Abstract
Accounts of bilingual cognitive advantages suggest an associative link between cross-linguistic competition and inhibitory control. We investigate this link by examining English-Spanish bilinguals’ parallel language activation during auditory word recognition and nonlinguistic Stroop performance. Thirty-one English-Spanish bilinguals and 30 English monolinguals participated in an eye-tracking study. Participants heard words in English (e.g., comb) and identified corresponding pictures from a display that included pictures of a Spanish competitor (e.g., conejo, English rabbit). Bilinguals with higher Spanish proficiency showed more parallel language activation and smaller Stroop effects than bilinguals with lower Spanish proficiency. Across all bilinguals, stronger parallel language activation between 300–500ms after word onset was associated with smaller Stroop effects; between 633–767ms, reduced parallel language activation was associated with smaller Stroop effects. Results suggest that bilinguals who perform well on the Stroop task show increased cross-linguistic competitor activation during early stages of word recognition and decreased competitor activation during later stages of word recognition. Findings support the hypothesis that cross-linguistic competition impacts domain-general inhibition.
Keywords: Parallel language activation, inhibition, Stroop task, bilingualism, eye-tracking
Bilinguals have been found to show fine-grained advantages in cognitive control relative to their monolingual peers. These advantages are becoming increasingly well-defined, and include aspects of cognitive control such as conflict resolution on Stroop (e.g., Bialystok, 2006; Bialystok, Craik, & Luk, 2008; Bialystok & DePape, 2009; Costa, Hernandez, & Sebastian-Galles, 2008; Costa, Hernandez, Costa-Faidella, & Sebastian-Galles, 2009; Hernandez, Costa, Fuentes, Vivas, & Sebastian-Galles, 2010; Luk, de Sa, & Bialystok, 2011) and Simon tasks (Bialystok, Craik, Klein, & Viswanathan, 2004; Salvatierra & Rosselli, 2011; Schroeder & Marian, 2012), conflict monitoring (Costa, Hernandez, Costa-Faidella, & Sebastian-Galles, 2009; Tao, Marcecova, Taft, Asanowicz, & Wodniecka, 2011), and task switching (Prior & MacWhinney, 2010). Bilingual cognitive advantages are thought to emerge over time due to the cognitive demands of bilingual language processing (e.g., Bialystok & Craik, 2010; Green, 2011; Kroll, 2008), and typically are positively associated with number of years of functional bilingualism (Luk, de Sa, & Bialystok, 2011), with daily immersion in both language contexts (e.g., Tao et al., 2011), and with high bilingual proficiency (e.g., Singh & Mishra, 2012). The exact mechanisms of bilingual language processing that give rise to these cognitive benefits are not yet fully understood. Here, we link the co-activation of a non-target language during spoken word recognition to enhanced control abilities.
A number of studies suggest direct links between cognitive control abilities and bilingual language processing (e.g., Kroll, Bobb, Misra, & Guo, 2008; Levy, McVeigh, Marful, & Anderson, 2007; Macizo, Bajo, & Martin, 2010; Martin, Macizo, & Bajo, 2010; Michael & Gollan, 2005; Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012; Morales, Paolieri, & Bajo, 2011). For example, in the language production domain, Prior and Gollan (2011) reported better task-switching abilities in bilinguals who code-switch frequently than in bilinguals who do not code-switch and Vega and Fernandez (2011) found that more balanced bilinguals made fewer perseveration errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Correlations have also been identified between bilingual language processing and cognitive control tasks on which bilinguals have previously shown cognitive advantages. Linck, Schwieter, and Sunderman (2011) found that smaller switching costs from L2 or L3 into L1 correlated with smaller Simon effects (a marker of better inhibition abilities) in trilingual young adults. Moreover, cross-linguistic activation has been linked to inhibitory control abilities. For example, stronger cross-linguistic cognate activation and facilitation during naming have been associated with less efficient Simon inhibition (Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008). Similarly, more cross-linguistic intrusion errors during a verbal fluency task have been linked to increased errors in an incongruent condition of a flanker task in older bilinguals (Gollan, Sandoval, & Salmon, 2011). Using a composite inhibitory control measure including nonlinguistic Stroop, Simon, and antisaccade task performance, Pivneva, Palmer, and Titone (2012) showed that increased inhibition abilities in bilinguals were associated with overall more efficient speech planning and production in L1 and L2. Finally, during learning, more efficient Simon performance has been associated with better implicit memory outcomes in high-interference contexts (Bartolotti, Marian, Schroeder, & Shook, 2011). Critically, in all of these correlational studies, better cognitive control was associated with less interference in the linguistic context, including less cross-linguistic activation. Interestingly, self-reports across these studies show moderate to high L2 proficiency and age of L2 acquisition between 5 to 10 years, suggesting that cognitive control mechanisms may be recruited for linguistic processing in bilinguals with varied proficiency and age of acquisition profiles.
Similar to language production in bilinguals, early evidence suggests that bilinguals’ receptive language processing is also supported by cognitive control mechanisms (e.g., Kaushanskaya, Blumenfeld, & Marian, 2011; Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008). Recent evidence suggests that simultaneous bilinguals outperform their monolingual peers in ignoring irrelevant auditory input during listening (Krizman, Marian, Shook, Skoe, & Kraus, 2012; Soveri, Laine, Hämäläinen, & Hugdahl, 2010). Involvement of inhibitory control mechanisms has also been identified at the linguistic level, including findings that bilinguals inhibit cross-linguistic homographs and that such inhibition is recruited across a window of time that extends past the homograph stimulus (Macizo, Bajo, and Martin, 2010; Martin et al., 2010).
Findings from Blumenfeld and Marian (2011) also speak to the temporal aspects of inhibition during receptive language processing. In an English within-language task, bilinguals who demonstrated smaller Stroop effects showed less residual inhibition of phonological cohort competitors 500ms after target identification (as indexed by priming probes). This correlation between phonological conflict resolution and performance on a nonlinguistic Stroop task was significant in bilinguals but not monolinguals. Differences between bilinguals and monolinguals were ascribed to bilinguals’ need to resolve higher levels of linguistic conflict. Such linguistic conflict includes auditory cohort competitors within-language, which are present in both monolingual and bilingual listeners (Marslen-Wilson, 1987; Marian & Spivey, 2003a,b). Yet, in bilinguals, even within-language processing may be more challenging due to the added contribution of simultaneous between-language competitors. Thus bilinguals are confronted with a larger cohort of similar-sounding words during auditory recognition, and may routinely recruit domain-general cognitive control mechanisms to meet expected linguistic needs.
Parallel language activation during auditory comprehension is a process that has been identified across various language contexts and proficiency levels, and may thus be a relatively ubiquitous source of increased competition during bilinguals’ receptive language processing (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007; Canseco-Gonzales, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer, & Wagner, 2010; Cutler, Weber, & Otake, 2006; Ju & Luce, 2004; Marian, Blumenfeld, & Boukrina, 2008; Marian & Spivey, 2003a, b; Weber & Cutler, 2004; Weber & Paris 2004). In general, the most robust parallel language activation has been found in bilinguals who are highly proficient and currently immersed in the non-target language (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007). Such increased parallel activation is likely due to more automatic activation of representations as proficiency increases (Segalowitz & Hulstijn, 2005). However, even when bilinguals listen in their more proficient language, they are likely to experience at least occasional co-activation of their less proficient language (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007; Ju & Luce, 2004). Co-activation of two languages has also been found in the presence of semantic contextual constraints (e.g., Chambers & Cooke, 2009; Schwartz & Kroll, 2006; Vandeberg, Guadalupe, & Zwaan, 2011). Together, findings suggest that nonselective language activation may occur across a range of proficiency levels and linguistic contexts, with the most reliable parallel language activation when the non-target language is highly proficient. It can be concluded that cross-linguistic activation and conflict resolution permeate bilingual receptive language processing in everyday environments.
