Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2014 Dec 16;8:404. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00404

The rights and wrongs of blood-brain barrier permeability studies: a walk through 100 years of history

Norman R Saunders 1,*, Jean-Jacques Dreifuss 2, Katarzyna M Dziegielewska 1, Pia A Johansson 3, Mark D Habgood 1, Kjeld Møllgård 4, Hans-Christian Bauer 5,6
PMCID: PMC4267212  PMID: 25565938

Abstract

Careful examination of relevant literature shows that many of the most cherished concepts of the blood-brain barrier are incorrect. These include an almost mythological belief in its immaturity that is unfortunately often equated with absence or at least leakiness in the embryo and fetus. The original concept of a blood-brain barrier is often attributed to Ehrlich; however, he did not accept that permeability of cerebral vessels was different from other organs. Goldmann is often credited with the first experiments showing dye (trypan blue) exclusion from the brain when injected systemically, but not when injected directly into it. Rarely cited are earlier experiments of Bouffard and of Franke who showed methylene blue and trypan red stained all tissues except the brain. The term “blood-brain barrier” “Blut-Hirnschranke” is often attributed to Lewandowsky, but it does not appear in his papers. The first person to use this term seems to be Stern in the early 1920s. Studies in embryos by Stern and colleagues, Weed and Wislocki showed results similar to those in adult animals. These were well-conducted experiments made a century ago, thus the persistence of a belief in barrier immaturity is puzzling. As discussed in this review, evidence for this belief, is of poor experimental quality, often misinterpreted and often not properly cited. The functional state of blood-brain barrier mechanisms in the fetus is an important biological phenomenon with implications for normal brain development. It is also important for clinicians to have proper evidence on which to advise pregnant women who may need to take medications for serious medical conditions. Beliefs in immaturity of the blood-brain barrier have held the field back for decades. Their history illustrates the importance of taking account of all the evidence and assessing its quality, rather than selecting papers that supports a preconceived notion or intuitive belief. This review attempts to right the wrongs. Based on careful translation of original papers, some published a century ago, as well as providing discussion of studies claiming to show barrier immaturity, we hope that readers will have evidence on which to base their own conclusions.

Keywords: blood-brain barrier, blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier, embryo, fetus, newborn, permeability, tight junctions, transporters

Introduction

The very first studies of barrier properties in embryos clearly demonstrated the intactness of the interfaces between the blood, brain and cerebrospinal fluid1. Thus Wislocki (1920) injected a guinea pig embryo with trypan blue (Figure 1A), the same dye, which was first used by Goldmann (1909; 2013 see below). Wislocki (1920) observed that in the embryo, as in adult animals, the dye stained almost all of the tissues of the body, with the notable exception of much of the brain.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Early demonstration of blood-brain barrier phenomenon in developing brain. (A) Mid gestation guinea pig embryo injected with trypan blue (Wislocki, 1920). Note lack of staining of brain and spinal cord as previously described in adult animals injected with trypan blue (Goldmann, 1909), methylene blue (Bouffard, 1906) and trypan red (Franke, 1905). (B) Very early pig embryo (9 mm, E19) injected with isotonic sodium ferrocyanide into spinal canal (arrow) treated with acid (Prussian blue reaction). Note that blue reaction product is confined to CSF, with a small amount of diffusion into brain stem and mid brain tissue, but no staining of somatic tissue. This indicates that the CNS is already a closed compartment separate from the rest of the embryo. From Weed (1917b).

Even earlier than this Weed (1917a,b) injected an approximately isotonic solution of potassium ferrocyanide, a small molecule about the same size as sucrose, into the neural tube of E19 and older pig embryos. Weed's results showed that in an E19 pig embryo the potassium ferrocyanide (demonstrated by the Prussian blue reaction) remained entirely within the fluid filling the neural tube (Figure 1B). This suggests that even this early in development, shortly after neural tube closure, the central nervous system (CNS) is separate from the rest of the embryo.

Similar results were obtained in chick embryos by Cohen and Davies (1938). The experiments of Wislocki, Weed and Cohen and Davies are the developmental equivalent of what are sometimes described as Goldmann's 1st and 2nd experiments, in which he reported iv injection (1909) and intrathecal injection (1913) of trypan blue in adult animals of various species. The evidence from the experiments of Weed, Wislocki, Chen and Davies in developing animals for the existence of a barrier between the blood and brain and blood and CSF was as convincing as Goldmann's experiments in adult animals. Yet at some point a belief, which has persisted for nearly 100 years, arose that in embryos, fetuses and even in the newborn and older, the blood-brain barrier is absent, leaky or immature (e.g., Statz and Felgenhauer, 1983; Adinolfi, 1985; Davies and Rudd, 1994; Rodier, 1994, 1995, 2004; Ahmed et al., 1996; ATSDR, 1999, 2007, 2014; Järup, 2003; Costa et al., 2004; Watson et al., 2006; Mhanna et al., 2008; Baldrick, 2010; Ehman, 2010; Srinivasan et al., 2012). These and other authors appear to equate immaturity with “leakiness.” The developing brain in all respects is by definition yet to reach a mature adult state and therefore can rightly be described as immature. But this does not tell us whether its brain barriers are “leaky.” The real question is their functional status, which seems to us to be more a matter of the relation between barrier functions (which are much more than the presence or absence of passive permeability) that are present at any particular age and stage of brain development. We wondered why such a dogma continues to be accepted in view of overwhelming experimental evidence to the contrary gathered over many decades, for as pointed out by Dobbing (1961): “There has never been a shred of satisfactory evidence in favor of this proposition.”

The general explanation seems to be a philosophical rather than an experimentally based one. There seems to have been an underlying assumption, often not clearly enunciated, that because the embryonic and newborn brains are developing, adult barrier mechanisms would necessarily be immature or absent. This was most explicitly stated in “The brain and its environment” by the English physiologist Barcroft (1938): “There is no reason why the brain of the embryo should require an environment of very great chemical constancy. It will of course require a certain minimum of the various materials necessary for growth, but otherwise on first principles we might suppose that the good things of life may exist in and may vary in fetal blood to an extent much greater than in the maternal.” However, this is a teleological argument, implying need/requirement or in this case, lack of need/requirement. One could as well argue that a complex organ like the brain would need (require) a specialized environment in which to develop rather than the general “soup” of the embryo as a whole. However, neither argument has any intellectual merit as they are based on opinion rather than experimental evidence. More recently, and in the face of much evidence to the contrary since Barcroft's time, Zheng (2002) has put forward a similar teleological argument: “During early development, the rapid growth of the cerebral cortex perhaps necessities a “leaky” structure of the BBB to accommodate the high demand of blood-borne nutrients for brain growth.” However, as we discuss below, the greater entry of some amino acids into the developing brain is a reflection of much higher expression of their transporters in brain endothelial and choroid plexus epithelial cells. This higher expression, which indicates a developmental specialization, is likely to be a reflection of the importance of amino acids for developing brain rather than a manifestation of a non-specific leak.

