Abstract
Complicated grief (CG) is increasingly recognized as a debilitating outcome of bereavement. Given the intensity of the stressor, its chronicity, and its association with depression, it is important to know the impact CG may have on cognitive functioning. This exploratory and descriptive study examined global and domain-specific cognitive functioning in a help-seeking sample of individuals with CG (n=335) compared to a separately ascertained control sample (n=250). Cognitive functioning was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). Controlling for age, sex and education effects, CG participants had lower total MoCA, visuospatial and attention scores relative to control participants. The two groups did not differ significantly in the domains of executive function, language, memory or orientation. Age, sex, and education accounted for much of the variance in MoCA scores, while CG severity and chronicity accounted for a very small percentage of MoCA score variance. Major depression was not a significant predictor of MoCA scores. This study is consistent with previous work demonstrating lower attention and global cognitive performance in individuals with CG compared to control participants. This study newly identifies the visuospatial domain as a target for future studies investigating cognitive functioning in CG.
Introduction
Bereavement and the experience of grief are among life's most stressful events1. Despite this stress, most individuals come to accept the finality of the death, its consequences, redefine their life goals and adjust to life without their loved one2. However, for some, the acute grief process is stalled, leading to prolonged or complicated grief (CG). Symptoms of CG include intense sorrow, guilt, deep yearning for the deceased; preoccupation for the loved one or events surrounding the death; avoidance of reminders of the loss; bitterness, and difficulty trusting or caring for others3,4.
Much evidence suggests that CG is a disorder distinct from conditions with overlapping symptomatology such as post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression5,6,7. The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has included provisional criteria for the diagnosis of CG, designated “Persistent Complex Bereavement Disorder (PCBD), in section III of the DSM-58.
Early studies have estimated the prevalence of CG to be 4–5% in the general population and 7–5% among bereaved individuals9,10.The impact of CG on society and among bereaved individuals and their families is profound. Comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders such as depression tends to be significantly higher in individuals with CG compared to the general public. In a clinical study, a concurrent major depressive disorder (MDD) was present in 55% of individuals with CG11. Given the intensity of the stressor and its association with depression, understanding the impact CG may have on cognition is important.
Previous studies have suggested that individuals with CG have greater neurocognitive deficits compared to both normally bereaved and non-bereaved control participants in community based samples12 13. This study represents an exploratory and descriptive analysis. It builds on previous work by exploring the association of CG with cognitive function in a sample of participants in a multisite NIMH-sponsored clinical trial researching treatment for CG and describing what may be an associated feature of the disorder. We examined cognitive functioning both globally and across six neurocognitive domains. After accounting for variables associated with cognitive functioning such as age, education, and depression, we examined whether variance in cognitive dysfunction might be explained by the presence of, severity or chronicity of CG symptoms.
Methods
1. Primary study description
We used data from a multicenter, double blind, placebo-controlled intervention trial entitled “Optimizing Treatment for Complicated Grief” (Healing Emotions After Loss: HEAL). The study began in March 2010 and is an ongoing NIMH sponsored clinical trial investigating the effects of citalopram versus placebo, with and without complicated grief therapy (CGT) [ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01179568]. The study is being conducted in Boston New York, Pittsburgh and San Diego. Participants were recruited through a variety of methods including referrals from health care professionals and facilities (21%), non-health care personnel or agencies (6%), print media (20%), and broadcast or internet media (41%). The analyses reported here used pretreatment data from all randomized individuals as of January 16, 2014.
Inclusion criteria required an Inventory of Complicated Grief (ICG)14 score of 30 or greater at least 6 months after the death of a loved one, CG confirmed as present and the primary problem on clinical interview, and English fluency. Individuals were excluded from the study for any of the following reasons: substance abuse or dependence within the past 6 months, history of a psychotic disorder, current psychotherapy or treatment with an antidepressant, a Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score <21, active homicidal ideation or when considered at immediate risk for suicide.