With a large body of evidence suggesting that parallel language activation is common during receptive processing in bilinguals, the question of how this competition is resolved remains under investigation. The current study provides a direct test of how bilinguals resolve competition during parallel language activation. We examine cross-linguistic activation and conflict resolution in bilinguals, and attempt to identify a link between language proficiency, parallel language activation (as indexed via eye-tracking), and performance on the same nonlinguistic Stroop task that was used in the within-language study by Blumenfeld and Marian (2011). Eye-tracking is a well-suited methodology to covertly index co-activation of similar-sounding words (e.g., English comb, Spanish conejo) over time, including the subsequent deactivation of irrelevant word candidates (e.g., Marian & Spivey, 2003a,b; Tanenhaus, Magnuson, Dahan, & Chambers, 2000; Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1995). Phonological competition resolution may share a common cognitive control process with the nonlinguistic Stroop task because, in both cases, the perceived stimulus can be interpreted in two different ways. That is, perceptual/conceptual conflict is inherent to the stimulus, and this conflict must be resolved in order to yield the most appropriate interpretation (e.g., during phonological competition, the target word is comb not conejo; on the classic Stroop task, the ink color is red not blue). This aspect of Stroop-type competition resolution, also referred to as stimulus-stimulus conflict (Kornblum, Stevens, Whipple, & Requin, 1999) is therefore a good candidate as a mechanism that may support cross-linguistic competition resolution in bilinguals.
The present study
In the present study, we aimed to relate bilinguals’ cross-linguistic activation and competition resolution to performance on a nonlinguistic Stroop task. Identification of such a link would establish cross-linguistic competition resolution during comprehension as one source of the previously-found bilingual advantage on Stroop-type tasks and would thus contribute to specification of the linguistic origins of bilingual advantages on cognitive control tasks. A bilingual visual world paradigm was employed to index parallel language activation during auditory word recognition, where English-Spanish participants heard words in English and identified corresponding pictures (e.g., comb) in the presence of cohort competitor pictures (e.g., conejo, Spanish for rabbit). Participants also completed a nonlinguistic spatial Stroop task (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2011, adapted from Liu, Banich, Jacobsen, & Tanabe, 2004). On this Stroop task, participants identified the direction of arrows (right or left) that appeared on the right or the left side of the screen, resulting in perceptual conflict (right vs. left) on trials where right-pointing arrows appeared on the left or left-pointing arrows appeared on the right. Neural correlates of a similar spatial Stroop task (Liu et al., 2004) have high overlap with neural correlates of cognitive control during bilingual language processing (Abutalebi, 2008), suggesting that similar cognitive mechanisms may underlie the two. Moreover, bilingual experience has been shown to change the neural substrates of Stroop-type tasks, relative to monolinguals (Luk, Anderson, Craik, Grady, & Bialystok, 2010), suggesting that Stroop-type mechanisms may be recruited for bilingual language processing. Consistent with these previous findings, Blumenfeld and Marian (2011) found a link between Stroop inhibition and within-language competition resolution in bilinguals. Therefore, the spatial Stroop task was chosen since it is likely to index cognitive control mechanisms that may be recruited for bilinguals’ linguistic competition resolution.
We predicted that a link would exist between Stroop performance and parallel language activation during auditory word identification and that this association would be mediated by language proficiency. To examine whether bilingual proficiency simultaneously influences parallel language activation and cognitive control, bilingual participants were divided into higher-proficiency and lower-proficiency groups. Increased bilingual proficiency has been associated with both, more robust parallel language activation (e.g., Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007) as well as more efficient cognitive control skills (e.g., Singh & Misra, 2007). This leads to a dual prediction about cross-linguistic activation as proficiency increases. First, parallel language activation is predicted to become more robust as non-target representations are co-activated more automatically. Second, cross-linguistic interference is predicted to be resolved more efficiently as cognitive control skills improve. We can situate these seemingly contradictory predictions within models of bilingual receptive processing in highly interactive architectures (BIA, Dijkstra & van Heuven, 2002; van Heuven, Dijkstra, & Grainger, 1998; BLINCS, Shook & Marian, 2012). Both the BIA and BLINCS posit bottom-up parallel language activation as well as the subsequent action of higher-level cognitive control mechanisms to guide word selection. According to these theoretical frameworks and previous literature, more robust parallel language activation and better inhibitory control may both be associated with higher proficiency, but the two effects are likely exerted at different points in the timecourse of word recognition. The present study explicitly tests this hypothesis, and juxtaposes the two predictions by examining the relationship between language activation and inhibitory control across the timecourse of co-activation and deactivation. First, given previous findings that higher bilingual proficiency is related to increased parallel language activation and to better cognitive control (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007; Singh & Misra, 2007), we examined whether greater parallel activation would be associated with better Stroop performance during early stages of word recognition. Second, since better cognitive control has also been linked to less cross-linguistic activation (Gollan, Sandoval, & Salmon, 2011; Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008), we expected better Stroop performance to be associated with less cross-linguistic activation during a later competition resolution stage where inhibitory control would be applied.
In sum, we predicted that higher-proficiency bilinguals would show stronger parallel language activation (e.g., Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007; Ju & Luce, 2004), suggesting more automatized access to robust representations, and smaller Stroop effects (e.g., Singh & Mishra, 2012), indicating honed abilities in inhibitory control. Moreover, across all bilinguals, we predicted that correlational relationships would emerge between parallel language activation and Stroop performance. Specifically, we predicted that higher-proficiency bilinguals would show initial co-activation of cross-linguistic competitors, followed by efficient resolution of such competition as a function of inhibition skills.
Method
Participants
Thirty-one English-Spanish bilinguals (9 males, mean age = 22.0, SD = 5.1) and 30 English monolinguals (6 males, mean age = 21.4, SD = 3.9) participated.1 Bilinguals were divided into a group with higher Spanish proficiency (n = 15) and a group with lower Spanish proficiency (n = 16) based on their Spanish verbal fluency performance (letters and categories combined) and using a median-split procedure. Higher Spanish proficiency bilinguals outperformed lower Spanish proficiency bilinguals on Spanish verbal fluency across letters and categories, t (29) = 6.7, p < .001 (see Table 1 for linguistic and cognitive background characteristics of the two bilingual groups). In addition, the higherproficiency group self-reported higher Spanish proficiency than the lower-proficiency group, t (29) = 2.1, p < .05. The two bilingual groups did not differ in age of Spanish acquisition, t (29) = 0.8, p > .1 or in their current exposure to Spanish, t (29) = 0.9, p > .1. The monolingual group and the two bilingual groups did not differ in age at the time of testing [F (2, 58) = 3.1, p > .05, monolinguals: M = 21.4, SE = 0.7; lower-proficiency bilinguals: M=20.3, SE=0.7; higher-proficiency bilinguals: M=24.0, SE=1.6], performance on the nonverbal portion of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence [F (2, 58) = 0.1, p > .5, monolinguals: M = 110.5, SE = 2.2; lower-proficiency bilinguals: M = 110.6, SE = 2.9; higher-proficiency bilinguals: M = 109.3, SE = 2.6], digit span [F (2, 58) = 0.5, p > .5, monolinguals: M = 17.5, SE = 0.4; lower-proficiency bilinguals, M = 17.8, SE = 0.7; higher-proficiency bilinguals, M = 17.5, SE = 0.3], or receptive vocabulary in English [Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: F (2, 58) = 0.2, p > .5, monolinguals: M = 116.7, SE = 2.1; lower-proficiency bilinguals: M = 114.9, SE = 2.9; higher-proficiency bilinguals: M =117.7, SE = 3.3].
Table 1.