Concept of the blood-brain barrier

Before dealing with experimental evidence of brain barriers during development we need to consider the concept of the adult blood-brain barrier itself. This is often attributed to Ehrlich on the basis of his experiments with dyes (e.g., Gröntoft, 1954; Bakay, 1956; Dempsey and Wislocki, 1955; Lee, 1971; Davson et al., 1987; Risau and Seulberger, 1990; Risau and Wolburg, 1990; Hawkins and Davis, 2005; Nag, 2011). However, as pointed out by Bradbury (1995); Ehrlich (1885, 1906) himself never subscribed to the view that the lack of staining of the brain by parenterally administered dyes could be ascribed to some special property of the cerebral endothelial cells. This idea was proposed by Biedl and Kraus (1898); Roux and Borrel (1898), and Lewandowsky (1900) based on comparison of intrathecal and parenteral injections of materials with neurotoxic effects, such as bile salts, after injection by the former route. This was specifically rejected by Ehrlich: “I am unable to accept that the vascular endothelium, as such, exercises different functions in different organs, so that, for example a liver capillary is permeable for certain substances that will not pass through other capillaries” (Ehrlich, 1906). This seems a rather curious conclusion given the description of some of his earlier dye studies (Ehrlich, 1885). In his extensive MD thesis (“Habilitationsschrift”) Ehrlich was primarily using dyes as a means of determining the oxygen usage in different organs by their color change (low oxygen or reduction resulting in a colorless state of the dye and on oxygenation resulted in the reverse). He used mainly Alizarin Blue-S (a compound of Alizarin blue and sodium bisulfite) and Indophenol white (which became blue when exposed to oxygen), injected usually subcutaneously into animals of various species. Both of these dyes stained the brain blue, exclusively in gray matter. Under conditions of reduced oxygen (as post mortem) the dyes became colorless. Ehrlich also used dyes treated with acids or heavy metals to give larger particle sizes-alizarin-sodium sulfite-alum and Coerulin-S. These did not stain the brain following subcutaneous injection. To explain this Ehrlich proposed, that “the brain was the most “finely porous” of the organs” he studied. Ehrlich does not say if these “pores” were in blood vessel walls or some other interface between blood and brain tissue. Later Ehrlich (1906) reported on a much larger number of dyes and concluded that acid dyes (with the exception of alizirin) did not stain the gray matter of the brain whereas many basic dyes did. He suggested that dye experiments could be explained by lack of affinity of brain tissue for the dyes rather than to a special property of brain blood vessels. So perhaps Ehrlich could best be described as the reluctant discoverer of the blood-brain barrier phenomenon, but not as the originator of the term “blood-brain barrier.”

Many authors, including Spatz (1934), Broman (1941), Davson (1967), Davson et al. (1987), Hawkins and Davis (2005), Engelhardt (2003, 2006), Ribatti et al. (2006), Saunders et al. (2012) have attributed the term “Blut-Hirnschranke” (blood-brain barrier) to Lewandowsky's (1900) publication.

Lewandowsky (1900) used strychnine and sodium ferrocyanide, in much higher doses than the potassium ferrocyanide used by Weed (1917b), who stated that in his experiments it was not toxic. Lewandowsky demonstrated in a variety of animal species that the toxins were effective in much lower doses when injected into the brain than parenterally. He also attributed the lesser penetration of strychnine and sodium ferrocyanide into the brain following subcutaneous injection compared to subarachnoid injection to a difference in the properties of the cerebral vessels compared to other tissues: “… die Hypothese dass die Cappillarwand den Uebertritt bestimmter Stoffe, wie des Natriumferrocyanats verhindert, oder mit anderen Worten, dass seine Affinitat der Capillarzellen des Centralnervensystems nothwendig ist um bestimmte Stoffe an die Nervenzellen hindurchgelangen zu lassen.”: the hypothesis that the capillary wall blocks the transfer of certain molecules like sodium ferrocyanide or in other words, that an affinity of capillary cells of the CNS is necessary in order to allow the transfer of certain molecules to nerve cells. A close reading of Lewandowsky's original paper reveals that the term “Blut-Hirnschranke” was not actually used, although as indicated by the above quotation, Lewandowsky clearly proposed that cerebral capillaries have specific restrictive properties with respect to some compounds.

The first use of the term “barrier” appears to be by Stern and Gautier (1918a,b), the latter paper is entitled “La barrière qui s'oppose au passage dans le liquide céphalo-rachidien de substances circulant dans le sang présente des différences notables suivant les espèces animales”: The barrier which opposes the movement into the CSF of substances in the blood shows notable differences in different animal species (Stern and Gautier, 1918b). Stern and colleagues subsequently performed detailed studies of penetration of a wide range of molecules from blood into cerebrospinal fluid (Stern and Gautier, 1921) into brain (Stern and Gautier, 1922) and within the subarachnoid and ventricular CSF (Stern and Gautier, 1923) of adult animals. It was Stern who first described the importance of the interstitial fluid environment of the brain for its normal function, very much in the tradition of Claude Bernard's milieu interne (1865).

Malamud et al. (1928), Franceschini (1929) and Joó (1987) appear to be among only a few authors to have acknowledged that Stern was probably the first to coin the term blood-brain barrier (“barrière hémato-encéphalique,” Stern and Gautier, 1921). Stern and Gautier were careful to point out that this term did not prejudge either the anatomical nature or functional mechanisms involved. The term “blood-brain barrier” has withstood the test of time, notwithstanding that to people outside the field it is misleading in that it implies an absolute impediment to entry into the brain and does not make it explicitly clear that exchange mechanism are involved—exactly as supposed by Stern when she proposed the term. However, Stern's important contributions to the field of blood-brain barrier biology have either been largely ignored or belittled (see below).

In addition to Lewandowsky's 1900 experiments, Biedl and Kraus (1898) injected bile and bile acids either systemically or directly into the brain. Only in the case of the latter did they see any toxic effects. They ascribed the ineffectiveness of systemically injected bile salts to “the relative impermeability of the capillary walls,” although they did not use the term “barrier” to describe this effect. Roux and Borrel (1898) reported on the comparison of subcutaneous and subdural injections of tetanus toxin with the finding that much less toxin is required to provoke central manifestations of tetanus, but only in rabbits; other species were unaffected. Their explanation for the difference was “sans doute, à ce que beaucoup du poison introduit n'arrive pas aux cellules nerveuses et est detruit quelque part dans l'organisme.”: without doubt little of the toxin reaches nervous tissue and is destroyed elsewhere in the organism. However, they did not comment on the mechanism that prevented toxin from reaching the brain.

Amongst the most cited papers for the concept of the blood-brain barrier are those of Goldmann (1909, 1913). He compared distribution of the dye trypan blue following parenteral or intrathecal injection. These are sometimes referred to as Goldmann's first and second experiments (Davson, 1976; Bradbury, 1979). Goldmann found that parenterally injected dye did not stain the brain (Goldmann, 1909), whereas it did when injected into the subarachnoid space, (Goldmann, 1913) as illustrated in Figure 2. Goldmann is usually given the credit for being the first to use a dye to demonstrate that some dyes administered parenterally do not enter the brain. Goldmann himself acknowledges earlier dye studies of Bouffard (1906) but incorrectly states that Bouffard used trypan blue. Bouffard makes an unreferenced mention of even earlier studies with trypan red and Congo red and describes his own studies with methylene blue. Bouffard (1906) injected this dye by various routes (subcutaneous, intravenous, intramuscular or intracardiac) into mice, guinea pigs and rabbits in single or multiple doses of increasing size. He reported unequivocally that there was no staining of the nervous tissue: “SYSTEME NERVEUX.—Aucune trace de couleur, ni dans les cellules nerveuses, ni dans les éléments névrogliques.” Bouffard did not report any toxic effects of methylene blue and did not seem to suggest any mechanism to explain the lack of staining of the nervous system. Bouffard (1906) mentioned, but did not reference, an even earlier study by a student of Ehrlich's, Franke, who used trypan red. According to Bouffard, Franke also showed that the dye did not enter the brain although it did enter most other tissues and organs. This citation appears to have been taken from Franke's (1905) thesis, in which Franke stated “Das Trypanrot gehört zu den polytropen Farbstoffen im Sinne EHRLICHS; es wird entsprechend den Beobachtungen, die bei anderen saueren Farbstoffen gemacht worden sind, von allen Organen mit Ausnahme des Nervensystems, aufgenommen”: Trypan red belongs to the polytropic dyes according to Ehrlich; it is taken up,- as observed with other acidic dyes, by all organs except the nervous system. None of these authors seems to have offered any explanation of the lack of staining of the brain by the different dyes; indeed Goldmann (1913) appears to have been surprised by this observation: “Es ist mir rätselhaft dass grosse Farbstoffmengen, welche bei der intravenösen Applikation in den Gehirngefässen zirkulieren, selbst die feinste Kapillarwand nicht zu durchdringen vermögen”: I fail to understand why high concentrations of dye circulating in the blood vessels after intravenous injection are unable to penetrate even the walls of the finest capillaries (Goldmann, 1913, p. 45).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Goldmann's trypan blue injection experiments. (A) Adult rat following systemic injection of trypan blue solution (Goldmann's “1st experiment”). From Goldmann (1909). (B) Brain and spinal cord of adult rat following lumbar subarachnoid injection of 0.5 ml, 0.5% trypan blue (Goldmann's “2nd experiment”). From Goldmann (1913).

