trial, consuming ASB beverages did not appear to provide a significant advantage in weight or metabolic outcomes compared with water or an AC. These interventional studies suggest the possibility that ASB are linked to lower risk of weight gain than SSB in lean children. However, in overweight or obese adults ASB are not more effective than water or a simple AC at improving weight loss or metabolic outcomes over 6 months. The reason for these different outcomes is unknown, but the study populations differed across a number of variables including BMI at the outset (overweight and obese vs lean), study setting (USA vs The Netherlands), duration (6 vs 18 months), and participant age (adults vs children). Although the data could indicate that children are less sensitive to the potentially negative effects of ASB, other studies have not found such effects and, as a whole, results of trials of ASB in children appear to be mixed, for a review, see [18].
Table 2.
Study | Sample characteristics | Duration | Interventions | Primary outcome | Other outcomes | Refs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CHOICE | 318 overweight and obese men and women | 6 months | Replacement of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB) with water Replacement of SSB with ASB Attentional control (AC) group | No differences in weight loss | ↓ Fasting glucose in water and AC groups | [10] |
DRINK | 641 boys and girlsa | 18 months | One ASB or one SSB per day | ↓ BMI z score in ASB group | SSB group had greater ↑ fat mass, skinfold thickness, and weight-to-height ratio | [31] |
Aged 4–11 years. Abbreviations: CHOICE, choose healthy options consciously every day; DRINK, double-blind, randomized intervention study in kids.
Take-home message from prospective cohort and interventional studies
Taken together, data from these recent studies suggest a link between consumption of ASB and a variety of negative health outcomes, including increased risk of being overweight and obesity, T2D, metabolic syndrome, and cardiovascular events [6,10,18–30], especially in adults. In none of these prospective studies was ASB consumption associated with significantly decreased risk; and in the adult interventional study ASB consumption was not associated with improved fasting glucose whereas water consumption was [10]. This general pattern of findings emerged across studies that varied widely in design, methodology, and population demographics. Although the models employed in most studies were adjusted for age, sex, level of physical activity, and smoking status, the methods used to specify each of these factors were variable.
Furthermore, the models employed in these studies differed with respect to the inclusion of demographic factors such as: race and/or ethnicity and education; dietary factors such as total number of calories, total amount of fat, grams of saturated fat, and fiber intake; and history of T2D or other metabolic disorders. Some models controlled for baseline BMI, but the method for controlling for this factor was not consistent across studies. Within individual studies, increased control of these types of factors tended to lower risk associated with ASB and SSB consumption. However, ASB and SSB consumption continued to be associated with significant elevations in risk even in models that attempted to control for all of these factors, including baseline BMI [6,18,22,23,25,26,29,30], with the magnitude of the effects of ASB and SSB consumption on these outcomes being generally similar when similar amounts of consumption were compared. This pattern suggests that family history, diet composition, and BMI at baseline may elevate health risks for people who consume ASB or SSB, but these factors are not sufficient to explain observed associations between consumption of ASB or SSB and negative health outcomes.
Reverse causality and cognitive influences
It has been suggested that the correlation between intake of ASB and increased incidence of negative health outcomes such as impaired energy and body weight regulation is an example of reverse causation [9], in which increasing body weight causes people to turn to the use of noncaloric sweeteners. Where reported, data from these prospective studies do indicate that those who regularly consume ASB tend to have higher BMI at baseline compared with those who do not [18,22,24,26,28,30], but some models that adjust for this baseline difference continue to find increased risk [6,18,22,23,25,26,29,30]. In addition, studies that separately analyzed risk among individuals who were not overweight or obese at baseline showed that ASB significantly increased risks of becoming overweight or obese [18], for T2D [26], and for vascular events [30], even when baseline BMI was considered. Thus, reverse causality does not seem plausibly to account for the increased risk in all studies. In addition, some of the effects of consuming ASB on these negative health outcomes could reflect a type of cognitive process in which knowledge that an ASB that is perceived to be ‘healthy’ grants permission to over consume other ‘non-healthy’ foods [32], and the consequences of ASB could be mediated through increased energy intake due to these types of cognitive distortions.
A role for more basic learning?