2. Archival control group
Our control sample was taken from previously published data comparing performance on the MoCA and the Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) in cognitively normal individuals15. Most participants were from a convenience sample consisting of spouses and friends of patients seen at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Huntington's disease Research Center and UCSD Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer's Disease Research Center. Participants were excluded if they reported a lifetime history of neurologic or psychiatric disorders, or the use of psychoactive substances or medications.
3. Measures
Complicated grief was measured using the ICG16. The ICG is a 19-item self-report questionnaire reflecting the core emotional, behavioral and psychological symptoms of CG. Each of the 19-items are given a severity score between 0 (never) and 4 (always). Possible total scores range from 0–76. All study participants scored 30 or greater on the ICG. 17 Cognitive function was measured using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) a screening tool for mild cognitive impairment and dementia18. The range of possible scores was 21–30. The lower limit was set at 21 in the HEAL study in order to rule-out individuals with probable dementia. Therefore, we used only control participants who earned scores in the same range (21–30). In each participant, we assessed the six neurocognitive domains (visuospatial ability, executive functioning, language, delayed memory, attention and orientation) represented in the MoCA. Executive function was measured by the sum of a participant's scores in the trail making, fluency, and abstraction tasks. Language was measured by the sum of the repetition and naming tasks. The visuospatial domain was measured using the sum of the cube and clock drawing tasks. Scores on the delayed recall task were used to assess delayed memory. Attention/ concentration and orientation were assessed as given on the MoCA.
Current mood disorder was evaluated using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Patient Edition (SCID)19.
Depression severity was assessed using the 16-item version of the Quick Inventory of depressive symptomatology self-report (QIDS-SR-16)20. Possible total scores range from 0–27.
Medical morbidity and burden were assessed using the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS)21 a comprehensive review of medical problems by 14 organ systems. The CIRS rates each organ system between 0 (no problem) and 4 (end organ failure/ severe functional impairment). The organ category “,other” was omitted in the primary (HEAL) study, therefore, severity ratings were assessed for 13 organ systems. Possible total scores range from 0–52. For the purposes of this analysis, the categories of vascular and heart disease (combined mean) and neurological disease were reported as these organ systems are most likely to impact cognition. Total score included all 13 organ systems. Number of organ systems affected indicates the number of organ system categories (range 0–13) rated with a severity greater than zero.
4. CG Consort chart description
One-thousand-nine-hundred-thirty-nine individuals were screened over the telephone using the brief grief questionnaire (BGQ)22. The BGQ is a five-item self-administered screening tool that evaluates some of the core CG symptoms. Responses were rated as 0, not at all; 1, somewhat; or 2, a lot. A score of 5 or greater raises clinical suspicion for CG23. Of the 1939 individuals screened, 1420 received a score of 5 or greater on the BGQ and were invited for a face-to-face baseline clinical asessment; 1,072 were excluded for various reasons (see fig. 1 for details). 348 participants were randomized to treatment, 13 had incomplete or missing MoCA data and were excluded from the analysis. The remaining 335 participants comprised the CG study group used in our analysis. All data for this analysis were collected at the baseline clinical assessment. The same procedures regarding recruitment and assessments described above were used at all four clinical sites.
Figure 1.
5. Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics were generated to characterize the CG and control participants on demographic measures (age, gender, race and education). Differences between groups were tested using t-tests for continuous variables and Chi-square test or Fisher Exact test for categorical variables. Descriptive statistics also further characterized the CG participants on the clinical measures of complicated grief, depression and medical burden.
Total MoCA scores were calculated using the sum of the items. Because persons with 12 years of education or less tend to have worse MoCA performance, one point is usually added to the total score to correct for educational effects24. For the purposes of our study an extra point was not added since level of education was used as covariate in our analyses. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tested for group differences controlling for age, gender and education. Analyses were repeated in participants ≥ 60 years of age since older adults are often most sensitive to cognitive changes. An additional sub analysis was done with CG participants alone since some CG participants MoCA's were administered at baseline before discontinuing any current antidepressant use. The ANCOVA was repeated in CG participants alone to test the possible effect of antidepressant medication.
