Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Dec 2.
Published in final edited form as: Vis cogn. 2013 May 10;21(2):10.1080/13506285.2013.796035. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.796035

Appendix.

(Abbreviations used: HP: holistic processing; CD: complete design; PD: partial design)

Claim Reply
#2: “Holistic face processing has received
several definitions that are not fundamentally
different from each other.”
This is an opinion. See Richler et al. (2012) for
a different opinion about various operational
definitions of HP.
#16: “Part-based analytic processing is
relatively well-preserved for inverted faces,
but…perception of the individual face as a
whole is impaired by inversion.”
See Richler, Mack et al. (2011) for data
indicating that inverted faces are processed
holistically, and McKone & Yovel (2009) for a
review of feature inversion effects (i.e.,
impairment in recognition of individual
features in inverted faces).
#22: “Individuation of a nonface object from
another member of the same category appears
to rely essentially on part-based analysis.”
The papers cited in support of this claim do
not take perceptual expertise into account.
#23: “…studies that have applied the
composite paradigm with nonface objects have
failed to report any composite effects.”
See Bukach et al. (2010) and Wong et al.(2009a)
for examples where HP was observed
for non-face objects of expertise in the
composite task.
#28: Holistic processing is supported by LSF
(vs. HSF) information.
See Cheung et al. (2008) for evidence that HP
is comparable for LSF and HSF faces in the
complete design. Goffaux (2009) finds
different result in a CD version that has no
misaligned condition and uses simultaneous
matching. Jury still out?
#38: “A recent study (Wang, Li, Fang, Tian & Liu, 2012)
…isolated the face-specificity
measure of face recognition performance by
subtracting performance at recognizing
nonface objects. The composite face effect in
correct RTs correlated significantly with this
face-specific measure.”
In Wang et al. the individual correlations with
the PD measure of HP revealed no correlation
with face recognition and a small but negative
correlation with the control object task (Jia Liu,
personal communication). Thus, these results
support a relationship between the PD
measure of HP and object processing, not face
processing (see Humphreys, 1990, for a
discussion of common errors in the
interpretation of correlations with difference
scores).
#41: “Overall, the use of the composite face
illusion in fMRI indicates that faces are
represented holistically in face-sensitive areas
of visual cortex"”.
See Gauthier & Tarr (2002) and Wong et al. (2009b)
for evidence based on the CD that
nonface objects of expertise are also
represented holistically in the same brain
areas.
#42: “This early [N170] effect…identified the
functional locus of the composite face effect at
the earliest face perception stage, suggesting
that facial-parts are not independently
processed as face-like entities before being
integrated into a holistic representation.”
This interpretation of the N170 effect cannot
be directly drawn from the data. Moreover, on
the timescale of neurons, 170ms is not “fast”
or “early” at all: neural responses in low-level
visual areas begin as rapidly as 20ms post-
stimulus onset (Casagrande et al., 2005), with
feedback between higher and lower visual
areas occurring in less than 50ms
(Pascal-Leone & Walsh, 2001).
#60: “…since participants are not aware [that
there are fewer different trials], it can only
increase their tendency to respond `different'
for `same' trials, leading to a higher proportion
of mistakes and the chance to observe more
clearly the composite face effect.”
This is an opinion about how to get a particular
behavioral effect, not how to investigate a
perceptual phenomenon.
#61: “..if one includes many `different' trials in
the study, participants might consider that in
comparison to these real `different' trials, the
illusory different top halves of faces do not
look that different after all.”
This is an opinion about how to get a particular
behavioral effect, not how to investigate a
perceptual phenomenon.
#91: “…behavioral studies using these other
paradigms have provided information about
holistic/configural face perception that
generally agrees with studies using the
composite face paradigm.”
See Richler et al. (2012) for a discussion of why
different measures of HP based on different
definitions need not produce the same results.
See Wong et al. (2010) for data showing that
one of the paradigms referred to here
(Thatcher illusion) is actually not face-specific.
#115: “…in the congruency/interference
paradigm one cannot determine whether it is
the incongruent context face half that
interferes with the processing of the target
face half, or if it is the congruent condition
context face half that facilitates processing of
the target face half”.
Interference vs. facilitation can be assessed by
including isolated parts trials in the
experiment (e.g., Richler, Bukach & Gauthier, 2009;
Richler, Tanaka et al. 2008).
See Richler, Tanaka et al. (2008) for evidence
that the incongruent face half interferes with
processing of the target part.
#119: “…the results of [Richler et al., 2009b] do
not fully support the authors' argument (Figure 1
of that paper) and suggest a contribution of
a naming response conflict.”
This claim is based only on the accuracy data,
which was at ceiling. Therefore, in that paper
we placed greater emphasis on the RT data
(see Richler, Cheung et al., 2009).
#121: “[Richler & Gauthier] have been very
critical of the misalignment manipulations in
many publications, claiming that `the
congruency effect provides a single measure of
holistic processing without necessitating a
misalignment manipulation to measure it'
(Cheung et al., 2008, p. 1328).”