Higher-proficiency bilinguals Mean (SE) | Lower-proficiency Bilinguals Mean (SE) | |
---|---|---|
Age of Spanish acquisitionns | 2.1 (0.7) | 2.9 (0.8) |
Current exposure to Spanish ns (%) | 25.9 (3.7) | 18.4 (3.6) |
Digit spanns | 17.5 (0.6) | 17.8 (0.7) |
WASI, matrix reasoningns | 109.3 (2.6) | 110.6 (2.9) |
English receptive vocabularyns | 117.7 (3.3) | 114.9 (2.9) |
Self-reported Spanish proficiency* (1–10 scale) | 8.1 (0.3) | 7.3 (0.3) |
Spanish letter fluency** | 12.3 (0.5) | 7.1 (0.3) |
Letter E | 11.3 (0.8) | 5.6 (0.8) |
Letter P | 12.3 (0.4) | 7.7 (0.8) |
Letter M | 12.4 (0.8) | 6.7 (0.4) |
Letter A | 12.5 (2.7) | 6.8 (0.7) |
Letter L | 12.0 (1.7) | 6.8 (0.4) |
Letter C | 15.0 (0.7) | 8.9 (0.9) |
Spanish category fluency** | 10.1 (0.9) | 7.0 (0.6) |
ANIMAL category (Animales) | 15.4 (2.3) | 8.2 (1.1) |
FRUITS category (Frutas) | 10.0 (1.9) | 5.9 (1.1) |
CLOTHES category (Vestidos) | 10.2 (2.2) | 6.2 (1.1) |
COLORS category (Colores) | 12.0 (0.8) | 11.0 (1.0) |
VEGETABLES category (Verduras) | 6.3 (1.0) | 3.6 (0.9) |
Overall Spanish verbal fluency** | 11.4 (0.6) | 7.0 (0.6) |
Between-group differences at p < .01;
between-group difference at p < .05; ns = not significant
Design
The eye-tracking component of the current study followed a 2 × 3 design, with fixations on pictures of cross-linguistic competitors relative to pictures of unrelated filler items as a within-subject variable and with membership in the higher-proficient bilingual group, the lower-proficient bilingual group or the monolingual group as a between-subjects variable. Similarly, the nonlinguistic Stroop component of the study followed a 2 × 3 design, with responses to trials with perceptually competing information (e.g., a left-pointing arrow on the right side), compared to trials without competing information (e.g., a left-pointing arrow on the left side), as a within-subject variable, and with group as a between-subjects variable. Finally, to examine the relationship between parallel language activation and cognitive control, a correlational design was employed where the extent of parallel language activation was correlated with nonlinguistic Stroop performance across participants and across the timecourse of language activation. Dependent variables included accuracy and latency of responses, as well as fixations on images during eye-tracking.
Stimuli
Cross-linguistic co-activation during word recognition
To index cross-linguistic competition, a word recognition eye-tracking paradigm was used. In addition to auditory targets, displays contained pictures of between-language competitors. For example, participants heard the word pool, while a picture of a thumb (Spanish pulgar) was also present, see Figure 1A. Phonological overlap between English targets and Spanish competitors was high, including at least 2 shared word-initial phonemes. Cross-linguistic phonological overlap was also measured in terms of duration of acoustic overlap, and averaged 251.1ms (SE = 31.1) across target-competitor pairs. Recordings were made in a sound-proof booth (44,100 Hz, 16 bits) by a female native speaker of English, using a Marantz Solid State recorder (PMD670). Normalization, segmentation, and phonological overlap analyses of sound files were performed using Praat and Sound Studio software. The resulting sound files were then imported into Superlab experimental software. During the experimental session, the name of the target picture was presented 500ms after the onset of the picture display, consistent with previous eye-tracking studies (e.g., Ju & Luce, 2004; Weber & Cutler, 2004). Stimulus trials were separated by 500ms inter-trial intervals and picture target identification was self-paced, with picture displays disappearing once a response was made.
Stimuli for the cross-linguistic competition task consisted of 20 trials that included cross-linguistic competitors, as well as 40 filler trials where no competitor image was present. The ratio of competitor trials to filler trials was 1:2 in order to minimize awareness of phonological overlap, and to maximize competition effects (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Green, 1998; Henik, Bibi, Yanai, & Tzelgov, 1997). Some of the target-competitor pairs were adapted from Ju and Luce (2004) and from Canseco-Gonzalez et al. (2010). Because Dahan, Magnusson, and Tanenhaus (2001) found that competitor pictures with a word frequency higher than that of the target picture competed with targets most strongly, we controlled the frequencies of our cross-linguistic competitors so that they were similar to English target word frequencies. Spoken word frequencies were obtained in English for the target stimuli (Celex lexical database, Baayen, Piepenbrock, & van Rijn, 1995) and in Spanish for the competitor stimuli (Corpus del Español, www.corpusdelespanol.com). Frequencies, in words per million, were statistically equivalent for English targets (M = 20.1, SE = 5.4) and Spanish competitors (M = 41.1, SE = 14.0), t (32) = 1.5, p > .1. For a list of stimuli, see Appendix A.
Stimulus displays consisted of four pictures and a central fixation cross. The four pictures in each display consisted of (1) the target word, (2) a Spanish competitor word in critical trials and a filler word in filler trials, and (3, 4) two additional filler items. Pictures consisted of black line drawings on a light grey background. Images were obtained from the International Picture Naming Database (Székely et al., 2004), the IMSI Master Clips database, and a previous study (Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007), and were adjusted for line thickness and salience within each display using Photoshop. Each stimulus picture was approximately 5 × 5 cm in size, and the center of each individual picture was located 13cm away from the central fixation cross in the middle of the display. One purpose of the monolingual control group was to ensure that findings were not influenced by differences in visual salience across stimulus displays, with an equal proportion of looks expected to competitors and fillers for monolinguals.
Nonlinguistic Stroop task
The nonlinguistic Stroop task employed in the present study was also used in Blumenfeld and Marian (2011). Participants were asked to respond to arrow direction (either right or left) but to ignore the location of the arrow on the display (either right or left), see Figure 1B–C. These two stimulus dimensions were manipulated to be either congruent or incongruent.2 Participants were instructed to press a response-key located on the left side of the keyboard when they saw a leftward-facing arrow, and a response-key on the right when they saw a rightward-facing arrow. The congruent trials consisted of 60 trials that contained a leftward-facing arrow presented to the left of the central fixation cross, and 60 trials that contained a rightward-facing arrow presented to the right of the central fixation cross. The incongruent trials consisted of 20 trials that contained a leftward-facing arrow presented to the right of the central fixation cross, and 20 trials that contained a rightward-facing arrow presented to the left of the central fixation cross. Each trial started with a 500ms central fixation cross, followed by a 700ms presentation of the stimulus display, and an 800ms presentation of a blank screen. Trials were presented in a fixed pseudo-randomized order.
Procedures and apparatus
After informed consent was obtained, participants completed the eye-tracking part of the study. Participants were not aware of a Spanish component to the task and, prior to completing the English-Spanish eye-tracking task, had been exposed to only English in the laboratory. During a practice session, participants were trained to identify the quadrant containing the correct stimulus target, by pressing one of four keys that were marked on the test computer’s keyboard. These keys corresponded spatially to the location of the quadrant on the stimulus display. Specifically, participants pressed an upper left key to identify a stimulus in the upper left quadrant, an upper right key to identify a stimulus in the upper right quadrant, a bottom left key to identify a stimulus in the bottom left quadrant, and a bottom right key to identify a stimulus in the bottom right quadrant. Participants practiced making responses using the spatially corresponding keys and without looking at the keyboard on 60 practice trials where they were asked to identify a nonlinguistic symbol in different quadrants. After task instructions and practice, participants were calibrated on the headmounted (ISCAN) eye-tracking system. A scene camera captured participants’ field of view, and an infrared camera allowed the software to track the pupil and corneal reflection. Gaze position was indicated by cross-hairs superimposed over the image generated by the scene camera, and participants’ eye-movements were calibrated to 9 points on the computer screen (G4 Macintosh, 27 × 34 cm).
Following eye-tracking, participants completed the nonlinguistic Stroop task and the verbal fluency tasks in English and Spanish. In the fluency tasks, letter sets E, P, M, A, L, and C and category sets Animals, Fruits, Clothes, Colors and Vegetables were counterbalanced across participants and languages, and standard procedures were followed in administration and coding of the task (e.g., Gollan, Montoya, & Werner, 2002). We chose verbal fluency, a generative production measure, as an index of proficiency since it poses greater retrieval challenges than a receptive task, and is thus a better indicator of the robustness of lexical knowledge. Bilingual disadvantages have been identified in category fluency tasks, and these tasks have been accepted as a good measure of lexical knowledge (e.g., Gollan et al., 2002). Lexical knowledge is also a prerequisite to high performance on letter fluency tasks (Gollan et al., 2002; Luo, Luk, & Bialystok, 2010). Participants were median-split into higher- and lower-proficiency groups based on this measure in order to examine parallel language activation and cognitive control skills separately for groups with higher and lower Spanish word retrieval skills. Participants were also administered the non-verbal components of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI, PsychCorp, 1999) as a measure of nonverbal cognitive reasoning, as well as the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Task (PPVT, Dunn & Dunn, 1997) which indexes English receptive vocabulary required in the word recognition task, and a digit span task to account for short term memory (Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999), a cognitive skill that has been shown to influence word retrieval in bilinguals (Kaushanskaya, Blumenfeld, & Marian, 2011). Finally, participants completed the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q, Marian, Blumenfeld, & Kaushanskaya, 2007).