Given the limited possible score range of each MoCA domain; an ordered logistic regression was used to test whether CG and control participants differed in any of the MoCA domains. These analyses controlled for age, gender and education. Bar charts show distribution of scores used in the analyses.
Multiple linear regression models were used in the CG sample alone in order to examine the effects of depression and CG severity on global MoCA scores after controlling for known effects of demographic variables.
6. Informed consent
“HEAL” was overseen by a Data and Safety Monitoring Board and reviewed by Institutional review boards at each of the participating study sites. Participants in both sample groups (CG and control) gave written informed consent.
Results
Participant descriptors
CG participants included both men and women between the ages of 19–89. CG participants had a mean (SD) age of 53(±15 years), were predominantly female (80%) and white (85%).. The control sample was half female. Years of formal aging were comparable in the two groups (Table 1)
Table 1.
Demographic Measures
Complicated Grief (CG) [n if reduced sample] N=335 | Control [n if reduced sample] N=250 | ||
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Age | 53.01 (14.57) | 54.42(19.81) | t(583)=−0.99, p=0.32 |
Median =55 | Median=57 | ||
Range =19–89 | Range=20–89 | ||
| |||
%Female | 79.70 (n=267) | 53.60 (n=134) | chisq(1) = 45.24, p<0.0001 |
| |||
Race | [n=326] | [n=185] | Fisher exact p=0.01 (White vs. other: Fisher exact p=0.42) |
% American Indian or Alaska Native | 1.84 (n=6) | 0.54 (n=1) | |
| |||
%Asian | 1.84 (n=6) | 5.95 (n=11) | |
| |||
%Black | 10.43 (n=34) | 5.41 (n=10) | |
| |||
%Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander | 0.61 (n=2) | 0.00 (n=0) | |
| |||
%White | 85.28 (n=278) | 88.11 (n=163) | |
| |||
Education Level | chisq(3) = 2.60, p=0.46 | ||
%<= 12 yrs | 12.54 (n=42) | 14.80 (n=37) | |
| |||
% 13–15yrs | 34.03 (n=114) | 33.60 (n=84) | |
| |||
% 16 yrs | 21.79 (n=73) | 25.20 (n=63) | |
| |||
% > 16 yrs | 31.64 (n=106) | 26.40 (n=66) | |
| |||
Site | -- | -- | |
%San Diego | 28.85 (n=100) | ||
| |||
% Boston | 27.46 (n=92) | ||
| |||
%New York | 11.04 (n=37) | ||
| |||
%Pittsburgh | 31.64 (n=106) |
Regarding clinical characteristics (Table 2) the mean score on the Inventory of Complicated Grief (ICG) was 43 (±9). The mean time since the death of a loved one was 5 (±7) years with median time of 2 years. 67% (n= 223/335) of CG participants had a diagnosis of current MDD. The mean total QIDS score was 13 (±4) indicating a mild to moderate level of depression. The mean total CIRS score for medical comorbidity was 6 (±4), with the mean number of organ systems affected, 5 (±3).
Table 2.
Clinical Measures
Complicated Grief (CG) N=335 | |
---|---|
| |
Time Since Loss | 4.74 (7.19) Median = 2.37 Range=0.50–58.67 |
| |
Inventory Complicated Grief (ICG score) | 43.05 (8.96) |
| |
Quick Inventory of depressive symptomatology self-report (QIDS-SR) | 13.46 (4.22) |
| |
%Current MDD | 66.57 (n=223) |
| |
Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS) | |
Total | 6.27 (4.31) |
| |
Number of affected organ systems | 4.70 (2.75) |
| |
Heart + Vascular | 0.93 (1.15) |
| |
Neurological | 0.44 (0.64) |
Global cognition
The mean (SD) MoCA score in CG was 26.8 (±2.2) compared with 27.1 (±2.2) in control participants. The median MoCA score was 27 in CG and 28 in control participants (Table 3.).
Table 3.