Goldmann (1913) did however, remark on the strong staining of the choroid plexus with trypan blue and suggested this represented the site at which dyes were prevented from entering the brain. Goldmann (1909, 1913) reported toxic effects with injected trypan blue and commented that the toxic effects that occurred following subarachnoid injections of the dye were made with amounts of trypan blue that were much less than when injected intravenously, particularly in rabbits. Because the choroid plexus stained very strongly with the dye, Goldmann suggested that it was primarily the choroid plexus that provided a protective mechanism for the brain and that this was the route by which some substances entered the brain. This subsequently led to the use of the term “Weg über den Liquor,” although not by Goldmann or by von Monakow to whom it is sometimes attributed.

But Goldmann clearly thought that the choroid plexuses had a protective role in preventing entry of toxic materials into the brain: “Der Plexus vermag andererseits die Zerebrospinalflüssigkeit und damit die Nervensubstanz vor dem Eindringen von Substanzen zu schützen, die sich bei der direkten Einfuhr in den Subarachnoidealraum als ein schweres Gift für die Ganglienzellen erweisen.” (Goldmann, 1913, p. 54): On the other hand the choroid plexus is capable to protect the liquor and therefore also the nervous substance against the influx of substances which when directly transferred into the subarachnoid space are a strong toxin for the ganglionic cells. He then summarizes his view of the choroid plexus as a protective regulatory mechanism: “Wir kommen somit zu dem Schluß, dass der Plexus ein wichtiger Schutz-und Regulationsmechanismus für das zentrale Nervensystem darstellt, und dass differenten Substanzen Tür und Tor für ihren Eintritt in das zentrale Nervensystem geöffnet werden, wenn die physiologische Eintrittspforte, der Plexus chorioideus, umgangen wird und solche differente Stoffe durch die Lumbalpunktion in eine direkte Berührung mit den Nervenelementen gebracht warden.” (Goldmann, 1913, p. 55): Thus we conclude that the plexus is an important protective-and regulatory mechanism for the CNS and that various substances will freely enter the CNS if the physiological gatekeeper, the choroid plexus, is circumvented by a lumbar puncture, those substances will come into direct contact with the nervous elements. However it should be noted that Goldmann makes no mention of any protective function of cerebral blood vessels.

Route of entry from blood into brain

Stemming from the studies of Goldmann (1909, 1913) described above and supported by von Monakow (1921) there came the suggestion that the choroid plexuses, rather than the cerebral blood vessels, were the CNS protective mechanism and the primary route of entry of nutritive substances into the brain (described by the term “Weg über den Liquor” by later authors). However, this proposed nutritive function of the choroid plexus seems to have been current earlier than this (see Meek, 1907). Stern in particular initially supported this idea in her interpretation of extensive studies of entry of a wide range of molecules from blood into CSF (Stern and Gautier, 1921) and brain (Stern and Gautier, 1922) in which she emphasized the importance of the choroid plexuses in this role. Davson (1956, 1976) considered this an untenable hypothesis and dismissed the idea as “intellectually inadequate” as it did not take account of substantial vascular circulation of the brain parenchyma. This view was shared by Friedemann (1942): “It is utterly unlikely that in an organ of the vital importance of the brain the capillaries should lack their chief uses, namely, mediation of the exchange between blood and tissue and adaptation of blood supply to functional needs” (Friedemann, 1942, p. 126). The idea was also dismissed by several other German authors (e.g., Friedemann and Elkeles, 1931, 1934; Walter, 1934). However, this dismissal is not justified, as these authors have ignored or miss-cited Stern and Rapoport's short papers (e.g., Stern and Rapoport, 1928a,b) from which it is clear that they believed in the existence of two routes of entry from blood into brain: one directly across the blood vessels (endothelial blood-brain barrier) and the other via the choroid plexuses (Goldmann and Stern's blood-brain barrier, now referred to as the epithelial blood-cerebrospinal fluid-barrier). For example, Stern and Rapoport (1928a) reported that in animals subjected to carbon monoxide poisoning, dyes entered brain tissue directly across the cerebral blood vessels, but only sodium ferrocyanide could be detected in the CSF, which was interpreted as suggesting two different routes of entry into the brain, one (for trypan blue or similar colloids) at the vascular endothelial cells of brain capillaries, the other (for sodium ferrocyanide) via the plexuses.

In Geneva Lina Stern, who was a physiologist and biochemist, did not have anybody in her laboratory to perform a thorough post-mortem histology of the brain of the experimental animals. In Moscow she found a competent neuropathologist/histologist Yakov Rapoport, who could localize which cells in the brain had been colored by the injected trypan blue or had been revealed by the Prussian blue reaction. His contribution was therefore of great importance. Unfortunately their cooperation came to an end in the early 1930's. It seems likely that the two disagreed on the respective roles of the blood-CSF and blood-brain barriers.

In contradistinction to her previous short papers with Rapoport, on the “two barriers model,” Stern in a short paper (1934) focused her attention on the blood-CSF barrier (which she calls the barrière hémato-encéphalique). Stern admits that the use of dyes or of other yes-or-no biological markers to assess the permeability of the barrière hémato-encéphalique (blood-CSF barrier) is open to criticism. “En effet il n'est guère possible de tirer des conclusions sur le comportement du liquide en partant simplement de la pénétration ou de la non-pénétration d'une substance donnée présente [dans le plasma sanguin].”: Actually the composition of the liquid cannot be directly deduced from the passage or non-passage of a substance present in blood plasma.

According to Stern (1934), the barrier has dual roles of protection and metabolic regulation of the brain. She also introduces the notions of barrier selectivity and barrier resistance. Thus in this 1934 paper, which was one of the last published in French, Stern makes a very clear statement about the importance of the blood-brain barrier in not only providing protection for the brain but also an appropriate physico-chemical environment for normal brain function: “Partant de l'idée que le liquide céphalo-rachidien represénte le milieu nutritif immédiat des éléments nerveux cérébro-spinaux et que la constance relative de la composition chimique et de l'état physico-chimique de ce milieu est une condition sine qua non du fonctionnment normal des centers nerveux, nous avons attributé un rôle de premier plan à l'activité de cet appareil régulateur que représente la barrière hémato-encéphalique.”: Starting with the idea that the CSF represents the immediate nutritive medium for the cells of the brain and spinal cord and that the relative constancy of the chemical composition and physico-chemical state of this medium is the sine qua non for normal function of the CNS we have attributed a primary role for this regulatory activity to the blood-brain barrier. The concept of the blood-brain barrier providing mechanisms that control the internal environment of the brain is a major physiological concept that was decades ahead of its time and really only began to be properly accepted and investigated with the advent of radioactive tracers in the 2nd half of the 20th Century as outlined below.

Stern (1934) does not mention the relative importance of the cerebral vessels (blood-brain barrier) and choroid plexuses (blood-CSF barrier) in these dual functions of protection and regulation of the internal environment of the brain. This remains a technical difficulty to this day. It does seem highly unsatisfactory that later authors did not take account of Stern's direct studies on permeability of the cerebral blood vessels under both normal and pathological conditions. Another extremely important point made by Stern (1934) was her insistence on the necessity of appreciating that the barrier does not have uniform properties with respect to the wide range of molecules that she studied in earlier works: “… le fonctionnement de la barrière ne présente pas un caractère uniforme vis à vis de toutes les substances examinées…”: the function of the barrier is not uniform with respect to all substances studied. This is an extremely important point, which is often still not realized to this day.

In contrast to the adult brain, in the developing brain it is now clear that the choroid plexuses are indeed the main portal of entry into the brain (Johansson et al., 2008). As will be recounted below, after returning from Geneva to Moscow in 1925 (Dreifuss and Sigrist, 2009) Stern made important contributions to the study of brain barriers in developing animals of a number of species in a series of short papers published in the period 1927–1929. If more notice had been taken of Stern's papers, the longstanding belief in the absence of the blood-brain barrier in the fetus and newborn might have been short-lived.