Coding and analyses
For both the critical trials on the eye-tracking task and for the nonlinguistic Stroop task, accuracy rates and reaction times were analyzed, and instances of incorrect picture identification or response latencies beyond 2.5 standard deviations from the mean were excluded from further analyses. In the auditory word recognition task, both by-subject and by-item analyses were performed; in the Stroop task, only by-subject analyses were performed as all items were identical within-condition. The video output from eye-tracking had a temporal resolution of 33.3ms per frame, and was manually coded using Final Cut Pro software. Eye-movements to pictures were coded as fixations if they entered the picture’s quadrant and remained there for at least one frame. Starting at the onset of the stimulus word, and across the timecourse of lexical activation, fixations on the target, competitor, or filler images were counted as “1”s and eye-movements outside of the four areas of interest were coded as “0”s. Data for 20 cross-linguistic trials were collected. Three items were excluded from analyses because, upon closer inspection, they included within-language phonological overlap in addition to between-language overlap.3 The remaining 17 trials were included in eye-tracking analyses. Fifteen percent of all eye-tracking data were re-coded by a second trained coder, and point-to-point inter-rater reliability was .93 (pair-wise Pearson r, p < .001). Since it takes approximately 200 milliseconds to plan an eye-movement (Hallett, 1986), timecourse analyses focused on activation beyond the initial 200ms post word onset. Activation curves between 0 and 2600ms post-word-onset were inspected visually, and looks between 200ms and 1100ms post word onset (when looks to targets had reached a maximum) were analyzed statistically. This approach to analysis of eye-tracking data is consistent with previous approaches (e.g., Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007, 2011; Weber & Cutler, 2004). Finally, to examine how individual differences in inhibitory control may relate to the extent of parallel language activation and how this relationship changes across the timecourse of word recognition, correlations were conducted between Stroop effects (incongruent minus congruent trials) and parallel language activation (looks to competitors minus looks to fillers) for each 33ms time-frame during the parallel language activation window (see Costa, Strijkers, Martin, and Thierry, 2009, for a similar approach with ERP data and behavioral reaction times during naming). We reasoned that, if cognitive control mechanisms were recruited for competition resolution, then correlations between Stroop performance and parallel language activation should be present across multiple adjacent time frames. For the analysis of time series with high autocorrelation, Guthrie and Buchwald (1991) conducted simulations and generated statistical significance thresholds (p < .05) for the probability of finding a series of significant tests in adjacent time windows. We compared our findings against these thresholds to correct for multiple comparisons in correlation analyses. Sporadic correlations in single time frames were treated as chance events for the above-stated theoretical and statistical reasons.
Results
Parallel language activation and bilingual proficiency
Accuracy and reaction time analyses on critical picture identification trials yielded similar performance for monolinguals (accuracy: M = 99.4%, SE = 0.3; reaction times: M = 1622.7ms, SE = 25.6), higher-proficiency bilinguals (accuracy: M = 99.3%, SE = 0.5; reaction times: M = 1719.3ms, SE = 59.7), and lower-proficiency bilinguals (accuracy: M = 97.8%, SE = 0.8, F1 (2,58) = 2.8, p = .07, F2 (2,48) = 2.2, p > .1; reaction times: M = 1708.4ms, SE = 64.3, F1 (2,58) = 1.6, p > .1), F2 (2,48) = 1.4, p > .1). Analyses on the extent of parallel language activation in higher-proficiency and lower-proficiency bilinguals relative to monolinguals during the 200–1100ms time-window were performed using a 2 × 3 ANOVA, with fixation type (competitor, filler) as a within-subject variable and group (higher-proficiency bilingual, lower-proficiency bilingual, monolingual) as a between-subjects variable. Results revealed a main effect of fixation type, F1 (1, 58) = 15.7, p < .001, η p2 = .2, F2 (2, 48) = 7.4, p < .01, η p2 = .1, and a by-subjects interaction between fixation type and group, F1 (2, 58) = 3.3, p < .05, η p2 = .1, F2 (2, 58) = 1.7, p = .2, η p2 = .1 (see Figure 2 and Table 2). Specifically, planned follow-up analyses showed that higher-proficiency bilinguals were more likely to look at cross-linguistic competitors compared to fillers (t1 (14) = 3.6, p < .01, t2 (16) = 2.5, p < .05), with equal percentages of looks to competitors and fillers for lower-proficiency bilinguals (t1 (15) = 1.2, p > .1, t2 (16) = 0.7, p > .1) and for monolinguals (t1 (29) = 1.6, p > .1, t2 (16) = 1.6, p > .1).
Table 2.
Competitor pictures % (SE) | Filler pictures % (SE) | Competition effect (competitor – filler) | |
---|---|---|---|
Higher-proficiency bilinguals | 15.8 (1.1) | 11.0 (0.5) | 4.8* |
Lower-proficiency bilinguals | 12.5 (0.9) | 11.3 (0.8) | 1.2% |
Monolinguals | 11.2 (0.9) | 9.8 (0.7) | 1.4% |
p < .01
Parallel language activation, language proficiency, and inhibitory control
To examine whether bilingual proficiency and parallel language activation can be directly linked to cognitive control, participants’ eye-tracking results were examined relative to their performance on the nonlinguistic Stroop task. Accuracy rates and reaction times during Stroop performance were entered into a 2 × 3 ANOVA, with trial type (incongruent, congruent) as a within-subject variable and group (higher-proficiency bilinguals, lower-proficiency bilinguals, monolinguals) as a between-subjects variable. For accuracy rates, a main effect of trial type was identified, F (1, 58) = 81.0, p < .001, η p2 = .6, with overall less accurate performance on incongruent trials (M = 85.0%, SE = 1.6) than on congruent trials (M = 91.1%, SE = 0.4), see Table 3. No interaction between trial type and group and no main effect of group were found, ps > .1. Reaction time analyses yielded a main effect of trial type, F (1, 58) = 498.4, p < .001, η p2 = .9, no main effect of group, F (2, 58) = .05, p > .5, and an interaction between trial type and group, F (2, 58) = 5.8, p < .01, η p2 = .2, suggesting slower responses on incongruent (M = 457.0ms, SE = 7.4) than on congruent (M = 370.4ms, SE = 6.4) trials (see Figure 3). Follow-up comparisons of Stroop effects (incongruent trials minus congruent trials) across groups suggested that higher-proficiency bilinguals had smaller Stroop effects (M = 66.9ms, SE = 5.7) than lower-proficiency bilinguals (M = 99.5ms, SE = 7.2), t (29) = 3.5, p = .001, or monolinguals (M = 93.6ms, SE = 5.8), t (43) = 4.3, p < .01. The Stroop effects were statistically equivalent in lower-proficiency bilinguals and monolinguals, t (44) = 0.6, p > .5. These findings suggest that non-target language proficiency mediated both the extent of parallel language activation and the ability to inhibit competing information in the nonlinguistic domain, with bilinguals who showed greater parallel language activation also showing reduced nonlinguistic Stroop effects.
Table 3.