The results of a number of well-controlled animal studies suggest an additional possibility. Rats and mice that have been randomly assigned to receive dietary supplements mixed with noncaloric sweeteners exhibit greater weight gain and altered physiological responses compared with animals that receive the same diets mixed with sucrose or glucose [33–36], for a review, see [37]. These alterations are attributable to reductions in energy expenditure and to a decreased ability to regulate intake of normal sweet-tasting foods that contain energy [35,38]. An associative learning account of these effects has been supported by recent data that showed that consuming saccharin reduced the ability of sweet tastes to signal the post-ingestive caloric consequences of eating sweet-tasting foods, but not foods that did not taste sweet [33]. Increased body weight gain was observed only when other foods that tasted sweet and provided energy were consumed [33]. In other words, artificial sweeteners appear to stimulate food intake by reducing the ability to compensate for energy provided by caloric sweeteners in the diet.
Sweet tastes are known to evoke numerous physiological responses that help to maintain energy homeostasis by signaling the imminent arrival of nutrients in the gut and by facilitating the absorption and utilization of energy contained in food [39]. By weakening the validity of sweet taste as a signal for caloric post-ingestive outcomes, consumption of artificial sweeteners could impair energy and body weight regulation by degrading the ability of sweet taste to evoke these physiological responses when consumption of sweet tastes is followed by energy gain. This failure to anticipate calories and sugar appropriately when they do arrive could ultimately lead to the negative health consequences associated with ASB described above, by impairing the ability of sweetness to predict the arrival of energy in the gut accurately, thereby reducing the efficient utilization of that energy and perhaps weakening the cascade of events that initiate satiety. So, when consumed along with a diet high in dietary sugars, ASB might actually exacerbate the negative consequences of these dietary sugars by blunting such responses.
Physiological responses to high-intensity sweeteners
Artificial sweeteners evoke different brain responses compared with sugars
Recent studies in humans have documented that a number of metabolic and hormonal factors, typically elicited by the consumption of caloric sweeteners, either do not occur or are of reduced in magnitude following consumption of artificial sweeteners. For example, imaging studies in the human brain have indicated that sucrose, but not sucralose, activates dopaminergic midbrain areas related to reward or pleasantness, and that, compared with sucrose, sucralose results in reduced activation in other taste-related pathways [40]. Further, brain responses to sucrose differ in humans who regulatory consume ASB compared with those who do not [41,42]. Patterns of brain activation differ in response to saccharin compared with sucrose in those that do not consume ASB, whereas activation patterns in brains of ASB consumers do not differentiate between saccharin and sucrose [41].
Artificial sweeteners alone do not stimulate insulin or incretin release in vivo
A common result from studies in humans has also been that acute changes in the release of a variety of hormones and markers for post-prandial glucose homeostasis [including insulin, glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), peptide YY (PYY), glucose-dependent insulinotropic peptide (GIP), and ghrelin] do not occur when artificial sweeteners are delivered directly into the stomach or intestines [43–45]. Further, release of these markers does not appear to occur following oral consumption of an unflavored sucralose solution or an ASB sweetened with aspartame [46,47] (Table 3). From another standpoint, these studies also indicated that consumption of sucralose along with maltodextrin [46] or consumption of a SSB [47] failed to elicit significant GLP-1 release, raising concerns that there was potentially low sensitivity to detect changes.
Table 3.