Early dye studies in immature animals

The important paper of Wislocki (1920), discussed above and illustrated in Figure 1A, was rarely cited in most of the literature of the 20th Century, perhaps because it did not fit with the prevailing belief in immaturity of the blood-brain barrier. Even King (1939), who published with Wislocki, does not mention the seminal 1920 paper preferring instead to cite Behnsen (1927) as evidence of barrier immaturity; the deficiencies of the latter study are discussed next. These are perhaps the most cited in support of immaturity or leakiness of the blood-brain barrier (Behnsen, 1926, 1927) particularly his 1927 paper (e.g., Spatz, 1934; King, 1939; Friedemann, 1942; Davson, 1967). In the 1926 paper Behnsen refers to sealing (“Abdichtung”) of the barrier in the postnatal animals, and writes in the summary on page 1146 that the blood brain barrier is considerably more leaky (original: “erheblich durchlässiger”) than in adult mice as staining with trypan blue appears in young animals more intense generally and also in different places of the CNS compared to the staining pattern of adults. However, in some slightly handicapped adult mice Behnsen also found additional areas in the brain which were stained, that he called those weak spots (orig. “schwache Stellen”), besides those where apparently no barrier exists (orig. “offene Stellen”) and puts this in contrast to Goldmann's view that the central nervous is entirely sealed from dye (Goldmann, 1909, 1913). He also suggested: The fact that the blood-brain barrier in the young is more leaky may also relate to immaturity of tissues and cells and a different level of cellular maturity. (orig.: “Im Speicherverhalten der nervösen Zellen zeigen sich Unterschiede, die … auf einen verschiedenen Reifezustand der Zellen zu beziehen sind,” Behnsen, 1926. p. 1146). It may be that Behnsen's finding of dye in some parts of the brain was due to the use of an excessive amount of dye (three injections of 1% trypan blue).

Behnsen's paper of 1927 is longer and more detailed and the results and interpretations differ in some respects to the 1926 paper. He speaks more cautiously about an increasing sealing of the barrier during postnatal development and points out that even in adult mice staining can be found in the brain due to local penetration of dye. He commented: “The sites of dye penetration in the adult animals correspond to the maximal storage in young mice. They are located 1. In the region of Retzius'ependymal wedge in the medulla oblongata; 2. At the attachment sites of the choroid plexus; 3. In the infundibular recess” (Translation of orig.: “Die Farbstoffdurchtrittstellen der erwachsenen Tiere entsprechen den Speicherungsmaxima der jungen Mäuse…”).

Behnsen (1927) himself does not appear to state unequivocally that the barrier is immature in the newborn and his experiments have a number of curious features. As was the case for many barrier studies in the early part of the 20th Century, they involved multiple injections of trypan blue into postnatal (2–3 week old) and adult mice. He reported that areas of the brain that stain for trypan blue at postnatal ages were more extensive than in the adult, but they are located at the same sites, which Behnsen calls “Grenzflächen” i.e., interfaces/membranes between brain and bloodstream or even more generally used by Behnsen: membranes involved in barrier functions are of the same structure in young and adult animals, they only differ gradually in their function. “Die Übereinstimmung der Speicherstellen der erwachsenen Tiere mit denen der jungen macht es sehr wahrscheinlich, daß die Grenzflächen im erwachsenen Zustand, nur graduell verschieden, dieselben örtlichen Bauunterschiede aufweisen, wie sie das Speicherbild für junge Tiere offenbarte.” (Behnsen, 1927, p. 570): The identical storage sites (of dye) in adult animals and in young ones makes it highly likely that the interfaces (“Grenzflächen”) in the adult condition differ only gradually, but are otherwise of the same structure as revealed by the dye storage of young animals.

This is illustrated in two figures reproduced here in Figure 3. The distribution of trypan blue is illustrated in sagittal sections of mouse brain from the two ages. However, it is the same line drawing of an adult brain that is used for both ages. This gives the misleading impression that the distribution of the dye in the postnatal brain was wider than it probably was in the actual microscopical slides. This is because the neonatal brain is less well developed particularly with respect to the neocortex. It is also notable that there is no staining indicated in the postnatal neocortex as this is the region with the least well developed vasculature in the postnatal period compared to the rest of the brain; were the blood-brain barrier to dye to be immature, this is precisely where dye staining would be expected to be apparent. Bakay (1956) commented that although Behnsen's conclusions are frequently misquoted “they do not reveal a generalized increase in permeability of the barrier.”

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Behnsen's much cited trypan blue experiments. (A) Brain from postnatal mouse injected systemically 3 times with trypan blue at P4–P14. (B) Adult mouse brain following systemic injection of trypan blue. Note that as described by Behnsen the dye staining in the postnatal brain is in the same regions as in the adult brain, but more extensive. This is probably a visual artifact due to the use of an outline of an adult mouse brain for both ages. In postnatal mouse brain (P14) the cerebral cortex and cerebellum are less developed than illustrated here. Note also that in the postnatal brain there does not appear to be any dye staining of the neocortex-the region of the developing brain with the least mature blood vessels. From Behnsen (1927).

Behnsen's experiments were repeated in careful detail by Moos and Møllgård (1993). They studied the distribution of trypan blue and Evans blue, a dye that superseded trypan blue from about mid-20th century for blood-brain barrier studies. Moos and Møllgård (1993) used a range of concentrations of the dyes some of which exceeded the binding capacity of plasma albumin to which these dyes bind. Binding of dyes to plasma albumin appears to have been overlooked by the blood-brain barrier field until the observations of Tschirgi (1950), although Ehrlich himself commented that the lack of some of his small molecular weight dyes in urine, was probably accounted for by binding to plasma albumin (Ehrlich, 1885). Also, Stern and colleagues (e.g., Stern et al., 1929) made a distinction in guinea pig embryos and neonatal animals of various species between the blood-brain barrier permeability of low molecular weight sodium ferrocyanide and the lack of penetration of trypan blue, which they described as a colloid. Whether this description reflects knowledge of protein binding or aggregation of the dye in blood is not clear. However, the difference between entry of small lipid insoluble molecules (sodium ferrocyanide in early qualitative experiments and sucrose and inulin in later quantitative experiments) and the lack of entry of trypan blue turned out to be a fundamental distinction in properties of blood-brain and blood-CSF barriers, as will be described below.

Stern et al. (1924) studied the entry of sodium ferrocyanide from blood into CSF in various species at different stages of development. They reported that in the guinea pig the barrier to this molecule was already as in the adult, but in species born at a less mature stage of development (rats, mice, cats) sodium ferrocyanide could be detected in the CSF. Stern and Peyrot (1927) extended the earlier study of Stern et al. (1924) examining entry sodium ferrocyanide into the CSF. In guinea pig fetuses this marker did enter the CSF, but as reported earlier not in the newborn of this species. They confirmed the entry of sodium ferrocyanide in the newborns of rats, mice and cats and in addition rabbits.

Stern and Rapoport (1928a) studied the injection of dyes (trypan blue and Congo red) in neonates of various species. They distinguished clearly between a barrier at the level of the vascular endothelium and the choroid plexus epithelium in dye studies where cellular staining following dye injections, but unlike other authors they thought this was a post mortem phenomenon. In a following year using the same dyes Stern et al. (1929) made the important point that it was necessary to limit the amount dye injected. They stressed that in experiments in adults and developing animals using similar amounts of dye, no dye entered the CSF. But if the amounts were increased substantially (30–40 times than used normally) then staining of the CSF occurred.

Another dye study that is sometimes cited as evidence of barrier immaturity in neonates (e.g., Ribatti et al., 2006) is that of Penta (1932). Penta injected multiple doses of trypan blue or trypan red into postnatal rabbits and cats. Penta seems to have considered that the results of Behnsen and Stern were similar, although as described above Stern et al. (1929) found no entry of trypan blue in the brains of developing animals providing not too much dye was injected. Penta summarized his results as showing that the nervous system of neonates is colored more intensely than that of adults and staining occurred in the regions where the mesenchyme penetrates into the neuraxis. As in many of the adult studies using trypan blue and against the advice of Stern (Stern et al., 1929) Penta mainly used multiple large doses of trypan blue and many of the animals died from the toxic effects of the dye; therefore staining of the brain was hardly surprising. However, Penta did describe one experiment in which much less dye was used in which no staining of the brain (apart from the choroid plexus) occurred, thus confirming Stern's results, without Penta apparently realizing it.

