Pre-natal |
Arm babbling in the womb De Vries et al., 1984
|
Proprioceptive-motor mapping of general movements |
1 |
Hand-mouth movements Rochat, 1993
|
Learning of home position through tactile feedback |
1 |
Directed (to the hemifield in which a target appears), but unsuccessful, hand movements von Hofsten and Rönnqvist, 1993; Ennouri and Bloch, 1996
|
Initial mapping of general movements to vision |
1 |
Initial reaching is goal directed, and triggered by a visual stimulus, but visual feedback is not used to correct movements mid-reach Bremner, 1994, p. 38 |
Visual stimuli trigger general reach movements |
3 |
Infants often move their hand to a pre-reaching position near the head before starting a reach Berthier et al., 1999, which then follows the line of sight Bruner, 1968, p. 44 |
Reaches conducted from “home” position |
3 |
Infants engaged in early reaching maintained a constant hand-body distance by locking the elbow, and instead used torso movements to alter the distance to targets Berthier et al., 1999
|
Constraints on elbow movements reduce learning space |
3 |
Successful reaching appears around 3–4 months after birth Shirley, 1933; Fiorentino, 1981; Berthier et al., 1999; Berthier and Keen, 2006
|
Primitive hand-eye mapping |
3 |
Gaze still focused on the target and not the hand Clifton et al., 1993; Butterworth and Harris, 1994; Clifton et al., 1994; Berthier and Carrico, 2010
|
Reaches are visually elicited, but without continuous feedback |
4 |
From 4 months, infants begin to use visual feedback to refine the movement of the hand White et al., 1964
|
Begin to map joint-visual changes and use visual feedback to correct reaches |
4 |
As infants age their reaching becomes straighter, with the hand following the shortest path Carvalho et al., 2007
|
Refined reaching with smooth and direct movements |