MoCA Scores
Complicated Grief (CG) N=335 | Controls (CTRL) N=250 | Age F,p/χ2,p | Sex F,P/χ2,p | Education F,p/χ2,p | CG F,P/χ2,P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MoCA Total* (range=0–30) | 26.79 (2.20) Median=27 Range=21–30 |
27.12 (2.24) Median=28 Range=21–30 |
57.42, p<0.001 | 5.39, p=0.02 | 9.78, p<0.0001 | 9.11, p=0.003 |
Domains Executive Functioning (range=0–4) |
3.46 (0.76) Median=4 Range=1–4 |
3.53 (0.66) Median=4 Range=1–4 |
13.21, p=0.0003 | 2.89, p=0.09 | 31.51, p<0.0001 | 2.46, p=0.12 |
Visuospatial (range=0–4) | 3.41 (0.74) Median=4 Range=1–4 |
3.63 (0.59) Median=4 Range=2–4 |
20.38, p<0.0001 | 3.74, p=0.05 | 7.32, p=0.06 | 12.52, p=0.0004 |
Language (range=0–5) | 4.65 (0.59) Median=5 Range=2–5 |
4.55 (0.72) Median=5 Range =2–5 |
6.01, p=0.01 | 3.95, p=0.05 | 9.35, p=0.03 | 0.62, p=0.43 |
Short term memory (range=0–5) | 3.79 (1.18) Median=4 Range=0–5 |
3.74 (1.34) Median=4 Range=0–5 |
47.61, p<0.0001 | 12.61, p=0.0004 | 1.79, p=0.62 | 2.41, p=0.12 |
Attention/Co ncentration (range=0–6) | 5.56 (0.76) Median=6 Range=2–6 |
5.72 (0.62) Median=6 Range=2–6 |
3.80, p=0.05 | 2.06, p=0.15 | 13.66, p=0.003 | 6.39, p=0.01 |
Orientation (range=0–6) | 5.92 (0.31) Median=6 Range=4–6 |
5.95 (0.24) Median=6.00 Range=4–6 |
1.23, p=0.27 | 0.03, p=0.86 | 3.14, p=0.37 | 1.99, p=0.16 |
No point added for participants with ≤ High School education
F statistic reported for ANCOVA (df=1,578) and χ2 (df=1) reported for the domains scores.
Results of ANCOVA showed that an increased age was associated with decrease in MoCA score (F(1,578)=57.42, p<0.0001, explained variance=9.3%). Being female and having more education were both associated with higher scores (F(1,578)=5.39, p=0.02, explained variance=0.3) and (F(3,578)=9.78, p<0.0001, explained variance=4.1%) respectively. Controlling for age, sex and education effects, control participants had higher MoCA scores than CG participants (F(1,578)=9.11, p=0.003, explained variance=1.3%, least square means: CG = 26.5 vs. CTRL = 27.1). Age, sex, education and group membership (CG vs. Control) explained 15% variance in MoCA total scores, while group membership alone explained 1.3% of variance above and beyond what was explained by demographic measures. In a sub-analyses examining only participants ≥ 60 years of age, group was no longer significant (F(1,210)=3.12, p=0.08).
Total MOCA scores did not differ in sub-analyses comparing CG participants on psychotropic medications vs. CG participants not on psychotropic medications (F(6,328)=0.01, p=0.91, LSMEANS adjusting for age, gender and education: 26.61 vs. 26.58 respectively).
Among potential participants with an ICG score of 30 or higher and at least 6 months elapsed since the death of a loved one, none had MOCA scores less than 21.
Domain specific cognition
Table 3 displays the mean, median and range of both CG and control participants for each of the six neurocognitive domains. Results of the ordered logistic regression show that increased age was significantly associated with lower scores in all domains except attention and orientation (at p<0.05). Females had higher MoCA scores than males in the language and memory domains. Higher education was associated with higher scores in the executive, language and attention domains. Controlling for age, sex and education effects, control participants were more likely to have higher visuospatial and attention scores than CG participants (odds ratios [95%CI]: 1.92 [1.34–2.75] and 1.68 [1.12–2.51] respectively.) The two groups did not differ significantly in the domains of executive function, language, memory or orientation.