There seems little doubt that at least part of the problem with many early dye studies, both in developing animals and adults, was that the large amounts and multiple injections used led to toxic effects. This might be indicative of a greater fragility of brain barriers in immature animals but should not be interpreted as evidence of an immature or absent blood brain barrier.

The binding of dyes to albumin in plasma was investigated systematically by Moos and Møllgård (1993). They showed that dye only entered the brains of neonatal (one day old) mice and rats if the albumin binding capacity was exceeded, except in regions of the brain known to be devoid of a blood-brain barrier. For concentrations of dye (trypan blue, 1–2%; Evans blue, 0.5–1.0%) that were shown not to exceed the binding capacity of albumin in plasma, neither dye penetrated into the brain by exudation across cerebral vessel walls in either P10 or adult mice, although there was some staining of choroid plexus epithelial and cerebral endothelial cells. At higher concentrations of the dyes (trypan blue, 2–4%; Evans blue, 2%) at which there was a substantial level of free dye, 60% of neonates and 20% of adults died, with higher proportions at the higher concentrations of dye. Those animals that survived showed various signs of neurotoxicity. Toxic effects of trypan blue had been reported earlier (e.g., Behnsen, 1927; Penta, 1932) but the significance of this in interpretation of barrier studies with dyes seems to have been overlooked by all but Stern who urged caution in the amount of dye to be injected into neonatal animals (Stern et al., 1929).

Later studies of dyes, albumin and other markers in immature animals

The use of dyes to study blood-brain barrier permeability in immature animals began to die out in the second half of the 20th Century. This is in contrast to the adult blood-brain barrier field, which continues to place a heavy reliance on dyes, particularly Evans blue, for assessing blood-brain barrier integrity. All of the studies published after 1950 agreed in showing that dyes (usually trypan blue) did not enter the developing brain no matter what age was investigated. The most important of these studies is probably that of Gröntoft (1954) who did experiments in both human fetuses (recent abortions) and newborns. Gröntoft emphasized that the condition of the fetuses was very important in determining whether or not the dye entered the brain. In a series of carefully conducted experiments he showed clearly that when trypan blue was injected as soon as possible after separation of the placenta in human fetuses of a wide range of gestational age, the dye did not stain the brain as a whole, but only those areas (choroid plexus and circumventricular organs) that are outside the blood-brain barrier (Figure 4); this was the case even in a very immature embryo of 5 cm in length (approximately 10 weeks post conception). However, when the dye was injected at progressively longer intervals after placental separation of the fetuses (10–30 min depending upon the gestational age) then the whole of the brain stained. Gröntoft carried out similar well-controlled experiments in rabbit fetuses with the same findings.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Gröntoft's dye experiments with postmortem aborted human fetuses. (A) Human fetuses (14 cm-18 weeks to 30 cm-31 weeks gestation) injected with trypan blue solution 30 min after caesarian section delivery. (B) Human fetuses (14 cm-18 weeks to 30 cm-31 weeks gestation) injected with trypan blue solution 10 min after caesarian section delivery. Note that for the shorter period of anoxia none of the brains stained with trypan blue indicating intact blood-brain barrier to trypan blue even in immature human fetuses. Reproduced with permission from Gröntoft (1954), © Wiley.

Grazer and Clemente (1957) injected by various routes solutions of trypan blue into rat embryos from E10.5 to birth. They found no evidence of dye penetration into the brain. Millen and Hess (1958) injected i.p. two doses, 24 h apart, of either trypan blue or sodium ferrocyanide, into neonatal (P0–P8) and adult rats and the animals were examined after 30 min. No staining of the brain for either marker was identified in the neonatal animals apart from the restricted regions (outside the blood-brain barrier) that also stained in the adult. Similar results were reported by Olsson et al. (1968) who injected fluorescently labeled bovine albumin intravascularly in rat embryos as early as E15, compared with neonates and adults.

In the second half of the 20th Century there was a switch from using dyes to using horseradish peroxidase (HRP) as a test of barrier integrity in the developing brain. HRP has a number of problems that are not always well recognized by those that use it. In the context of development the main problem is that, as with the dyes in earlier experiments, too large a volume or concentration of HRP was injected, thus damaging fragile cerebral blood vessels and giving the appearance of a “leaky” blood-brain barrier (see Table 1). Several other authors reported the entry of HRP into brains of very immature embryos, for example chick embryos (Delorme et al., 1970; Delorme, 1972; Bertossi et al., 1992). However, insufficient detail of exactly what was injected makes it difficult to evaluate the results of these studies. The finding that HRP entered the brain at different embryonic ages in the same species or at different stages of brain development in different species suggests that the findings from these studies are likely to have been artefactual. There have been a few reports of transendothelial passage of markers such as HRP in developing brain (e.g., in tubulovesicular structures, Lossinsky et al., 1986) which may explain the entry of the marker into immature brain in some experiments, as lack of pinocytotic vesicles is a characteristic of adult cerebral endothelial cells.

Table 1.

Injection experiments used as test of blood-brain barrier integrity in fetal and neonatal animals.

Authors Species Age◊ (days) Body weight (g)a Injection volb (% total blood vol) Increase in plasma protein concentrationc “Leaky” barrier claimed• Injected materiald
Grazer and Clemente, 1957 Rat E10.5 <0.06 Not stated No Trypan blue i.p./i.v.b
Olsson et al., 1968 Rat E15 0.14 <5% No Fluorescein- albumin, Umb.A.
Delorme et al., 1970 Chick E4.5 0.1 Not stated 3% Reaction product in neuropil ECS until E10 HRP i.v.
Wakai and Hirokawa, 1978, 1981 Chick E8 0.5 10% 500% Yes HRP i.v.
Dziegielewska et al., 1979, 1980 Sheep E60 60 7.5% 20% No Human plasma protein i.v.
Lossinsky et al., 1986 Mouse Newborn 1.4 35% 2% Yes, “massive leakage” HRP i.v.
Roncali et al., 1986 Chick E6 0.34 >150% 50% “Unimpeded extravasation” HRP intracardiace
Risau et al., 1986 Mouse E13 0.08 250% 1% increase “Fully permeable” HRP intracardiace
Vorbrodt et al., 1986 Mouse Newborn 1.4 100% 200–300% Yes** HRP i.v.
Stewart and Hayakawa, 1987 Mouse E15 0.26 Not stated 100%* Yes HRP i.p.f
Dziegielewska et al., 1988 Tammar wallaby Newborn 0.50 10–20% 25–50% No HRP/HSA i.v.
Hulsebosch and Fabian, 1989 Rat Newborn 5.0 50%* >400%* IgG penetration into neuraxis IgG i.p.b
Dermietzel et al., 1992 Rat E14.5 0.14 100–150% >100% Yes HRP or 40 kDa dextran intracardiacd
Ribatti et al., 1993 Chick E6 0.36a >150% 50% “Massive diffusion” HRP or Evans blue, intracardiac
Xu and Ling, 1994 Rat Newborn 5.0 10% 65% Yes Ferritin solution i.v.
Knott et al., 1997 Opossum P3 0.20 8%* 0.5%* No Human albumin i.p.
a

Body weights and circulating blood and plasma volumes for chick embryos from Romanoff (1967) and Davis and Garrison (1968). Body weights of fetal rats from unpublished data. Body weights of opossum from Saunders et al. (1989) and unpublished. Weight of youngest animal given.

b

The authors' injection volume when stated has been compared to estimates of circulating blood volume (10% of body weight and allowing a factor of 2 for placental blood volume in the case of fetuses and for extra-embryonic blood volume in chick embryos, although it is doubtful if under the conditions of some of these experiments, much mixing with fetal placental blood would have occurred.

c

The actual circulating blood volume in sheep fetuses has been measured (Dziegielewska et al., 1991). The effect of the amount of injected protein on the plasma protein concentration is also estimated when sufficient information is available. Plasma protein concentrations from Birge et al. (1974, chick embryo) and Dziegielewska et al. (1981, rat fetus).

d

Information is not usually available about the speed of injection nor about the likely distribution volume (e.g., in experiments involving intracardiac injection.

e

In which the normal circulation may not have continued or i.p. injection.

f

In which it is not clear what proportion of the volume and protein will have entered the circulation).