Congruent | Accuracy rates % (SE) Incongruent | Overall | Congruent | Reaction times Milliseconds (SE) Incongruent | Overall | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Higher-proficiency bilinguals | 99.5 (0.8) | 89.6 (3.1) | 94.5 (1.7) | 380.3 (12.2) | 447.2 (14.3) | 413.8 (12.7) |
Lower-proficiency bilinguals | 99.2 (0.7) | 83.7 (3.0) | 91.4 (1.7) | 361.6 (11.8) | 461.0 (13.8) | 411.3 (12.3) |
Monolinguals | 98.6 (0.5) | 81.8 (2.2) | 90.2 (1.2) | 369.2 (8.6) | 462.9 (10.1) | 416.0 (9.0) |
To examine the link between parallel language activation and inhibitory control abilities, correlations were performed between the two across the window where parallel language activation was present (between 200–833ms, resulting in comparisons across 20 time frames). In higher and lower proficiency bilingual participants combined, parallel language activation effects (competitor fixations minus filler fixations) were correlated with Stroop effects (incongruent trials minus congruent trials). Significant negative correlation coefficients were identified between 300–500ms post word onset (r = −.5, p = .01, 95% confidence interval for r = −.3 to −.6). In addition, significant positive correlation coefficients were identified between 633–767ms post word onset (r = .4, p < .05, 95% confidence interval for r = .1 to .6). Notably, the negative correlations between 300–500ms corresponded to the timecourse of competitor activation (increased differences between competitor vs. filler fixations), while the positive correlations between 633–767ms corresponded to competitor de-activation (reduced differences between competitor vs. filler fixations, see Figure 4A, open triangles). These findings are consistent with our expectations of multiple consecutive significant correlations across the course of parallel activation. Both windows where consecutive significant correlations were present passed the threshold of significance within a 10-frame sequence where autocorrelation (lag 1) = .9, p < .05 (Guthrie & Buchwald, 1991). A Fisher r-to-z transformation suggested that the two correlation coefficients were significantly different from each other, z = 3.3, p < .001. This pattern suggests that bilinguals who showed strong parallel language activation between 300–500ms and reduced parallel language activation between 633–767ms also showed smaller Stroop effects (see Figure 4B–C). To examine whether similar patterns were present in higher- and lower-proficiency bilinguals, correlation analyses between Stroop performance and parallel language activation were also performed for each group separately. Patterns toward negative correlations were identified in the 300–500ms time-window for both higher-proficiency (r = −.4, p = .1) and lower-proficiency groups (r = −.4, p = .2), and significant positive correlations were found in the 633–767ms time-window for both higher-proficiency (r = .5, p < .05) and lower-proficiency groups (r = .6, p = .01). When the same correlation analyses were conducted in the monolingual control group, only temporary (one frame in terms of the eye-tracking sampling rate) and inconsistent positive correlations were found at 200–233ms, at 400–433ms, and at 467–500ms (all ps < .05). It is possible that these correlations were driven by monolinguals who performed well on the Stroop task and made overall fewer looks to any non-target items. However, their temporary nature suggests that no consistent pattern was present in the monolingual data linking eye-movements during comprehension to Stroop inhibition.
To more closely examine how Stroop performance influences the timecourse of parallel language activation, bilinguals were median-split into individuals with smaller Stroop effects (M = 59.2ms, SE = 4.2) and larger Stroop effects (M = 106.7ms, SE = 5.1), t (29) = 7.1, p < .001. The bilinguals with smaller Stroop effects looked to competitors (M = 25.5%, SE = 2.7) significantly more frequently than to fillers (M = 15.5%, SE = 1.4) between 300–500ms post stimulus onset (see Figure 5A), t1 (14) = 2.7, p < .05, t2 (16) = 1.2, p > .1. The bilinguals with larger Stroop effects looked to competitors (M = 15.3%, SE = 1.4) significantly more frequently than to fillers (M = 9.4%, SE = 1.0) between 567–833ms post stimulus onset (see Figure 5B), t1 (15) = 3.3, p < .01, t2 (16) = 2.3, p < .05. A 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA with stimulus type (competitor, filler) and time-window (300–500ms, 567–833ms) as within-subject variables and with group (bilinguals with smaller Stroop effects, bilinguals with larger Stroop effects) as a between-subjects variable yielded a significant 3-way interaction, suggesting that the two groups differed significantly in the timecourse of parallel language activation, F1 (1,29) = 15.4, p < .001, ηp2 = .4, F2 (1,32) = 6.5, p < .05, ηp2 = .2. These findings suggest that differences in the timecourse of activation across groups with smaller vs. larger Stroop effects may contribute to correlations between parallel language activation and Stroop performance. Together, correlation analyses between parallel language activation and Stroop performance suggest that bilinguals showed a close and continuous relationship between the two processes, and that the nature of this relationship changed across the timecourse of competitor activation.
Discussion
In the current study, we examined whether parallel language activation during auditory word recognition is directly related to bilingual advantages in inhibitory control. Using a cross-linguistic visual world eye-tracking task and a nonlinguistic Stroop task, we identified a link between parallel language activation and Stroop performance, with higher-proficiency bilinguals showing stronger parallel language activation and cognitive advantages in the form of smaller Stroop effects. Moreover, across all bilinguals, correlational relationships emerged between parallel language activation and Stroop performance. Higher bilingual proficiency and better Stroop performance were associated with higher cross-linguistic activation during word recognition early in the timecourse (300–500ms post word onset). During a later time-window (633–767ms post word onset), better Stroop performance became associated with less parallel language activation. Bilinguals with better Stroop performance showed deactivation of competitors during this later time-window, while bilinguals with less efficient Stroop performance showed competitor activation. These findings suggest a dynamic relationship between cross-linguistic activation and cognitive control, where better Stroop inhibition abilities are associated with early competitor activation.
The present findings suggest that the nature of the relationship between parallel language activation and Stroop inhibition changes across time. These findings can be interpreted within the framework of models of bilingual receptive processing, such as the Bilingual Language Interaction Network for Comprehension of Speech (BLINCS, Shook & Marian, 2012) or the Bilingual Interactive Activation model (BIA, Dijkstra & van Heuven, 2002; van Heuven, Dijkstra, & Grainger, 1998). In these models, early stages of comprehension are characterized by automatic and parallel bottom-up activation of representations. Following this initial parallel activation process, higher-level cognitive control mechanisms are available to guide word selection. Our findings fit well with the two stages exemplified by the BIA and BLINCS, with higher-proficiency bilinguals showing strong parallel activation during early word recognition (300–500ms post word onset) and recruitment of cognitive control mechanisms to resolve competition later on.
It should be noted that studies linking better inhibition to less cross-linguistic activation have used production tasks that rely on response output measures (e.g., naming, Gollan, Sandoval & Salmon, 2011; Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008), thus indexing performance at a later stage where inhibitory control would likely have already been applied to resolve competition. In contrast, the present study employed an online word recognition task that targeted early stages of comprehension where activation was automatic. Therefore, the present study captures both automatic cross-linguistic activation and competition resolution stages during the timecourse of word recognition.
The role of proficiency and cognitive control across the timecourse of auditory word recognition
The findings of the present study suggest that the relationship between parallel language activation and Stroop performance is likely to change over the timecourse of word recognition. While the current study cannot make claims about the direction of the relationship between proficiency, parallel language activation, and Stroop performance, cognitive control and parallel activation are likely to mutually influence each other as bilingual proficiency is developed. One possible progression in the development of the patterns identified in the current study is that (1) the non-target language must reach a certain proficiency level before it becomes routinely and automatically co-activated; (2) the need to resolve cross-linguistic competition due to parallel activation may engage and hone cognitive control mechanisms; and finally, (3) as cognitive control mechanisms become more efficient, associated nonlinguistic and linguistic competition resolution becomes more efficient. In addition, it is likely that pre-existing individual differences in bilinguals’ inhibitory control contribute to competition resolution. For example, previous studies have suggested that increased cross-linguistic activation can be linked to reduced inhibitory control abilities (Gollan, Sandoval, & Salmon, 2011; Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008), suggesting that while cross-linguistic competition may hone inhibitory control, proficient bilinguals with weaker cognitive control mechanisms will nevertheless experience more cross-linguistic interference.
Results identified in the present study map onto the hypothesized progression of high bilingual proficiency, cross-linguistic competition, and honed cognitive control/linguistic conflict resolution: First, bilinguals who showed higher proficiency in the non-target language also showed increased parallel language activation (for similar findings, see Blumenfeld & Marian, 2007). Second, bilinguals with better Stroop performance also showed more parallel language activation early during word recognition (negative correlation between 300–500ms post word onset), supporting the hypothesis that the presence of cross-linguistic competition hones aspects of cognitive control. It is possible that the timecourse differences in competitor activation that were identified for bilinguals with smaller vs. larger Stroop effects are mediated by language proficiency. For example, Blumenfeld and Marian (2007, cognate condition) showed co-activation of German competitors earlier in the activation timecourse for German-English bilinguals than for English-German bilinguals. Similarly, Dahan, Magnuson and Tanenhaus (2001) showed earlier activation of higher-frequency words. In this sense, the negative correlation between 300–500ms may reflect identification of individuals who show the most immediate and automatized parallel language activation, a pre-condition of high cross-linguistic competition and of potential benefits to cognitive performance. Conversely, the positive correlation between 633–767ms may reflect actual engagement of cognitive control mechanisms, since less parallel language activation corresponds to better Stroop performance during this time period. While this explanation is consistent with current research on the emergence of bilingual cognitive advantages (e.g., Bialystok & Craik, 2010; Green, 2011; Kroll, 2008), future studies may further examine the development of the identified relationships between language activation and Stroop performance.