A misaligned condition is always necessary in
the PD, and in the CD it is not necessary in the
same way, as a congruency effect can be
measured and interpreted for aligned stimuli.
We have in fact discussed very explicitly
situations where the misaligned condition in
the CD is particularly important (Richler, Wong & Gauthier, 2011).
This critique is also out of
touch with our published work: a
misalignment manipulation was included in all
but 4 (Richler, Gauthier et al., 2008,
Experiment 1; Richler, Tanaka et al., 2008,
Experiment 2; Richler, Bukach & Gauthier, 2009,
Experiment 3; Richler, Mack et al., 2009)
of the 16 experiments (in nine empirical
papers) using the composite task that we
(G&R) have published together.
#127: “…because misaligned trials give rise to
significant congruency effects at times of
comparable magnitude to those observed for
aligned trials, the authors claimed that
misaligned faces were processed holistically
(Richler et al., 2008b, experiment #1).”
This is a misrepresentation of our results and
conclusions. We found that misalignment
at study had no effect on HP. Misalignment at
test led to significant congruency effects that
were significantly smaller in magnitude than
those obtained for aligned faces. Therefore,
we concluded that misalignment does not
eliminate, but significantly attenuates, HP.
#128: “Moreover, despite the fact that
congruency effects for nonface objects
(`Greebles') were equally large for aligned and
misaligned stimuli (Gauthier et al., 1998;
Gauthier & Tarr, 2002), these authors
concluded that these nonface objects were
processed holistically.”
Note that these experiments predate any
formal comparison of the CD and PD. This
critique focuses on a p=.05 level as a cliff, to
suggest that effects were not existent in
studies that were low-powered (n=10 in each
case) because they were costly training
studies. The focus is on effects associated with
p-values of .06 and .09. The reader is invited to
read the original papers, consider that effect
sizes in short-term training studies are going to
be smaller than for the same effects in real-
world experts (as in car experts in Bukach et al., 2010).
Much has been written about the
perils of dichotomous thinking about
significance, an interested reader could start
with (Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989).
#131: “GRC do not only include both the `same'
and `different' trials: the consider an
interaction between two factors…but also a
main congruency effect…and an alignment
effect…as evidence for holistic processing.”
“The authors typically interpret all three kinds
of effects instead of one”.
This is inaccurate. We have never used all
three of these effects as possible places were
we could find HP. Indeed, in all of our recent
work (all papers published after 2009) we only
measure HP using the congruency × alignment
interaction (see Richler, Wong & Gauthier, 2011,
for a review).
#137: Only misaligning the test face and not
the study face is a methodological confound.
This is inaccurate. We only misalign faces at
test because misalignment at study has been
shown to produce contextual congruency
effects in objects in novices (Richler, Bukach & Gauthier, 2009),
but does not impact
performance with faces (Richler, Tanaka et al., 2008).
It is therefore an empirically motivated
guard against the kind of spurious HP effects
Rossion himself warns against (does he, or
does he not, want us to be conservative about
what we call HP?). Moreover, when we
compare HP for faces to that for other object
categories, or novices to experts, we use the
same procedure, so this cannot account for
differences we observe.
#140: “However, with the exception of one
study (Cheung et al., 2008), GRC also
introduced another significant modification in
their congruency/interference paradigm. That
is, participants have to consider both halves of
the study face”.
This is inaccurate. Participants also only made
judgments on top face halves in Richler, Cheung & Gauthier (2011b)
and McGugin et al. (2012).
#142: If the cued part is unknown at study “the
participant will certainly make more mistakes
when there is an incongruent part anywhere in
the second visual display presented (i.e.,
incongruent trials)…”
This is an opinion. See Richler, Tanaka et al. (2008,
Experiment 2) for data showing no
effect of congruency when the target and task-
irrelevant face halves are presented side-by-
side at test. In other words, participants do not
make more mistakes when there is an
incongruent part anywhere in the test display.
Moreover, when we compare HP for faces to
that for other object categories, or novices to
experts, we use the same procedure, so this
cannot account for differences we observe.
#143: “In the standard composite face
paradigm the participant knows in advance
which of the two halves…has to be encoded,
and can therefore fixate gaze accordingly…If
presentation time is longer, [the participant]
will probably alternate between fixating one of
the two halves of the study face…thus, you
cannot fairly compare the effects observed
when presentation duration is variable at
encoding, especially when you compare
conditions allowing only one fixation (e.g.,
17ms to 183ms) to conditions allowing several
fixations (>183ms until 800ms;
Richler et al., 2009a).”
This is precisely the point of the manipulation
of presentation time: to compare the
magnitude of HP when there is more time
available to potentially encode both object
parts even though only one is relevant.
Importantly, sensitivity (d') does not differ
between conditions when presentation time of
the study face is limited and conditions when
presentation time of the test face is limited
(Richler, Mack et al., 2009).
#146: The complete design has too many
trials. “This is not parsimonious at all, and
violates a general principle in research that
one should include only the conditions in the
paradigm that allow to test specific hypotheses
rather than manipulate all possible variables
and then expect some regularizes (`laws') to
emerge.”