Youngest age studied. E is embryonic age from day of conception.

*

Depending on proportion entering circulation.

**

But “not detected at E19.”

•Not all authors of experiments in which marker entered brain tissue from blood vessels describe this as a “leak” or due to immaturity.

Entry of small molecular compounds into the brain and CSF of immature animals

Lina Stern and her colleagues published a preliminary report of studies in a range of species of developing animals (Stern et al., 1924). This was followed by a series of brief papers in the period 1927–1929 on the permeability of the “barrière hémato-encéphalique” to sodium ferrocyanide and several other low molecular weight compounds in fetal guinea pigs and newborns of several species (Stern and Peyrot, 1927; Stern and Rapoport, 1928a,b; Stern et al., 1929). Following injection into fetuses via the uterine horn of pregnant guinea pigs early in gestation, sodium ferrocyanide could be demonstrated by the Prussian blue reaction in the CSF and CNS tissue. However, when sodium ferrocyanide was injected into term embryo or newborn guinea pigs its presence could not be detected in either CSF or CNS tissue. In contrast, when sodium ferrocyanide was injected into the newborns of rabbits, rats, mice, dogs, and cats it could be demonstrated in CSF and nervous system tissue and for several days into the neonatal period. Stern and Peyrot (1927) correlated the cessation of sodium ferrocyanide entry into CSF and nervous tissue with the time of eye opening and suggested that this was the time when the blood-brain barrier (“barrière hémato-encéphalique”) to certain substances reached a “normal value,” presumably meaning that it reached the adult condition. They concluded that in species born at less mature stages of development (which they suggested probably included human newborns) the “activity” of the barrier was insufficient to protect the CNS against many substances circulating or introduced into the blood.

These qualitative observational results with a small molecule (sodium ferrocyanide 304 Da) foreshadowed quantitative experiments with radiolabelled small molecules such as mannitol (182 Da), sucrose (342 Da), and inulin (5200 Da) in newborns of rats (Ferguson and Woodbury, 1969; Woodbury et al., 1974; Johanson, 1989; Habgood et al., 1993) and fetal sheep (Evans et al., 1974; Dziegielewska et al., 1979), which showed higher CSF/plasma and brain/plasma ratios for the these molecules in immature animals. Although initially interpreted as indicating greater permeability of the blood-brain and blood CSF barriers, it became apparent from measurements of CSF secretion in embryos (Evans et al., 1974; Fossan et al., 1985) and newborns (Bass and Lundborg, 1973; Johanson and Woodbury, 1974) and increase in volume of CSF in rat embryos (Johansson et al., 2006, 2008) that the explanation is more likely to be that the much lower turnover (sink effect) of CSF in the developing brain allowed a greater accumulation of marker in CSF and brain (Bass and Lundborg, 1973; Johansson et al., 2008). However, it took the development of the use of small molecular weight markers that could be both quantified and visualized at the electron microscopic, EM level to show that the route of penetration from blood to CSF of these small markers was via an intracellular route in a subpopulation of epithelial cells in the choroid plexuses (Ek et al., 2001, 2003, 2006). These cells appear to be responsible for the transfer of inert dextrans and several plasma proteins from blood into CSF both in the developing and adult brain (Dziegielewska et al., 1991; Liddelow et al., 2009, 2011a). The absolute number of cells involved in this transfer actually increases with age as the choroid plexuses develop, but the CSF/plasma ratios for dextrans and proteins fall, because of the increased turnover of CSF and a larger space into which these markers are diluted (Johansson et al., 2006, 2008).

Kernicterus in the newborn

One of the persistent beliefs about the blood-brain barrier in the immature organism is that the toxic effects of unconjugated bilirubin that accumulate in the blood of newborn infants suffering from erythroblastosis, generally due to Rhesus blood group incompatibility, are due to “immaturity” of the blood-brain barrier in the newborn. This is a suggestion generally attributed to Spatz (1934) e.g., Broman (1941); Davson (1967), and Lee (1971). Others stated that this was the case but without attribution (e.g., Ganong, 1993; Cotran et al., 1994). Bakay (1956) expressed the view that “the phenomenon of kernicterus appeared to offer irrefutable proof of this theory [that the blood-brain barrier in the newborn is immature].” In his later review Bakay is more circumspect and suggests that the causation of kernicterus has many components of which an immature blood-brain barrier might be one (Bakay, 1968).

Most of Spatz's lengthy paper is taken up with describing and repeating some of Behnsen's (1927) experiments with trypan blue. But he does have a short section on icterus (jaundice) in the adult, with an illustration showing that yellow pigment of bilirubin was confined to dura. He compared this (without illustration) to brain damage (kernicterus) that only occurs in the newborn with the deposition of bilirubin in the brain, characteristically in the basal ganglia and brain stem (Figure 5). He does not seem to distinguish between conjugated bilirubin, which accumulates in the circulation of adults sometimes to very high levels, and unconjugated bilirubin that accumulates in newborns because the activity of the conjugating liver enzyme, glucuronyl transferase, is very low at birth and normally increases during the first days of life. These two forms of bilirubin had been identified earlier (van den Bergh and Müller, 1916). What was not known in the 1930s was that unconjugated bilirubin binds tightly to plasma albumin and a key factor (but not the only one) in the occurrence of kernicterus is when the binding capacity of albumin for unconjugated bilirubin is exceeded; this form of bilirubin which is highly lipid soluble (in contrast to conjugated bilirubin) enters the brain without restraint.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Kernicterus in brain of neonatal monkey with jaundice. Note staining of putamen (one of the basal ganglia), which does not occur in adult brain as jaundice in adults, is generally due to high levels of conjugated bilirubin to which the blood brain barrier is impermeable. From Windle (1969).

It is noteworthy that clinicians, who have had to deal with the problem of kernicterus in the newborn for many years, rarely seem to have invoked the state of maturity of the blood-brain barrier in the neonate as a major factor in the occurrence of kernicterus (e.g., Iskander et al., 2014), although it has occasionally been mentioned (e.g., Zuelzer and Mudgett, 1950; Spiegel-Adolf et al., 1954; Ernster et al., 1957); rather they developed clinical treatments aimed at reducing the level of unconjugated bilirubin in infants who were at risk: exchange transfusion (Allen et al., 1950; Gerrard, 1952) barbiturate induction of glucuronyl transferase and phototherapy (Lucey, 1971) as well as prevention by immunization of Rh-ve women against the D blood group antigen (Clarke, 1989). But the linking of blood-brain barrier immaturity to kernicterus has persisted amongst other groups of scientists and clinicians for decades after Spatz (1934). A good example is a physiologist, (Ganong, 1993) who stated that: “Bile pigments do not enter the brain in the adult but in the fetus and newborn infant the blood-brain barrier is not developed.” Following correspondence with one author (NRS) in later editions Ganong (1999) modified this to state that “the bilirubin penetrates the brain of infants with erythroblastosis at least in part because the blood-brain barrier is more permeable in infancy”; he was not willing to take account of the effect of binding of unconjugated bilirubin to plasma albumin. Similar wording has been retained by the new authors of Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology (Barrett et al., 2012) following his unfortunate demise. Some aspects of bilirubin neural toxicity are discussed by Palmela et al. (submitted).

Some pathologists have also considered that kernicterus is due to immaturity of the blood-brain barrier (Cotran et al., 1994). A detailed consideration of kernicterus is beyond the scope of this review. But comprehensive papers are available both on the pathogenesis and treatment particularly by Wennberg and his colleagues (e.g., Wennberg, 2000; Ahlfors and Wennberg, 2004; Wennberg et al., 2006) and in Volpe (2008). However, it is becoming increasingly clear that the mechanisms involved in whether or not kernicterus occurs in newborns are much more complex than the absolute level of unconjugated bilirubin and the binding capacity of albumin in plasma. These complexities have been reviewed by Ghersi-Egea et al. (2009) and include the finding that bilirubin is a substrate for several ABC efflux transporters, which would thus be expected to contribute to limiting the entry of bilirubin into the brain.