Parallel language activation as a source of a bilingual cognitive advantage
Regardless of the specific explanation of correlations between Stroop performance and parallel activation across the timecourse of performance, the present findings suggest that, when bilinguals were divided into higher-proficiency and lower-proficiency speakers in the non-target language (L2), the higher-proficiency group showed more parallel language activation and better Stroop performance. These differences between higher and lower Spanish proficiency groups were observed despite similarity in the two groups on nonverbal IQ, digit span performance, and English proficiency. Moreover, a direct relationship was established between Stroop performance and parallel language activation across two continuous time-windows during auditory word identification. These findings support the prediction that bilingual parallel language activation can indeed be directly and systematically linked to the involvement of cognitive control mechanisms. Crucially, bilingual parallel language activation was linked to Stroop performance, a type of inhibitory control that has repeatedly yielded advantages in bilinguals compared to monolinguals (e.g., Bialystok, 2006; Bialystok, Craik, & Luk, 2008; Bialystok & DePape, 2009; Costa et al., 2008, 2009; Hernandez et al., 2010; Luk, de Sa, & Bialystok, 2011) and that has been shown to rely on different neural pathways in bilinguals vs. monolinguals (Luk, Anderson, Craik, Grady, & Bialystok, 2010), suggesting its modulation with bilingual experience. In fact, in the current study, higher-proficiency bilinguals showed advantages on the nonlinguistic Stroop task relative to lower-proficiency bilinguals and monolinguals.
Finding a Stroop inhibition advantage only in higher-proficiency bilinguals is consistent with previous literature where groups of bilinguals who showed advantages were described as highly proficient and immersed in both languages (Hilchey & Klein, 2011; Vega & Fernandez, 2011). Findings are also consistent with a recent study by Singh and Mishra (2012) who adapted a color-word Stroop task for eye-tracking and found that higher-proficiency Hindi-English bilinguals fixated on relevant information and resolved Stroop competition more quickly than lower-proficiency bilinguals. The proficiency measure used in the current study, a generative verbal fluency task, provides insight into the robustness of bilinguals’ lexical representations and is sensitive to bilingual proficiency differences (Gollan et al., 2002). However, the letter fluency component of this task has also been linked to bilingual advantages in cognitive control when proficiency is accounted for (e.g., Luo, Luk, & Bialystok, 2010), a finding that is perhaps not surprising given that correlations have been identified between cognitive control skills and a number of language tasks with high retrieval demands (e.g., Gollan, Sandoval, & Salmon, 2012; Linck, Kroll, & Hoshino, 2008; Pivneva, Palmer, & Titone, 2012) or switching components (Festman & Münte, 2012; Festman, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Münte, 2010; Prior & Gollan, 2011; Prior & MacWhinney, 2010). Therefore, since functional proficiency is likely to be measured more successfully with generative tasks that are more demanding, cognitive control abilities and measured proficiency are likely to be closely interconnected. As such, we can conclude that generative proficiency measures pattern together with a nonverbal cognitive control measure in predicting the extent and timecourse of parallel language activation.
Given the group-level and correlational links between parallel language activation, bilingual proficiency, and Stroop performance, the hypothesis is supported that substantial experience with parallel language activation and cross-linguistic competition resolution may be one “training ground” for bilingual advantages on Stroop-type cognitive control tasks. The idea that increased competition might result in increased use of dedicated cognitive mechanisms is grounded in the monolingual literature. Under the assumption that bilinguals use similar cognitive tools as monolinguals to support language processing, the monolingual literature provides support for within-language lexical competition (Marslen-Wilson, 1987; McClelland & Elman, 1986) and recruitment of cognitive control mechanisms to resolve competition during auditory comprehension (Desroches, Newman, & Joanisse, 2009; Gernsbacher & Faust, 1991; Swinney, 1979; Taylor, O’Hara, Mumenthaler, Rosen, & Yesavage, 2005). In bilinguals, increased competition likely comes in the form of a combination of within-language and between-language competition. Intriguingly, in addition to within-language and between-language competition, bilinguals may also face additional lexical competition in L2 that is a result of non-native phonetic categories (e.g. Broersma & Cutler, 2011; Weber & Cutler, 2004). For example, when within-language activation was probed in their L2 English, Dutch speakers considered an image of a panda more than native English speakers upon hearing pencil, since the first vowels of these words are confusable to native Dutch speakers (i.e., the underlined vowels are part of the same vowel category in Dutch, Weber & Cutler, 2004). In sum, given additional lexical competition in bilinguals’ auditory comprehension, one source of a cognitive advantage may be increased recruitment of inhibitory control mechanisms to resolve competition. In the present study, a link between parallel language activation and cognitive control is identified in bilinguals, relative to monolinguals who do not show crosslinguistic activation and, consistently, do not recruit Stroop skills during the current eye-tracking task.
The role of Stroop inhibition in bilingual language processing
While, in the present study, Stroop-type inhibitory control was linked to parallel language activation, it remains an open question whether Stroop-type inhibition is indeed the specific mechanism that supports competition resolution during comprehension, or whether related types of inhibition are directly involved. Stroop-type inhibition is a reasonable candidate for involvement in competition resolution during comprehension, and can be examined within the framework of the Dimensional Overlap Model (Kornblum et al., 1999), a model that accounts for a variety of different inhibition mechanisms. On the classic Stroop task, there are two stimulus dimensions: the color of a word’s ink (e.g., blue, green) and the meaning of the word (e.g., blue, green). The Dimensional Overlap Model posits that this conflict between two dimensions of the same stimulus (i.e., stimulus-stimulus conflict) originates at the perceptual level. Similarly, in the nonlinguistic Stroop task employed in the current study, the two conflicting stimulus dimensions are arrow direction (right, left) and arrow location (right, left). The inhibition mechanism employed to resolve Stroop conflict may therefore be similar to the mechanism employed to resolve phonological competition because, in both cases, the perceived stimulus (e.g., a color-word in a differently-colored ink on a classic Stroop task or an auditory word during comprehension) initially can be mapped onto two concepts. That is, perceptual/conceptual conflict is inherent to one bivalent stimulus, and this conflict must be resolved in order to yield the most appropriate interpretation (e.g., the ink color is red not blue, or the word is cat not cab).
In the current study, the Stroop effect was derived by comparing reaction time differences between congruent and incongruent trials. Previous research (Liu et al., 2004) had identified neural correlates for incongruent vs. congruent Stroop conditions that were consistent with cognitive control employed during bilingual language processing (Abutalebi, 2008). Further, Blumenfeld and Marian (2011) identified a correlation between within-language linguistic conflict resolution and the incongruent-congruent spatial Stroop effect, also pointing to a possible link between Stroop performance and bilingual processing. In the current study, higher-proficiency bilinguals recruited cognitive mechanisms during parallel language activation that also afforded them greater skill at ignoring irrelevant information. On the Stroop task, the smaller incongruent-congruent effect was driven by shorter response times to incongruent trials, suggesting less interference from irrelevant information, as well as longer response times to congruent trials, suggesting less facilitation from irrelevant information (see Table 3). However, it remains an open question whether the facilitation and inhibition components on the Stroop task have shared or separate underlying mechanisms (e.g., MacLeod, 1991; Roelofs, 2010; van Heuven et al., 2011), and future research is needed to identify whether mechanisms that underlie facilitation and inhibition components of the Stroop task are differentially recruited during parallel language activation. In the current data, post-hoc analyses suggest that reaction times on incongruent trials were closely related to the incongruent-congruent Stroop effect (r = .6, p = .001), while reaction times on congruent trials were not related to the incongruent-congruent effect (r = −.001, p > .5). This pattern suggests that the Stroop inhibition component may be primarily responsible for the correlation with parallel language activation. Direct correlations between incongruent trial reaction times and parallel activation confirm this conclusion --for the 300–500ms window, r = −.2 for incongruent trials and .028 for congruent trials; for the 633–767ms window, r = .3 for incongruent trials and −.006 for congruent trials. Therefore, we can conclude that the primary mechanism indexed by the incongruent-congruent Stroop effect and recruited to resolve parallel language activation is related to inhibition of incongruent information.