Participant and premeal characteristics | Treatment prior to premeal | Delay between premeal and test meal | Sweetener conditions | Effects of premeal | Test meal characteristics | Effects after test meal compared to sugar-sweetened premeal | Effects after test meal compared to water premeal | Refs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
One man, seven women BMI = 18.8–23.9 50 ml liquida |
~12 h fast | N/A | Water | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | [46] |
Sucralose (~41 mg) + Maltodextrin (~25 g) |
↑ Glucoseb ↑ Insulinb |
|||||||
Sucralose (~41 mg) | No effectsb | |||||||
12 men, 12 women overweight and obese Mean BMI = 31.4 500 ml liquidc |
Overnight fastd | N/A | Water | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | [47] |
Sucrose (53 g) | ↓ Ghrelinb Smaller ↓ in GLP-1b ↑ GIPb |
|||||||
Aspartame (~225 mg)e | No effects | |||||||
Seven men, three women Mean BMI = 25.5 400 ml liquida premeal |
~12 h fastd | 15 min | Glucose (40 g) | N/A | 65 g powdered potatoes 20 g glucose egg yolk 200 ml water | N/A | Not assessed | [49] |
Sucralose (60 mg) | ↓ Glucosef ↓ Insulinf ↓ GLP-1f ↓ GIPf |
↓ Glucose ↓ Insulin ↓ GLP-1 ↓ GIP |
Not assessed | |||||
16 lean, 12 obese men and women Mean BMI = 27.5 400 g tea with crackers and cream cheeseg premeal |
Breakfast prior to premealh | 20 min | Sucrosei | N/A | Sandwiches, potato chips, cookiesj | N/A | Not assessed | [48] |
Aspartamei | ↓ Glucosef ↓ Insulinf |
No effects | Not assessed | |||||
Steviai | ↓ Glucosef ↓ Insulinf |
↓ Glucose ↓ Insulin |
Not assessed | |||||
Eight women Mean BMI = 22.2 355 ml liquida premeal |
~10 h fast | 60 min | Sucrose (50 g) | ↑ Glucoseb ↑ Insulinb |
Fixed quantityk scrambled eggs with cheese orange juice buttered whole wheat toast | N/A | N/A | [50] |
Water | N/A | ↑ Glucose | ||||||
Sucralose (6 g Splenda ≈420 mg sucralose) | No effects | ↑ Glucose | No effects | |||||
Sucrose (50 g) + Sucralose (6 g Splenda) |
No effectsf | No effects | No effects | |||||
Ten males, 12 femalel Mean BMI = 25.6 240 ml liquidm premeal |
~10 h fast | 10 min | Carbonated water | Not testedn | 75 g glucoseo | Not assessed | N/A | [51] |
~46 mg sucralose + ~26 mg acesulfame-K |
Not assessed | ↑GLP-1 | ||||||
13 male, 12 femalel Mean BMI = 25.7 240 ml liquidm premeal |
~10 h fast | 10 min | Carbonated water | Not testedn | 75 g glucoseo | Not assessed | N/A | [52] |
~46 mg sucralose + ~26 mg acesulfame-K |
Not assessed | ↑ GLP-1 release | ||||||
Three male, six femalep Mean BMI = 21.7 240 ml liquidm premeal |
Carbonated water | Not assessed | N/A | |||||
~46 mg sucralose + ~26 mg acesulfame-K |
Not assessed | No effectsq | ||||||
One male, nine femaler Mean BMI = 35 240 ml liquidl premeal |
Carbonated water | Not assessed | N/A | |||||
~46 mg sucralose + ~26 mg acesulfame-K |
Not assessed | ↑ GLP-1 releasep |
Unflavored.
Compared to premeal containing water.
Coca Cola or diet Coca Cola; semi-skimmed milk was also tested in this study but these results are omitted for clarity.
Following standardized evening meal.
Aspartame concentration in diet Coca Cola was estimated based on published analysis of aspartame concentration in cola in Denmark [54].
Compared to premeal containing sugar (no water control included).
Premeal is described as ‘a 400 g preload of tea and crackers with cream cheese sweetened with stevia (Whole Foods 365 brand), aspartame (equal sweetener), or sucrose’. Thus, it appears that the cream cheese was sweetened whereas the tea was not. Specific weight of the tea, cream cheese, or crackers is not reported.
Breakfast was standardized, but timing of breakfast relative to premeal not reported.
Quantity of sweetener was not reported.
Self-selected by participants.
Not specified.
Young healthy participants aged 12–25 years.
Diet rite cola or unflavored carbonated water.
Data only reported for AUC of premeal and meal combined.
Young participants aged 12–25 years with type 1 diabetes.
Volume and concentration not indicated.
For blood glucose, AUC was slightly but not significantly increased after diet soda compared to carbonated water.
Young participants aged 12–25 years with type 2 diabetes.