Experiments with radioisotopically labeled molecules in developing animals

The introduction of radiolabelled markers was a major advance in experimental approaches to understanding blood-brain barrier mechanisms. The use of inert markers of permeability such as sucrose and inulin has been mentioned above. In this section we shall deal briefly with experiments involving the use of P32 and amino acids labeled with 3H or 14C. Compared to inert markers there are potential problems with metabolically active radiolabelled molecules such as amino acids. Thus it needs to be shown that the radioactive tag is still attached to the molecule and that the measured radioactivity is due to transfer into the brain separate from its metabolic incorporation, for example into proteins.

32Phosphorus

Bakay (1953, 1956) studied the uptake of 32P into brain and other organs following parenteral injection into pregnant (3rd–4th week), newborn, up to 7 weeks postnatal rabbits and in the adult. Animals were killed 24 h after injection and various organs dissected out. In some cases radioautographs of the heads or brains of fetuses were prepared. The results showed a much higher uptake of 32P in the younger the animals. In fetuses and newborns the brain/blood ratio was >1, compared to about 0.3 in the adult, which Bakay (1953) interpreted as indicating that the vascular permeability in the fetal brain is increased. He discusses whether the increased permeability reflects a leaky, incompletely formed blood-brain barrier or increased metabolism and faster turnover in the developing nervous system. He concluded that neither explanation was satisfactory. Bakay (1953) observed that in the radioautographs of fetal brain the distribution of 32P was almost uniform whereas in the brain of the mother it was concentrated in the choroid plexuses and the lining of the ventricles. Rather puzzlingly he took this as evidence that there is a blood-brain barrier to 32P in the fetus but that its permeability is greater than in the adult.

Amino acid transport

In early experiments with radiolabelled amino acids there was a similar finding of a much greater entry of some (but not all) into the developing than adult brain of several species (Himwich et al., 1957; Roberts et al., 1959; Purpura and Carmichael, 1960; Lajtha and Toth, 1961; Seta et al., 1972; Baños et al., 1978) but as with 32P it was not possible to distinguish between cerebral endothelial cell transport and metabolic incorporation into brain tissue in accounting for the greater uptake in developing brain. Some authors interpreted such results as indicating “lesser effectiveness” of the blood-brain barrier for amino acids in young animals (e.g., Kuttner et al., 1961; Lee, 1971). The problem of determining cerebral transport of amino acids across the blood-brain barrier without the complication of metabolic contamination of brain samples was solved by Oldendorf (1971) who developed a short pass technique in which exposure of amino acid or other metabolically active molecules to the brain from the cerebrovascular circulating blood was limited to one circulation time, thus being too quick for significant metabolic incorporation to occur. The technique was adapted for newborn and postnatal animals by Oldendorf's students (Braun et al., 1980; Cornford et al., 1982; Pardridge and Mietus, 1982) and by Lefauconnier and Trouvé (1983), Al-Sarraf (2002), and Al-Sarraf et al. (1997). These experiments showed that many amino acids and other metabolically active compounds were transported into developing brain at much higher rates than in the adult; this was interpreted as reflecting the greater metabolic demand of the developing brain rather than immaturity of the blood-brain barrier. However, some authors have continued to suggest that greater uptake may reflect barrier immaturity (e.g., Watson et al., 2006).

Thus it has been clear for sometime that transport mechanisms at the brain barrier interfaces determine the composition of the internal environment of developing brain and supply essential nutrients and other molecules important for growth and differentiation of the brain. What has been lacking until recently is information on the presence and expression of specific transporters in the brain barrier interfaces. This is now available from expression studies of isolated cerebral endothelial cells from neonatal mice (Daneman et al., 2010a) and from choroid plexuses of fetal mice and rats compared to the adult (Liddelow et al., 2012, submitted). Many of these so called Slc transporters are expressed at higher levels in the developing brain compared to the adult, thus accounting for the earlier observations of higher entry of specific amino acids into the developing brain. There are differences in the expression patterns of these Slc transporters in the cerebral endothelial cells and in the choroid plexus epithelial cells and many more are expressed at higher levels in the fetal choroid plexus (see reviews by Saunders et al., 2013 and Liddelow et al., submitted). This reflects the greater importance of the choroid plexuses for transport into the brain compared to the vasculature at very early stages of brain development. The plexuses develop much earlier than the blood supply (Johansson et al., 2008). The findings also help to explain a number of important observations on developmentally different effects of amino acids on brain function. For example it was reported many years ago that glutamate is toxic to the brain if administered in the neonatal period (Olney and Ho, 1970) which some attributed to “immaturity” of the blood-brain barrier (Viña et al., 1997). However, it can now be seen that the barrier contribution to toxicity is much more likely to be due to greater transport of glutamate by e.g., Slc1a4 (see Saunders et al., 2012).

The paracellular pathway and tight junctions in barrier interfaces in the developing brain

The concept of the paracellular pathway is an important tenet of epithelial biology that was extended to cerebral endothelial interfaces. It arose from some ingenious experiments of Frömter and Diamond (1972). These authors measured transepithelial resistance in a variety of epithelia in vitro by passing a microelectrode over the external surface of the epithelium. They observed that when the electrode tip was over the region of the border between adjacent cells there was a marked drop in resistance across the epithelium. Frömter and Diamond proposed that this indicated a low resistance pathway across the epithelium for water and ion flow. They subsequently extended this concept to include the paracellular pathway as the route by which small lipid insoluble molecules such as sucrose crossed epithelia (Diamond, 1974). The physical basis for the paracellular pathway is the junctional complex between adjacent cells and in particular the tight junction component of the complex (Brightman and Reese, 1969). However, there are some important limitations to this interpretation of the sites of low resistance across epithelia. Firstly the dimensions of the tip of the microelectrodes used by Frömter and Diamond was about 5 μM compared to the intercellular space at the level of tight junctions, which is measured in nanometers, if not zero where adjacent cell membranes are fused. Thus an alternative explanation for the low resistance pathway is that there is an intracellular pathway close to the border of the cell membrane between adjacent cells.

Diamond (1974) seems to have acknowledged this possibility as he suggested that in some epithelia the low resistance pathway might be due to “leaky” cells, although he thought this would be exceptional. Secondly, water and ions cannot yet be visualized with sufficient resolution for their route across epithelia to be defined. Until recently this was also the case for low molecular weight compounds such as sucrose and inulin; however, this problem has been solved by the use of biotin labeled small molecular sized compounds that can be visualized at both the light and electron microscopical level. At least in the case of choroid plexus epithelial and cerebral endothelial cells in the South American opossum, Monodelphis domestica, the intercellular junctions are impermeable to a compound biotin ethylenediamine that is smaller than sucrose (Ek et al., 2001, 2003, 2006). In addition it was found that biotin labeled markers transfer across a subpopulation of plexus epithelial cells and not between them (Ek et al., 2001, 2003, 2006; Liddelow et al., 2009). These studies require independent replication, preferably in several different species, but they indicate that the earliest vessels growing into the brain are structurally well enough developed for their tight junctions to be impermeable to even very small molecules. These results also suggest that the presence of a low resistance pathway across epithelial cells, rather than the paracellular route, as envisaged by Diamond (1974) as an exceptional situation may indeed exist.

Over many years the results of ultrastructural studies of tight junctions in the cerebral vasculature and choroid plexuses of the developing brain have been conflicting. Two of the earliest ultrastructural studies of the developing mammalian brain barriers were those of Donahue and Pappas, (1961, rat cerebral capillaries) and Tennyson and Pappas (1964, rabbit choroid plexus). Most of their descriptions dealt with subcellular structures, but they do comment on and illustrate what in those days were called terminal bars that were present in the earliest embryos examined (E14). These appear to correspond to structures associated with what were subsequently described as tight junctions (Brightman and Reese, 1969). Particularly considering that the 1960s were early days for electron microscopy of developing brain tissue (which in those days was regarded as very difficult to fix adequately) the micrographs of Donahue and Pappas and Tennyson and Pappas are of high quality and show clearly well formed intercellular tight junctions. Tennyson (1975) published a more comprehensive paper on choroid plexus development in human and rabbit fetuses. It includes illustrations of what she now refers to as “tight junctions” in the earliest rabbit fetuses studied (E15).