Cognitive changes attributed to depression and severity of CG
In the multiple linear regression models using only the CG sample, age and education, but not gender, were significant predictors of total MoCA score. Increased age was associated with lower MoCA scores, and higher levels of education, with higher MoCA scores. There was a site difference such that the NY site had lower MoCA scores than the other sites. We controlled for this site related difference in the analyses. The demographic variables explained 12.1% of variance seen in total MoCA scores. Current MDD diagnosis was not a significant predictor and neither was time since loss (explained variance <0.01%). ICG severity explained approximately 1% of total MoCA scores above and beyond demographic measures, diagnosis of depression and time since loss. (Table 4.)
Table 4.
Results from linear regression looking at effects of depression and CG severity
Model:F(9,325)=5.57, p<0.0001, R-square = 0.1337 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Parameter Estimate | Standard Error | t Value | p-value | Standardized Estimate | %Variance Explained | |
Intercept | 29.77 | 0.77 | 38.87 | <0.001 | 0 | -- |
Age | −0.03 | 0.02 | −3.61 | <0.001 | −0.20 | 3.4 |
Female | 0.28 | 0.28 | 0.99 | 0.32 | 0.05 | 0.2 |
ED: <= 12yrs | −1.54 | 0.39 | −3.92 | <0.001 | −0.23 | 4.0 |
ED:13–15yrs | −0.93 | 0.28 | −3.27 | 0.001 | −0.20 | 2.8 |
ED: 16yrs | −0.35 | 0.32 | −1.10 | 0.27 | −0.07 | 0.3 |
Site:NY | −1.00 | 0.39 | −2.60 | 0.01 | −0.14 | 1.8 |
Current MDD Dx | 0.16 | 0.26 | 0.61 | 0.54 | 0.03 | 0.0 |
Time Since Loss | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.77 | 0.44 | 0.04 | 0.1 |
ICG score | −0.03 | 0.01 | −1.93 | 0.06 | −0.11 | 1.0 |
Discussion
Summary of major findings
Complicated Grief was associated with somewhat lower levels of cognitive function compared to control participants after controlling for age, sex, and education. Although differences in global scores were statistically significant they were modest; their clinical significance is not clear. Compared to control participants, those with CG displayed greater decrements in the attention/concentration and visuospatial domains. Age, sex and education accounted for the majority of the variance in cognitive functioning while CG symptoms accounted for a smaller percentage of the variance. Although individuals with CG displayed high rates of major depression, depression accounted for very little of the variance in cognitive functioning.
We would not have been surprised to find clinically significant cognitive deficits in those with CG, given the immense and chronic level of distress these individuals experience. However, in this relatively young sample cognitive decline would not necessarily be expected. Somewhat unexpectedely, the sub-analysis examining participants age≥ 60 years yielded no group significance. This may reflect differences in our archival control group which may have had participants with caregiver stress and burden.
Study novelty
This study adds to current knowledge by evaluating multiple domains of cognition in the largest clinical sample of help-seeking individuals with CG to date25,26, 27. Moreover, studies that have assessed global cognitive functioning in CG have relied on the MMSE, which does not assess executive function.28, 29, 30.
Comparison to previous literature
Using the MMSE in a community-based sample, Newson et al 31 found modest difference in global cognitive functioning between normal grievers M=27.43 (2.2) and those with CG M=26.98 (2.4) (F(1,1086)=5.38, p<0.05). Also using the MMSE, O'Connor et al 32 showed slightly greater differences in cognitive functioning when comparing both non-bereaved M=28.5 (1.7) and normally bereaved M=27.9 (2) control groups to CG participants M=26.5 (3.5) (F(2,72) = 4.25, p = 0.02). Similar to our analysis, both studies showed statistically significant, albeit small, global cognitive deficits among CG participants compared to control participants.