Unlike caloric sweeteners, artificial sweeteners do not augment insulin or incretin release in response to meals
Studies that measured responses to artificial sweeteners combined in various ways with nutrient signals also suggest that artificial sweeteners may not augment nutrient-dependent release of insulin or the incretins (Table 3) in the same way that caloric sugars do. For example, Anton [48] reported that glucose and insulin levels were higher after participants consumed a sucrose-sweetened premeal of tea, cream cheese, and crackers, compared with the same premeal sweetened with either aspartame or stevia, a difference that would be expected due to the additional energy and carbohydrate in the sucrose-sweetened premeal. Effects of these premeals on glucose homeostasis during lunch were also assessed, but are difficult to interpret because the volume and composition of the lunch meal was self-selected by the participant and therefore may have varied after the different premeals. As part of another study [49], subjects consumed unflavored liquid premeals sweetened with either glucose or sucralose prior to eating a potato meal fixed in volume and composition. The sucralose premeal alone did not elevate glucose, insulin, GLP-1, or GIP, whereas the glucose premeal did, and after the mixed meal was consumed the sucralose premeal was associated with reduced GLP-1 release compared with the glucose premeal. As in the Anton study, the total amount of carbohydrate consumed was significantly higher after the glucose premeal compared with the sucralose premeal and thus these differences between the premeal groups would be expected. In a study that did include a control for total energy and carbohydrate intake [50], an unflavored liquid sucralose premeal had no effects on glucose and insulin levels prior to a mixed breakfast meal compared to water, whereas a sucrose premeal produced increased blood glucose and insulin levels prior to the mixed meal and decreased blood glucose after the mixed meal, compared with the water and sucralose premeals. As evidenced in Table 3, these clinical studies have been highly variable with regard to a number of procedural aspects including: length of fast prior to testing; participant demographics such as age, sex, and weight; composition, form, flavoring, and amount of premeal (e.g., liquid vs solid); delay between premeal and meal; meal composition and sweetener concentration; and comparison groups. Although this variability complicates conclusions about the effects of artificial sweeteners, the data nonetheless appear consistent with the idea that physiological responses that typically occur following consumption of caloric sweeteners are not elicited by artificial sweeteners or are of much smaller magnitude.
Artificial sweeteners may weaken learned responses
Such results have typically been interpreted as indicating that artificial sweeteners are largely inert with regard to effects on glucose homeostasis because they do not reliably elicit post-ingestive responses similar to caloric sugars. However, when considered within the framework of Pavlovian conditioning principles, experiences with noncaloric sweet tastes that are not accompanied by typical and expected post-ingestive consequences, such as post-prandial release of insulin, GLP-1, or GIP, or activation of brain regions sensitive to energy or reward, might eventually degrade or partially extinguish the capacity of caloric sweet tastes to evoke those responses. And this weakening could occur even if ASB evoke responses that are similar in direction to those evoked by caloric sweeteners but greatly reduced in magnitude. For example, Brown et al. [51,52] found that, compared to a carbonated water premeal, consumption of a flavored ASB premeal appeared to have no effects, but the ASB premeal did augment GLP-1 release in response to an oral glucose load in healthy subjects and subjects with type 1 diabetes, but not in those with T2D. However, the magnitude of this GLP-1 effect in responseto the ASB was not compared with that evoked by aSSB; if the ASB-evoked release was of a lower magnitude than an SSB-evoked release, learned responses would be weakened. This remains to be tested. In addition, the factors that led to these studies [51,52] demonstrating a significant physiological response to an ASB compared with others that did not are not yet clear, but probably relate to the wide variability in procedural details across such studies.
Potential consequences of weakening learned responses
These data are generally consistent with the idea that ASB do not evoke responses like those evoked by caloric sweeteners. Regular consumption of ASB might thereby come to result in weaker responses to sweet tastes when they are produced by consumption of caloric sweeteners. Some evidence for this type of effect comes from recent studies in rats in which animals that had previously consumed saccharin-sweetened yogurt had a blunted thermic effect of food in response to a novel, sweet-tasting meal compared with those that had previously consumed glucose-sweetened yogurt [35]. In a second experiment, a significantly weaker GLP-1 response was shown in response to consumption of a sweet caloric solution by rats that were ASB consumers, compared with rats that did not consume ASB [36]. To date, brain-imaging studies [41,42] have provided some support for potentially similar consequences in humans, but no similar tests of physiological responses have been reported.
Concluding remarks
Recent data from humans and rodent models have provided little support for ASB in promoting weight loss or preventing negative health outcomes such as T2D, metabolic syndrome, and cardiovascular events. Instead, a number of studies suggest people who regularly consume ASB are at increased risk compared with those that do not consume ASB; with the magnitude of the increased risks similar to those associated with SSB [6,10,18–30]. In a number of cases, these effects cannot be attributed to baseline characteristics such as family history or BMI [6,18,22,23,25,26,29,30]. This somewhat counterintuitive result may reflect negative consequences of interfering with learned relationships between sweet tastes and typical post-ingestive outcomes, which may result in impaired ability to compensate for energy provided when caloric sweeteners are consumed. Paying increased attention to the ability of learning to modulate physiological and neural signals related to energy balance and metabolic regulation may improve our ability to understand circumstances under which reductions in the energy content of foods and beverages may lead to worsened and not improved health outcomes (see also Box 1).