We resent the implication here that we
manipulate “everything” and then “data mine”
to find significant effects. We believe that all
the trials in the CD are necessary if we want to
test specific hypotheses about HP and guard
against bias confounds.
#147: “Such a high number of conditions is
particularly problematic when one needs to
assess holistic face processing in (very) young
children…or in brain-damaged patients.”
This is an opinion. In the CD, 160 trials are
typically necessary to assess HP (e.g.,
Richler, Cheung & Gauthier, 2011a,b). In
de Heering, Houthuys & Rossion (2007) children are tested
on 100 trials, and in Ramon, Busigny & Rossion (2010)
patients are tested on 116 trials. This
hardly seems like a dramatic increase, and
frankly, it seems hardly ethical to test anyone
at all in measures that have demonstrated low
validity.
#150: “…weaker correlation [between holistic
processing and the CFMT] reported by
DeGutis et al. (2013).”
This is misleading. The correlation we reported
in Richler, Cheung & Gauthier (2011b) was .40;
the correlation reported by Degutis et al. was
.33. Moreover, this result has also been
replicated in McGugin et al. (2012), where the
first-order correlation was .26, and the partial
correlation (factoring out age, sex, and their
interactions with holistic processing) was .33.
#152: “Researchers in this field are not
interested in the general processes that can
drive such inter-individual correlations, but
rather in what specifically differs between
upright face processing, namely holistic face
perception.”
Rossion seems to be confusing his own
research interests with those of an entire field.
A very cursory review found 16 articles
published from 2010 onwards that use an
individual differences approach in the study of
face recognition: Avidan et al. (2011),
Bukach et al. (2012), Davis et al. (2011), DeGutis et al. (2013),
Dennett et al. (2011), Dennett et al. (2012),
Germine et al. (2011), Konar et al. (2010),
McGugin et al. (2012), Mondloch & Desjarlais (2010),
Richler et al. (2011), Wang et al. (2012),
Wilhelm et al. (2010),
Wilmer et al. (2010), Zhou et al. (2012), Zhu et al. (2010).
#154: “…the study of Mack et al. (2011)
entitled `Indecision on decisional separability'
dismissed entirely the conclusions reached by
Richler et al. (2008a) that holistic processing
has a decisional locus.”
In Mack et al. (2011) we explained why the
analysis tool developed by Kadlec & Townsend (1992)
and used in Richler, Gauthier et al. (2008)
is not valid. Indeed, we only used that
analysis in that single paper because we
discovered these issues and made them public
immediately.
#159: “…Richler et al. (2009a) did not use any
misaligned faces or inverted faces as a control,
which makes it impossible to interpret their
effects.”
This is an opinion, and the term “impossible”
seems overly strong. Note also that we
ourselves acknowledged this shortcoming, and
addressed it in Richler, Mack et al. (2011,
Experiment 2).
#163: “Inverted faces are usually presented for
reasonably long durations, so that Richler et al's (2011)
claim that inverted faces would not
be processed holistically only at short
durations is clearly inconsistent with that
literature.”
This is misleading. Studies that find no HP for
inverted faces presented for long durations all
use the PD (or simultaneous presentation,
Goffaux, 2009). It is therefore not surprising
that our results using the CD are incompatible
because these two designs often yield
different results.
#164: “GRC's congruency/interference
paradigm was initially developed to study the
processing of nonface objects…and was
applied only later to faces…”
This is inaccurate. The CD was first used by
Farah et al. (1998) with faces.
#165: The conclusion that objects of expertise
are processed holistically are only based on
congruency effects (Gauthier et al, 1998; 2002;
2003).
This is inaccurate. Alignment effects are
reported in Gauthier & Tarr, (2002).
#166: “[In Wong et al. 2009]…On RTs, there
was a non-significant trend of an interaction
between expertise, congruency and
alignment…
This is rhetorical. This three-way interaction is
at p=.053. See answer to claim 128. We hope
that enough expertise training studies will
eventually have been done that a meta-
analytical approach to thinking about what is
going on is possible.
#167: However, this advantage for the
congruent-aligned condition was larger in
novices than experts on sensitivity, pointing to
a speed-accuracy trade-off in the task (Figure 5
of Wong et al., 2009)…”
Rossion is comparing the Individuation training
group to the No training group and apparently
suggesting that a group × congruency ×
alignment interaction in d' that is non-existent
(F=.0004) can explain an effect in RTs (F=3.33).
#168: “This is problematic because if one aims
at demonstrating that faces do not call upon
specific holistic processes, the proper approach
is to test visual experts with nonface object
categories by means of the very same
paradigm used to obtain the strongest face-
specific effects in novices.”
We never aimed at demonstrating faces do
not elicit special HP. Many of our studies aim
at testing whether HP increases with expertise.
We believe the proper approach is not to use
the design that gives the “strongest” effect
with faces, but the “cleanest” effect, one that
can be validly associated with HP rather than
response biases.