Using the emotional-counting Stroop (ec-stroop) task, O'Connor et al 33 found that CG participants had longer reaction times across 3 blocks of grief-related words compared to control participants. Maccallum et al 34 found similar results. The ec-stroop task measures attentional bias, a specific function within the domain of attention/ concentration. O' Connor et al also found no difference among groups in working memory or set-shifting (an aspect of executive functioning). In addition to global cognitive functioning, our results are consistent with O'Connor et al in the following ways. CG participants showed deficits in the attention/ concentration domain, but did not differ from control participants with regard to executive functioning and memory. Cognitive deficits were independent of MDD. Beyond the domain of attention/ concentration; our findings highlight the possibility that aspects of the visuospatial domain may be impaired in some individuals with CG.
Study limitations
Because a MoCA score of less than 21 was an exclusion criteria in the HEAL study we were unable to capture individuals with CG who were most impaired cognitively. A wider range of MocA scores may increase the effect sizes of the predictors in the statistical models used. However, this would likely add individuals with dementia to the group, Because CGT has not been developed and tested for use in persons with dementia we needed to exclude all such individuals in the primary study (HEAL). This is why a cutoff score of less than 21 was a criteria for exclusion. Even so, very few persons initially screened had MoCA scores of less than 21.
Although a widely used clinical screening tool for dementia, the MoCA is a neither as sensitive nor specific in evaluating neurocognitive impairment or dementia as thoroughly as a comprehensive neuropsychological battery.
We also did not sample a control group recruited to match our CG participants but used an archival control group that differed from our CG group in terms of time of recruitment and possible psychiatric morbidity. Because our control group consisted of friends and spouses of patients with Huntington's disease, Alzheimers disease, MCI and other illnesses, we cannot rule-out the possibility that they may have experienced elevated levels of caregiver stress and burden thus negatively impacting their cognitive performance. Furthermore, it is possible that individuals in the control group were bereaved or suffering from CG. A small percentage of our control participants were relatives of those with Huntington's disease, Alzheimer's disease, and multiple sclerosis. Therefore, we also cannot be certain of the extent to which genetic factors contributed to cognitive performance in the control group.
Although we did control for education level in our analyses, premorbid IQ was not measured and thus could not be controlled for. Education, like premorbid IQ, has been shown to be an important predictor of performance on cognitive screening tests including the MoCA35.
Implications for future studies
This exploratory and descriptive analysis adds to the growing literature exploring cognitive functioning in CG. These data point to the need for further research in cognitive health as it relates to CG, specifically with regard to the attention/concentration and visuospatial neurocognitive domains.
Highlights.
We compare performance on the MoCA in persons with CG and an archival control group.
Individuals with CG displayed mild deficits in global cognitive functioning.
Deficits were observed in the attention and visuospatial neurocognitive domains.
Age, sex and education accounted for much of the variance in MoCA scores.
Very little MoCA score variance was due to CG severity, chronicity or current MDD.
Acknowledgements
There are no acknowledgements to report.
Conflicts of Interest Statements Charles Hall reports no conflicts of interest. Charles F. Reynolds III, MD reports receiving pharmaceutical support for NIH-sponsored research studies from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Forest, Pfizer, and Lilly; receiving grants from the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institute on Aging, National Center for Minority Health Disparities, National Heart Lung and Blood Institute, Center for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS), Patient Centered Outcomes Research Institute (PCORI),the Commonwealth of Pennsylvania, the John A Hartford Foundation, National Palliative Care Research Center (NPCRC), Clinical and Translational Science Institute (CTSI), and the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention; and serving on the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry editorial review board. He has received an honorarium as a speaker from MedScape/WEB MD. He is the co-inventor (Licensed Intellectual Property) of Psychometric analysis of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) PRO10050447 (PI: Buysse). Supported by the National Institutes of Health through Grant Numbers P60 MD000207; P30 MH090333; UL1RR024153, UL1TR000005; and the UPMC Endowment in Geriatric Psychiatry. Meryl A. Butters, PhD reports no conflicts of interest specifically pertaining to this project. In the past year, Dr. Butters has received grant support from NIMH (R01s 072947 & 080240) and has received remuneration from GlaxoSmithKline for neuropsychological services. Sidney Zisook, MD has no conflicts of interest to report. As of February 14, 2014, Naomi Simon MD has received research grant support in the past 36 months from American Cancer Society, American Foundation for Suicide Prevention, Department of Defense, Forest Laboratories, Highland Street Foundation, NIMH, and NCCAM. She has received honoraria for speaking for the MGH Psychiatry Academy. Her spouse has equity in Elan, Dandreon, G Zero, Gatekeeper. Jody Corey-Bloom, MD, PhD has no conflicts of interest with regard to this manuscript, “Cognitive Functioning in Complicated Grief”, for submission to the Journal of Psychiatric Research. Barry D. Lebowitz PhD, Amy Begley, and Christine Mauro report no conflicts of interest. M. Kathryn Shear received a contract with Guilford Press for writing a complicated grief treatment manual for publication.
