Experimental paradigms
|
1. HHI research has predominantly focused on sensorimotor collaboration, not cooperation |
Sensorimotor collaborations
|
Reed [56, 65], Groten [70], Melendez-Calderon [43], Ikeura [50], Rahman [51], Basdogan [52], Sallnas [53], Gentry [54, 64], van der Wel [55], Feth [66] |
Sensorimotor cooperation
|
Ikeura [60], Galvez [86] |
2. The majority of HHI research has used visuomotor tasks with limited degrees-of-freedom |
Constrained visuomotor tasks
|
Melendez-Calderon [43], Ikeura [50], Rahman [51], Basdogan [52], Sallnas [53], Gentry [54, 64], van der Wel [55], Reed [56] |
Whole-body non-visuomotor tasks
|
Galvez [86] |
3. In most HHI research, specific roles for each member of a dyad are rarely defined ahead of time |
Unassigned roles
|
Reed [65], Groten [70], Melendez-Calderon [43], Rahman [51], Basdogan [52], Sallnas [53], Gentry [54, 64], van der Wel [55], Reed [56, 65], Feth [66] |
Assigned roles
|
Ikeura [50, 60] |
Experimental outcomes
|
4. Dyads typically perform as well or better than either member of a dyad alone |
Superior dyad performance
|
Reed [65], Gentry [54], Feth [66] |
Equivalent individual and dyad performance
|
van der Wel [55] |
5. The addition of haptic feedback improves dyad performance compared to visual feedback alone |
|
Basdogan [52], Sallnas [53], Gentry [64], Groten [67, 71] |
6. Members of a dyad apply higher forces than during either of their individual performances |
|
van der Wel [55], Reed [68], Groten [70, 71], Feth [66] |
7. Members of a dyad spontaneously assume specific roles, performing portions of a joint motor task |
|
Melendez-Calderon [43], Reed [32, 56] |
Starting or ending movement
|
Reed [32, 68] |
Adding or absorbing energy
|
Feth [66] |