Box 1. Outstanding questions.
Does regular consumption of high-intensity sweeteners result in changes in physiological responses to caloric sweeteners in humans? If so, what mechanisms are responsible for these changes?
What role might differential brain responses to nutritive compared with non-nutritive sweeteners play in modulating signals related to energy balance and glucose homeostasis?
Are sweeteners, artificial or caloric, consumed in beverage form particularly problematic? Is consumption of artificial sweeteners in other forms, with or without other foods, associated with increased, decreased, or unaltered health risks?
Does experience with high-intensity sweeteners interfere with learning about the energetic value of nutritive sugars in people? If so, can principles of learning contribute to strategies to repair the deficits?
Does replacement of ASB with unsweetened beverages have advantageous effects on being overweight, obesity, or other metabolic derangements?
In addition, although consumption of ASB may contribute to being overweight, obesity, and metabolic derangements, other factors must also be in operation, particularly because not everyone consumes ASB or uses artificial sweeteners. Further, negative consequences of ASB should not be interpreted to suggest that sugars should be consumed in preference to artificial sweeteners. Instead, consumption of artificial sweeteners may exacerbate the negative effects of sugars by reducing the ability to predict the consequences of consuming sugars reliably and/or by altering cognitive processes that lead to overconsumption. Finally, most of the data documenting increased risks have come from studies of ASB; artificial sweeteners are now increasingly included in products other than beverages, often in combination with caloric sweeteners [12,16,53]. Whether such products have positive, negative, or neutral effects on body weight or other metabolic outcomes is even less clear than for ASB. However, current findings suggest that caution about the overall sweetening of the diet is warranted, regardless of whether the sweetener provides energy directly or not.
Acknowledgments
Research supported by NIH R01DK076078 and P01HD052112. The author wishes to thank Terry Davidson and three anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments.
Glossary
- Artificially sweetened beverages (ASB):
also known as ‘diet’ soft drinks, beverages manufactured with one or more high-intensity sweeteners in place of energy-yielding sugars like sucrose or high-fructose corn syrup with the purpose of reducing or eliminating calories.
- Body mass index (BMI):
used as an index of risk for weight-related health outcomes and is calculated as (kg/m2). In adults BMIs of 18.5–24.9 are considered to be within the normal range, whereas BMIs from 25 to 29.9 are classified as overweight and a BMI greater than 30 is classified as obese.
- Hazard ratio (HR) and odds ratio (OR):
statistical measures of how often an event occurs in one group compared to another. A HR or OR of 1 means there is no difference between the groups and an HR or OR >1 means there is an increased likelihood that the event will occur in the group of interest relative to the comparison group.
- High-intensity sweeteners:
also known as low-calorie sweeteners, artificial sweeteners, non-nutritive sweeteners, or noncaloric sweeteners are chemicals that produce the perception of sweet taste at very low concentrations. High-intensity sweeteners currently used commonly in foods and beverages include sucralose, aspartame, saccharin, and acesulfame potassium, as well as newly approved extracts from the plant Stevia rebaudiana. Although some high-intensity sweeteners can be metabolized by the body, foods and beverages typically contain them in such small quantities that even those that can be metabolized contribute minute amounts of energy to the diet.
- Incretin hormones:
hormones such as glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) and glucose-dependent insulinotropic peptide (GIP) that are released from L cells and K cells in the intestine, respectively, and serve to enhance the release of insulin from beta cells, slow the rate of gastric emptying, and may contribute to satiety.
- Metabolic syndrome:
a group of factors that occur together and contribute to increased risk for coronary artery disease, stroke, and type 2 diabetes (T2D). Typical definitions require three or more of the following: blood pressure >130/85 mmHg; fasting blood glucose >100 mg/dl; large waist circumference (men >102 cm, women >89 cm); low high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol (men <40 mg/dl; women <50 mg/dl); triglycerides >150 mg/dl.
- Post-prandial glucose homeostasis:
following meals (post-prandial) levels of glucose in the blood are tightly regulated by the release of a variety of hormones that contribute to clearance of glucose. For example, release of insulin from the beta cells of the pancreas is required to move sugar from the blood into cells.
- Sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB):
also known as ‘regular’ soft drinks, manufactured with one or more caloric sweeteners such as sucrose or high-fructose corn syrup.
- Thermic effect of food:
increase in metabolic rate after consumption of a meal related to energy required to process and metabolize the consumed food.
- Type 2 diabetes:
chronic elevation of blood glucose due to insulin resistance that is also characterized by impaired incretin secretion.
References
- 1.Johnson RK, et al. Dietary sugars intake and cardiovascular health: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2009;120:1011–1020. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.192627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Malik VS, et al. Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of metabolic syndrome and type 2 diabetes: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. 2010;33:2477–2483. doi: 10.2337/dc10-1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Malik VS, et al. Intake of sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84:274–288. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/84.1.274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Te Morenga L, et al. Dietary sugars and body weight: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and cohort studies. BMJ. 2013;346:e7492. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.van Baak MA, Astrup A. Consumption of sugars and body weight. Obes Rev. 2009;10(Suppl. 1):9–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Duffey KJ, et al. Dietary patterns matter: diet beverages and cardiometabolic risks in the longitudinal Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:909–915. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.026682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fitch C, Keim KS. Position of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics: use of nutritive and nonnutritive sweeteners. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:739–758. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gardner C, et al. Nonnutritive sweeteners: current use and health perspectives: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association and the American Diabetes Association. Circulation. 2012;126:509–519. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31825c42ee. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mattes RD, Popkin BM. Nonnutritive sweetener consumption in humans: effects on appetite and food intake and their putative mechanisms. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:1–14. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Tate DF, et al. Replacing caloric beverages with water or diet beverages for weight loss in adults: main results of the Choose Healthy Options Consciously Everyday (CHOICE) randomized clinical trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:555–563. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.026278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Raben A, Richelsen B. Artificial sweeteners: a place in the field of functional foods? Focus on obesity and related metabolic disorders. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2012;15:597–604. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e328359678a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yang Q. Gain weight by “going diet?” Artificial sweeteners and the neurobiology of sugar cravings: Neuroscience 2010. Yale J Biol Med. 2010;83:101–108. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brown RJ, et al. Artificial sweeteners: a systematic review of metabolic effects in youth. Int J Pediatr Obes. 2010;5:305–312. doi: 10.3109/17477160903497027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Pepino MY, Bourne C. Non-nutritive sweeteners, energy balance, and glucose homeostasis. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2011;14:391–395. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e3283468e7e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wiebe N, et al. A systematic review on the effect of sweeteners on glycemic response and clinically relevant outcomes. BMC Med. 2011;9:123. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sylvetsky AC, et al. Low-calorie sweetener consumption is increasing in the United States. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96:640–646. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.034751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Barclay AW, Brand-Miller J. The Australian paradox: a substantial decline in sugars intake over the same timeframe that overweight and obesity have increased. Nutrients. 2011;3:491–504. doi: 10.3390/nu3040491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fowler SP, et al. Fueling the obesity epidemic? Artificially sweetened beverage use and long-term weight gain. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16:1894–1900. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Laska MN, et al. Longitudinal associations between key dietary behaviors and weight gain over time: transitions through the adolescent years. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2012;20:118–125. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dhingra R, et al. Soft drink consumption and risk of developing cardiometabolic risk factors and the metabolic syndrome in middle-aged adults in the community. Circulation. 2007;116:480–488. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.689935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lutsey PL, et al. Dietary intake and the development of the metabolic syndrome: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities study. Circulation. 2008;117:754–761. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.716159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nettleton JA, et al. Dietary patterns and incident cardiovascular disease in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90:647–654. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fagherazzi G, et al. Consumption of artificially and sugar-sweetened beverages and incident type 2 diabetes in the Etude Epidemiologique aupres des femmes de la Mutuelle Generale de l’Education Nationale-European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:517–523. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.050997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Koning L, et al. Sugar-sweetened and artificially sweetened beverage consumption and risk of type 2 diabetes in men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93:1321–1327. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.007922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bhupathiraju SN, et al. Caffeinated and caffeine-free beverages and risk of type 2 diabetes. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:155–166. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.048603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Romaguera D, et al. Consumption of sweet beverages and type 2 diabetes incidence in European adults: results from EPIC-InterAct. Diabetologia. 