Contributions: Dr. Reynolds and Mr. Hall designed the study and prepared the manuscript. Dr. Butters provided expertise in the analysis and interpretation of neuropsychological data. Dr. Corey-Bloom provided control data for comparison purposes with MOCA scores obtained in participants with complicated grief. Amy Begley undertook the statistical analyses. Christine Mauro participated in statistical analyses and data presentation. Drs. Shear, Simon, Lebowitz, and Zisook all had critical input into the development and revision of the manuscript, and all participated in the design of the parent study, HEAL, a multisite clinical trial sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Role of Funding Source The sponsors of the study (the National Institute of Mental Health and the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention) played no role in the conduct of the study, data analyses, or preparation of this manuscript.
Supported by NIMH R01MH060783 NIMH R01MH085297 NIMH 5R01MH085308 NIMH R01MH085288 P30 MH90333 American Foundation for Suicide Prevention R01MH080240 NIMH 5R25MH054318-18
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Holmes TH, Rahe R. The Social readjustment scale. J Psychosom Res. 1967;11(2):213–8. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4. PubMed: 6059863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Shear K, Shair H. Attachment, loss, and complicated grief. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;47:253–267. doi: 10.1002/dev.20091. PubMed: 16252293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn) (DSM-V) American Psychiatric Association; Arlington, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Shear MK, Simon N, Wall M, et al. Complicated grief and related bereavement issues for DSM-5. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28(2):103–17. doi: 10.1002/da.20780. PubMed: 21284063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Prigerson HG, Bierhals AJ, Kasl SV, et al. Complicated grief as a disorder distinct from bereavement-related depression and anxiety: a replication study. Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153(11):1484–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.11.1484. PubMed: 8890686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Boelen PA, van den Bout J. Complicated grief, depression, and anxiety as distinct postloss syndromes: a confirmatory factor analysis study. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(11):2175–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2175. PubMed: 16263861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Simon NM, Shear KM, Thompson EH, et al. The prevalence and correlates of psychiatric comorbidity in individuals with complicated grief. Compr Psychiatry. 2007;48(5):395–9. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.05.002. PubMed: 17707245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn) (DSM-V) American Psychiatric Association; Arlington, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Newson RS, Boelen PA, Hek K, et al. The prevalence and characteristics of complicated grief in older adults. J Affect Disord. 2011;132(1–2):231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.021. PubMed: 21397336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kersting A, Brähler E, Glaesmer H, et al. Prevalence of complicated grief in a representative population-based sample. J Affect Disord. 2011;131(1–3):339–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.032. PubMed:21216470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Simon NM, Shear KM, Thompson EH, et al. The prevalence and correlates of psychiatric comorbidity in individuals with complicated grief. Compr Psychiatry. 2007 Sep Oct;48(5):395–9. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.05.002. PubMed: 17707245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.O'Connor MF, Arizmendi BJ. Neuropsychological Correlates of Complicated Grief in Older Spousally Bereaved Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014 Jan;69(1):12–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt025. PubMed: 23551907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Newson RS, Boelen PA, Hek K, et al. The prevalence and characteristics of complicated grief in older adults. J Affect Disord. 2011;132(1–2):231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.021. PubMed: 21397336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Prigerson HG, Maciejewski PK, Reynolds CF., 3rd Inventory of Complicated Grief: a scale to measure maladaptive symptoms of loss. Psychiatry Res. 1995 Nov 29;59(1–2):65–79. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02757-2. PubMed: 8771222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gluhm S, Goldstein J, Loc K, et al. Cognitive Performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment Across the Healthy Adult Lifespan. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013 Mar;26(1):1–5. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31828b7d26. PubMed:23538566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Prigerson HG, Maciejewski PK, Reynolds CF., 3rd Inventory of Complicated Grief: a scale to measure maladaptive symptoms of loss. Psychiatry Res. 1995 Nov 29;59(1–2):65–79. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02757-2. PubMed: 8771222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shear K, Frank E, Houck, et al. Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2005 Jun 1;293(21):2601–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.293.21.2601. PubMed: 15928281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005 Apr;53(4):695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. PubMed: 15817019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J. Structured clinical interview for axis I DSM-IV disorders (SCID) American Psychiatric Association Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Ibrahim HM. The 16-Item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2003 Sep 1;54(5):573–83. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01866-8. PubMed: 12946886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Miller MD, Paradis CF, Houck PR, et al. Rating chronic medical illness burden in geropsychiatric practice and research: application of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. Psychiatry Res. 1992 Mar;41(3):237–48. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(92)90005-n. PubMed:5646906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shear KM, Jackson CT, Essock SM, et al. Screening for complicated grief among Project Liberty service recipients 18 months after September 11, 2001. Psychiatr Serv. 2006 Sep;57(9):1291–7. doi: 10.1176/ps.2006.57.9.1291. PubMed: 16968758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shear KM, Jackson CT, Essock SM, et al. Screening for complicated grief among Project Liberty service recipients 18 months after September 11, 2001. Psychiatr Serv. 2006 Sep;57(9):1291–7. doi: 10.1176/ps.2006.57.9.1291. PubMed: 16968758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005 Apr;53(4):695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. PubMed: 15817019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ward L, Mathias JL, Hitchings SE. Relationships between bereavement and cognitive functioning in older adults. Gerontology. 2007;53(6):362–72. doi: 10.1159/000104787. PubMed: 17622731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Newson RS, Boelen PA, Hek K, et al. The prevalence and characteristics of complicated grief in older adults. J Affect Disord. 2011;132(1–2):231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.021. PubMed: 21397336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.O'Connor MF, Arizmendi BJ. Neuropsychological Correlates of Complicated Grief in Older Spousally Bereaved Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014 Jan;69(1):12–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt025. PubMed: 23551907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Newson RS, Boelen PA, Hek K, et al. The prevalence and characteristics of complicated grief in older adults. J Affect Disord. 2011;132(1–2):231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.021. PubMed: 21397336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.O'Connor MF, Arizmendi BJ. Neuropsychological Correlates of Complicated Grief in Older Spousally Bereaved Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014 Jan;69(1):12–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt025. PubMed: 23551907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. PubMed: 15817019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Newson RS, Boelen PA, Hek K, et al. The prevalence and characteristics of complicated grief in older adults. J Affect Disord. 2011;132(1–2):231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.021. PubMed: 21397336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.O'Connor MF, Arizmendi BJ. Neuropsychological Correlates of Complicated Grief in Older Spousally Bereaved Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014 Jan;69(1):12–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt025. PubMed: 23551907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.O'Connor MF, Arizmendi BJ. Neuropsychological Correlates of Complicated Grief in Older Spousally Bereaved Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014 Jan;69(1):12–8. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt025. PubMed: 23551907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Maccallum F, Bryant RA. Attentional bias in complicated grief. J Affect Disord. 2010 Sep;125(1–3):316–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.01.070. PubMed: 20483163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Alves L, Simões MR, Martins C, et al. Premorbid IQ influence on screening tests' scores in healthy patients and patients with cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2013 Jun;26(2):117–26. doi: 10.1177/0891988713484194. Epub 2013 Apr 12. [PubMed: 23584853] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]