As previously discussed, the linguistic sources of bilingual advantages are likely diverse, given the wide range of language processing contexts in which bilinguals may experience higher cognitive demands. For example, a number of studies have linked bilingual processing and learning to enhanced performance on the Simon task (Bartolotti, Marian, Schroeder, & Shook, 2011; Linck, Hoshino, & Kroll, 2008; Linck, Schwieter, & Sunderman, 2011). Since the source of conflict on the Simon task is between stimulus and response mappings (e.g., Kornblum et al., 1999), it is possible that competition during bilingual language production (where activated information must be mapped onto one output) is more likely to drive bilingual advantages on the Simon task. In addition to conflict between features within a bivalent stimulus (location, direction), the nonlinguistic Stroop task employed in the current study also includes stimulus-response conflict (Kornblum et al., 1999; Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008). Specifically, the irrelevant stimulus dimension (right display side, left display side) can be either congruent or incongruent with the side where the response is made (right key, left key). Therefore, the current Stroop task requires conflict resolution at two levels: the stimulus level and the response level. Since the early stages of auditory word recognition do not require an explicit response component, with parallel activation effects occurring before 1000ms post word onset, and with overt responses occurring only around 1700ms post word onset, we reasoned that response-level conflict resolution components would not be recruited to resolve conflict during the early word recognition stages. To completely rule out the possibility that stimulus-response conflict resolution mechanisms play a role in early auditory word recognition, future work will need to employ a nonverbal Stroop task where no stimulus-response conflict is present (e.g., Liu, Banich, Jacobsen, & Tanabe, 2004). Further research can examine how different aspects of language processing may uniquely relate and contribute to various types of cognitive control tasks.
In addition to the possibility that demands in different language contexts hone different types of cognitive control, the nature of the bilingual experience may influence the cognitive system both quantitatively and qualitatively. For example, Tao et al. (2011) found that early bilinguals showed overall reaction time advantages, while late bilinguals showed advantages in conflict resolution. Cognitive demands and bilingual advantages may also differ in individuals who do and do not code-switch (Green, 2011) or who engage in challenging tasks such as simultaneous interpreting (Christoffels, De Groot, & Kroll, 2006). Moreover, competition within one processing modality (e.g., auditory) may be related to bilingual advantages. Emmorey, Luk, Pyers and Bialystok (2008) identified better performance on the flanker task in unimodal but not bimodal bilinguals, even though cross-linguistic (i.e., cross-modal) competition has been identified during comprehension in bimodal bilinguals (Morford, Wilkinson, Villwock, Piñar, & Kroll, 2011; Shook & Marian, 2012). In sum, a diverse range of factors (including language experience, type of processing, and task demands) is likely to influence the nature and extent of bilingual cognitive advantages.
Conclusion
In the present study, we identified direct relationships between parallel language activation and Stroop-type inhibition in early English-Spanish bilinguals. Findings suggest that bilinguals who perform well on the Stroop task show early cross-linguistic competitor activation and that higher-proficiency bilinguals show more parallel language activation and more efficient Stroop inhibition. Results support the hypothesis that cross-linguistic competition in bilinguals engages Stroop-type domain-general inhibition, and contribute to the ongoing research effort to specify the linguistic origins of bilingual cognitive advantages.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Dr. Guillaume Thierry, Dr. Marcel Giezen, Dr. Joey Lin, and Dr. Margarita Kaushanskaya, members of the Bilingualism and Psycholinguistics Laboratory at Northwestern University, including James Bartolotti, Anthony Shook, Scott Schroeder, Sarah Chabal, Jennifer Krizman, and Xin Wang, and members of the Bilingualism and Cognition Laboratory at San Diego State University, including Vanessa Howes and Shirlene Wade, for helpful discussions of this work, as well as Dr. Judy Kroll and two anonymous reviewers for feedback on a previous version of this manuscript. This project was supported in part by a John and Lucille Clarke Dissertation Scholarship and San Diego State University Grant Program grant #242338 to the first author, and by grant NICHD 1R01HD059858 to the second author.
APPENDIX A.
English Target | Spanish Competitor | Competitor Translation into English | Filler 1 | Filler 2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | comb | conejo | rabbit | sword | rail |
2 | pea | pie | foot | window | boy |
3 | peal | pila | battery | whistle | globe |
4 | pool | pulgar | thumb | candle | log |
5 | plug | plancha | iron | button | |
6 | mice | maíz | corn | jar | glove |
7 | pliers | playa | beach | wood | collar |
8 | luggage | lata | Can | pigeon | skis |
9 | eagle | iglesia | church | letter | present |
10 | poodle | puño | fist | ruler | olive |
11 | cup | caballo | horse | wheel | bench |
12 | song | sombrero | hat | queen | bear |
13 | puppy | pan | bread | music | rose |
14 | corn | cola | tail | skirt | bride |
15 | yarn | llave | key | sink | roof |
16 | soap | sobre | envelope | castle | lion |
17 | nun | naranja | orange | pipe | belt |
Footnotes
Thirty monolinguals were recruited to ensure that null findings in monolinguals’ correlations between eye-tracking and cognitive control measures were not due to a lack of power.
The spatial Stroop task in the current study consisted of only congruent and incongruent trials, consistent with Liu, Banich, Jacobsen and Tanabe (2004) and with Blumenfeld and Marian (2011). In this study, we aimed to establish an initial link between parallel language activation and a cognitive task that has been shown to have neural correlates (Liu et al, 2004) consistent with those of cognitive control processes in bilinguals (Abutalebi, 2008; Liu et al., 2004). We reasoned that, if participants can inhibit irrelevant location information, then incongruent location information should interfere less with their response and congruent location information should provide less facilitation toward their response, thus providing a reduced difference score between congruent and incongruent conditions.
The three target-competitor pairs that were omitted because of within-language overlap were (1) target seal and Spanish competitor silla (“chair”) due to the English synonym seat; (2) target beans and Spanish competitor bigote (“moustache”) due to the English word beard; and (3) target female and Spanish competitor fiesta (“party”) due to the English word feast and because monolingual participants were likely familiar with the word fiesta.
Contributor Information
Henrike K. Blumenfeld, San Diego State University
Viorica Marian, Northwestern University.