2013;56:1520–1530. doi: 10.1007/s00125-013-2899-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fung TT, et al. Sweetened beverage consumption and risk of coronary heart disease in women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:1037–1042. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.de Koning L, et al. Sweetened beverage consumption, incident coronary heart disease, and biomarkers of risk in men. Circulation. 2012;125:1735–1741. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.111.067017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cohen L, et al. Association of sweetened beverage intake with incident hypertension. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27:1127–1134. doi: 10.1007/s11606-012-2069-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gardener H, et al. Diet soft drink consumption is associated with an increased risk of vascular events in the Northern Manhattan Study. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27:1120–1126. doi: 10.1007/s11606-011-1968-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.de Ruyter JC, et al. A trial of sugar-free or sugar-sweetened beverages and body weight in children. N Engl J Med. 2012;367:1397–1406. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1203034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chandon P. How package design and packaged-based marketing claims lead to overeating. Appl Econ Perspect Policy. 2012;31:7–31. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Davidson TL, et al. Intake of high-intensity sweeteners alters the ability of sweet taste to signal caloric consequences: implications for the learned control of energy and body weight regulation. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2011;64:1430–1441. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.552729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jurgens H, et al. Consuming fructose-sweetened beverages increases body adiposity in mice. Obes Res. 2005;13:1146–1156. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Swithers SE, Davidson TL. A role for sweet taste: calorie predictive relations in energy regulation by rats. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122:161–173. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.1.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Swithers SE, et al. Experience with the high-intensity sweetener saccharin impairs glucose homeostasis and GLP-1 release in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;233:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Swithers SE, et al. High-intensity sweeteners and energy balance. Physiol Behav. 2010;100:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Feijo FM, et al. Saccharin and aspartame, compared with sucrose, induce greater weight gain in adult Wistar rats, at similar total caloric intake levels. Appetite. 2013;60:203–207. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Smeets PA, et al. Cephalic phase responses and appetite. Nutr Rev. 2010;68:643–655. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2010.00334.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Frank GK, et al. Sucrose activates human taste pathways differently from artificial sweetener. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1559–1569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Green E, Murphy C. Altered processing of sweet taste in the brain of diet soda drinkers. Physiol Behav. 2012;107:560–567. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rudenga KJ, Small DM. Amygdala response to sucrose consumption is inversely related to artificial sweetener use. Appetite. 2012;58:504–507. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ma J, et al. Effect of the artificial sweetener, sucralose, on gastric emptying and incretin hormone release in healthy subjects. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol. 2009;296:735–739. doi: 10.1152/ajpgi.90708.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ma J, et al. Effect of the artificial sweetener, sucralose, on small intestinal glucose absorption in healthy human subjects. Br J Nutr. 2010;104:803–806. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510001327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Steinert RE, et al. Effects of carbohydrate sugars and artificial sweeteners on appetite and the secretion of gastrointestinal satiety peptides. Br J Nutr. 2011;105:1320–1328. doi: 10.1017/S000711451000512X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ford HE, et al. Effects of oral ingestion of sucralose on gut hormone response and appetite in healthy normal-weight subjects. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2011;65:508–513. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2010.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Maersk M, et al. Satiety scores and satiety hormone response after sucrose-sweetened soft drink compared with isocaloric semi-skimmed milk and with non-caloric soft drink: a controlled trial. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66:523–529. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2011.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Anton SD, et al. Effects of stevia, aspartame, and sucrose on food intake, satiety, and postprandial glucose and insulin levels. Appetite. 2010;55:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wu T, et al. Effects of different sweet preloads on incretin hormone secretion, gastric emptying, and postprandial glycemia in healthy humans. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:78–83. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.021543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brown AW, et al. Short-term consumption of sucralose, a nonnutritive sweetener, is similar to water with regard to select markers of hunger signaling and short-term glucose homeostasis in women. Nutr Res. 2011;31:882–888. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2011.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Brown RJ, et al. Ingestion of diet soda before a glucose load augments glucagon-like peptide-1 secretion. Diabetes Care. 2009;32:2184–2186. doi: 10.2337/dc09-1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Brown RJ, et al. Effects of diet soda on gut hormones in youths with diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2012;35:959–964. doi: 10.2337/dc11-2424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ng SW, et al. Use of caloric and noncaloric sweeteners in US consumer packaged foods, 2005–2009. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:1828–1834. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Leth T, et al. Estimated intake of intense sweeteners from nonalcoholic beverages in Denmark. Food Addit Contam. 2007;24:227–235. doi: 10.1080/02652030601019429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]