References
- Abutalebi J. Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta psychologica. 2008;128(3):466–78. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baayen H, Piepenbrock R, van Rijn H. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Linguistic Data Consortium; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Bartolotti J, Marian V, Schroeder SR, Shook A. Bilingualism and inhibitory control influence statistical learning of novel word forms. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011 Nov;2:324. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E. Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2006;60(1):68–79. doi: 10.1037/cjep2006008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E, Craik FIM. Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010;19(1):19–23. doi: 10.1177/0963721409358571. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E, Craik F, Luk G. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008;34(4):859–873. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Klein R, Viswanathan M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging. 2004;19:290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E, DePape A-M. Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive function. Journal of Experimental Psychology; Human Perception and Performance. 2009;35(2):265–274. doi: 10.1037/a0012735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenfeld HK, Marian V. Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition. 2011;118(2):245–257. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenfeld HK, Marian V. Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2007;22(5):633–660. doi: 10.1080/01690960601000746. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review. 2001;108(3):624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broersma M, Cutler A. Competition dynamics of second-language listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2011;64(1):74–95. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.499174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canseco-Gonzalez E, Brehm L, Brick CA, Brown-Schmidt S, Fischer K, Wagner K. Carpet or Cárcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2010;25:669–705. doi: 10.1080/01690960903474912. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers CG, Cooke H. Lexical competition during second-language listening: sentence context, but not proficiency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition. 2009;35(4):1029–1040. doi: 10.1037/a0015901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoffels IK, De Groot AMB, Kroll JF. Memory and language skills in simultaneous interpreters: The role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;54(3):324–345. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Costa A, Hernandez M, Costa-Faidella J, Sebastian-Galles N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition. 2009;113(2):135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa A, Hernandez M, Sebastian-Galles N. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition. 2008;106(1):59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa A, Strijkers K, Martin C, Thierry G. The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106(50):21442–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908921106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler A, Weber A, Otake T. Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics. 2006;34(2):269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2005.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dahan D, Magnusson JS, Tanenhaus MK. Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology. 2001;42:317–367. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desroches AS, Newman RL, Joanisse MF. Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(10):1893–906. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijkstra T, van Heuven WJB. The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2002;5(03) doi: 10.1017/S1366728902003012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Emmorey K, Luk G, Pyers JE, Bialystok E. The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals. Psychological Science. 2008;19(12):1201–1206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festman J, Münte TF. Cognitive control in Russian-german bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012 Apr;3(115) doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festman J, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Münte TF. Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and brain functions. 2010;6:5. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gernsbacher MA, Faust ME. The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1991;17(2):245–62. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollan TH, Montoya RI, Werner GA. Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:562–576. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.4.562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollan TH, Sandoval T, Salmon DP. Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science. 2011;22(9):1155–64. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green DW. Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011 May;2:103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green DW. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 1998;1:67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie D, Buchwald JS. Significance testing of difference potentials. Psychophysiology. 1991;28(2):240–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb00417.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallett PE. Eye movements. In: Boff K, Kaufman L, Thomas J, editors. Handbook of perception and human performance. Vol. 1. New York: Wiley-Interscience; 1986. pp. 10–112. [Google Scholar]
- Henik A, Bibi U, Yanai M, Tzelgov J. The Stroop effect is largest during first trials. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. 1997;2:57. [Google Scholar]
- Hernández M, Costa A, Fuentes LJ, Vivas AB, Sebástian-Gallés N. The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2010;13(3):315–325. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hilchey MD, Klein RM. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2011;18:625–658. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ju M, Luce PA. Falling on sensitive ears: constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science. 2004;15:314–318. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00675.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaushanskaya M, Blumenfeld HK, Marian V. The relationship between short-term memory and vocabulary measures in monolingual and bilingual speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2011;15(4):408–425. doi: 10.1177/1367006911403201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornblum S, Stevens G, Whipple A, Requin J. The effects of irrelevant stimuli I: The time course of S-S and S-R consistency effects with Stroop-like stimuli (DO Type 4 task), Simon-like tasks (DO Type 3 task), and their factorial combinations (DO Type 7 task) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1999;25:688–714. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.3.688. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krizman J, Marian V, Shook a, Skoe E, Kraus N. Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012:1–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201575109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroll JF. Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychological Science Agenda, American Psychological Association. 2008:22. Retrieved on July 14 2012 from http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2008/01/kroll.aspx.
- Kroll JF, Bobb SC, Misra M, Guo T. Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica. 2008;128:416–430. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BJ, McVeigh ND, Marful A, Anderson MC. Inhibiting your native language: the role of retrieval-induced forgetting during second-language acquisition. Psychological Science. 2007;18:29–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01844.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linck JA, Hoshino N, Kroll JF. Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon. 2008;3(3):349–374. doi: 10.1075/ml.3.3.06lin. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linck JA, Schwieter JW, Sunderman G. Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2011:1–12. doi: 10.1017/S136672891100054X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Banich MT, Jacobson BL, Tanabe JL. Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage. 2004;22(3):1097–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luk G, Anderson JAE, Craik FIM, Grady C, Bialystok E. Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition. 2010;74:347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luk G, de Sa E, Bialystok E. Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2011 doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Luo L, Luk G, Bialystok E. Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition. 2010;114(1):29–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macizo P, Bajo T, Martín MC. Inhibitory processes in bilingual language comprehension: evidence from Spanish-English interlexical homographs. Journal of Memory and Language. 2010;63:232–244. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological bulletin. 1991;109(2):163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martín MC, Macizo P, Bajo T. Time course of inhibitory processes in bilingual language processing. British Journal of Psychology. 2010;101(4):679–93. doi: 10.1348/000712609X480571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marian V, Blumenfeld HK, Kaushanskaya M. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2007;50(4):940–967. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marian V, Blumenfeld HK, Boukrina O. Sensitivity to phonological similarity within and across languages. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2008;37:141–170. doi: 10.1007/s10936-007-9064-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marian V, Spivey M. Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2003a;24:173–193. doi: 10.1017.S0142716403000092. [Google Scholar]
- Marian V, Spivey M. Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2003b;6:97–115. doi: 10.1017/S1366728903001068. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marslen-Wilson WD. Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition. 1987;25:71–102. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(87)90005-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martín MC, Macizo P, Bajo T. Time course of inhibitory processes in bilingual language processing. British Journal of Psychology. 2010;101(4):679–93. doi: 10.1348/000712609X480571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin-Rhee MM, Bialystok E. The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2008;11(01):81–93. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McClelland JL, Elman JL. The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology. 1986;18(1):1–86. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michael E, Gollan TH. Being and becoming bilingual: Individual differences and consequences for language production. In: Kroll JF, De Groot AMB, editors. Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 389–407. [Google Scholar]
- Misra M, Guo T, Bobb SC, Kroll JF. When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language. 2012;67(1):224–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales L, Paolieri D, Bajo T. Grammatical gender inhibition in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011 Oct;2:284. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morford J, Wilkinson E, Villwock A, Piñar P, Kroll J. When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition. 2011;118(2):286–292. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pivneva I, Palmer C, Titone D. Inhibitory Control and L2 Proficiency Modulate Bilingual Language Production: Evidence from Spontaneous Monologue and Dialogue Speech. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012 Mar;3:1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prior A, Gollan T. Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;17(4):682–691. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prior A, MacWhinney B. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2010;13(2):253–262. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- PsychCorp. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Roelofs A. Attention and facilitation: converging information versus inadvertent reading in Stroop task performance. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition. 2010;36(2):411–22. doi: 10.1037/a0018523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salvatierra JL, Rosselli M. The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2011;15(1):26–37. doi: 10.1177/1367006910371021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder S, Marian V. A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2012 doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.669367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz AI, Kroll JF. Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;55(2):197–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Segalowitz N, Hulstijn J. Automaticity in bilingualism and second language learning. In: Kroll JF, de Groot ABB, editors. Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 371–388. [Google Scholar]
- Shook A, Marian V. Bimodal bilinguals coactivate both languages during spoken comprehension. Cognition. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shook A, Marian V. The Bilingual Language Interaction Network for Comprehension of Speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2012 doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soveri A, Laine M, Hämäläinen H, Hugdahl K. Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2010;14(03):371–378. doi: 10.1017/S1366728910000118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Singh N, Mishra RK. Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2012:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Swinney DA. Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979;18:645–659. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90355-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Székely A, Jacobsen T, D’Amico S, Devescovi A, Andonova E, Herron D, Lu CC, Pechmann T, Pleh C, Wicha N, Federmeier K, Gerdjikova I, Gutierrez G, Hung D, Hsu J, Iyer G, Kohnert K, Mehotcheva T, Orozco-Figueroa A, Tzeng A, Tzeng O, Arevalo A, Vargha A, Butler AC, Buffington R, Bates E. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of Memory and Language. 2004;51(2):247–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanenhaus M, Magnuson J, Dahan D, Chambers C. Eye movements and lexical access in spoken language comprehension: Evaluating a linking hypothesis between fixations and linguistic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2000;29:557–580. doi: 10.1023/a:1026464108329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanenhaus MK, Spivey-Knowlton MJ, Eberhard KM, Sedivy JC. Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science. 1995;268:1632–1634. doi: 10.1126/science.7777863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tao L, Marzecová A, Taft M, Asanowicz D, Wodniecka Z. The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011 Jun;2:123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JL, O’Hara R, Mumenthaler MS, Rosen AC, Yesavage JA. Cognitive ability, expertise, and age differences in following air-traffic control instructions. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(1):117–133. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandeberg L, Guadalupe T, Zwaan RA. How verbs can activate things: Cross-language activation across word classes. Acta Psychologica. 2011;138(1):68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Heuven WJB, Conklin K, Coderre EL, Guo T, Dijkstra T. The Influence of Cross-Language Similarity on within- and between-Language Stroop Effects in Trilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011 Dec;2:374. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Heuven WJBV, Dijkstra T, Grainger J. Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language. 1998;39:458–483. [Google Scholar]
- Vega C, Fernandez M. Errors on the WCST correlate with language proficiency scores in Spanish–English bilinguals. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2011;26:158–164. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Pro-Ed, Inc; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Weber A, Cutler A. Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language. 2004;50(1):1–25. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00105-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weber A, Paris G. The origin of the linguistic gender effect in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from non-native listening. Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 2004:1446–1451. [Google Scholar]