Subsequent to the early paper of Tennyson and Pappas (1964) there have been numerous transmission electron microscope studies of tight junctions in the developing brain, mainly in chick embryos (Delorme et al., 1970; Delorme, 1972; Roy et al., 1974) rodents (Stewart and Hayakawa, 1994) fetal sheep (Møllgård and Saunders, 1975, 1977; Møllgård et al., 1976, 1979) but also some in human fetuses (Møllgård and Saunders, 1975, 1986; Virgintino et al., 2004). Most, but not all, seem to agree that the typical appearance of closely apposed cell membranes of adjacent cerebral endothelial cells appears very early in brain development, although some authors (e.g., Stewart and Wiley, 1981) nonetheless conclude that the blood-brain barrier at these early stages is more permeable than in the adult. This was based on citing papers showing greater entry of both metabolically active and inert molecules into the developing brain and a high concentration of protein in CSF (cf. Sections 6, 8, and 11).

Some authors also used HRP in an attempt to delineate the age at which there was a change from blood-brain barrier permeability to one of impermeability. However, insofar as the methodological information is available it seems likely that in many experiments excessive volumes or concentrations of HRP solution or other markers were injected, thus mechanically disrupting the fragile blood vessels (e.g., Wakai and Hirokawa, 1978, 1981; Risau et al., 1986; Stewart and Hayakawa, 1987; Bertossi et al., 1988; Ribatti et al., 1993; and see Table 1). Also HRP has toxic effects and may give rise to anomalous permeability results, even in adult animals (e.g., Mazariegos et al., 1984). Nevertheless Tauc et al. (1984) were able to show that tight junctions in the choroid plexuses of rat embryos at least as early as E14 were impermeable to HRP. These observations were supported by freeze fracture studies showing a similarly early development of tight junction structure.

Møllgård et al. (1976) carried out a quantitative study of freeze fracture tight junction complexity in fetal sheep embryonic choroid plexus. This showed that there were only minor changes in junctional complexity over a large period of development in contrast to reports in the same species of large developmental changes in apparent permeability from blood to CSF for small molecular markers (Evans et al., 1974; Dziegielewska et al., 1979). It should be noted that the large change in apparent permeability was probably mainly due to an increase in CSF secretion rate (see above), but this does not diminish the significance of the finding that the freeze fracture appearance of choroid plexus tight junctions appeared very early in development.

Dermietzel et al. (1977) published a detailed freeze fracture study of the development of intercellular junctions of the chick embryo choroid plexus from E5, the day on which it begins to differentiate. A junctional network of strands was apparent by E6 and by E9 intercellular zonula occludens were more or less complete.

Attempts to correlate expression of individual tight junction genes with some supposed age when cerebral blood vessels stop being leaky, seem doomed to failure unless experiments are conducted under adequate physiological conditions. The findings that different tight junction genes are expressed at different ages and that the junctions become molecularly more complex with age seem more likely to reflect changes in the mechanical properties of tight junctions in the face of increasing hydrostatic pressure (systemic blood pressure) during embryonic and fetal development (Evans et al., 1974; Dziegielewska et al., 1979). The question of whether or not there is a stage of early vascularization of the brain when the blood vessels are leaky is a functional one that can only be answered by permeability experiments conducted under reasonably physiological conditions. In spite of several attempts (e.g., Risau et al., 1986; Mizee et al., 2013; Ben-Zvi et al., 2014, see also Bauer et al., 2014) there is no convincingly demonstrated link between the expression of one particular gene and the supposed sealing of blood vessels entering the CNS tissue, apart from Nitta et al. (2003) who showed increased permeability to small molecules (<800 Da) in claudin 5-deficient mice; this was attributed to tight junction permeability although not confirmed at the ultrastructural level.

Ionic gradients between CSF and plasma in the developing brain

Some of the most convincing evidence that some critical brain barrier properties develop very early in the embryo comes from studies of ion gradients between CSF and plasma. In the adult the ionic composition of CSF is characteristically different from that of plasma and is also appreciably more stable. This reflects the importance of a homeostatically constant environment that is a prerequisite for normal brain function (Davson, 1967; Davson and Segal, 1996). The gradients are set up by ion pumps in the epithelial cells of the choroid plexuses (Damkier et al., 2013). Similar pumps in the cerebral vascular endothelial cells may also contribute via the extracellular fluid of the brain (Davson and Segal, 1996). An essential structural component of this homeostatic mechanism is the presence of tight junctions between the adjacent cells of the choroid plexus epithelium and cerebrovascular endothelium. Thus in development, if even a single ion gradient is present, it indicates the presence of both a cellular ion pump and functional integrity of the tight junctions, as without the latter a gradient simply cannot be established. In the 1970s–80s there were several studies in a variety of species showing the presence of ion gradients between CSF and plasma (Flexner, 1938; Bito and Myers, 1970; Bradbury et al., 1972; Amtorp and Sørensen, 1974; Sedlácek, 1975; Mitchell et al., 1982; Nattie et al., 1984). More recently gene expression studies have shown that key ion channel and transporter genes are expressed very early embryonic life in the choroid plexuses (Liddelow et al., 2012, 2013).

Concentration and origin of proteins in CSF in developing brain

One of the pieces of evidence sometimes put forward in favor of immaturity or leakiness of the blood-brain barrier in the developing brain is that the CSF in fetal and newborn animals contains a much higher concentration of protein than in the adult (Adinolfi et al., 1976; Adinolfi and Haddad, 1977; Ramey and Birge, 1979; Adinolfi, 1985; Fishman, 1992). Strictly speaking the composition of CSF is much more determined by movements of molecules across the choroid plexuses rather than entry via the blood vessels and brain interstitial fluid, although clinicians frequently (but erroneously) interpret CSF changes as reflecting changes in permeability of the blood-brain barrier. Since the middle of the last century it has been known that the concentration of protein in human newborn CSF (e.g., Spiegel-Adolf et al., 1954; Nasralla et al., 1958) is higher than in the adult (Davson and Segal, 1996) and even more so if the infants are prematurely born (e.g., Otila, 1948; Bauer et al., 1965). Results from these and other studies are summarized in Table 2. The mean values reported for healthy term babies were in the range 63–115 mg/100 ml. For pre-term infant the mean values in different series were generally higher (up to nearly 190 mg/100 ml). Little information about human fetal CSF is known but Adinolfi published important studies on CSF and plasma proteins obtained from aborted human fetuses (Adinolfi et al., 1976; Adinolfi and Haddad, 1977; Adinolfi, 1985). These studies showed that the concentration of total protein and of individual proteins such as a-fetoprotein, transferrin and IgG were much higher than reported for live-born infants; the highest individual total protein value reported was 730 mg/100 ml at 24 weeks gestation (Adinolfi et al., 1976) although it needs to be born in mind that the CSF samples were obtained from aborted fetuses of uncertain physiological state.

Table 2.

Total protein concentration (mg/100 ml) in CSF of full term and pre-term infants estimated during the first few days of life.

Authors Mean (mg/100 ml) S.E.M Range n Condition
FULL TERM
Spiegel-Adolf et al., 1954 103 9.9 14 Normal
Widell, 1958 80.9 6.2 11 Healthy
Nasralla et al., 1958 115 5.8 46–194 34 Normal
Naidoo, 1968 63 1.6 32–240 135 Healthy
Piliero and Lending, 1959 70 6.0 25–90 35 Normal
Watson, 1964 77 5.3 26–180 51 Cerebral anoxia
Heine et al., 1981a 73.0 17.6 53–95 10 No cereb meningitis
Ahmed et al., 1996 80.8 30.8* 17 Prev. healthy
Chadwick et al., 2011 106 94–115*** 54 No meningitis
Srinivasan et al., 2012 78^ 137** 60–100 130 Normal
PRE TERM
Otila, 1948b 100 5.5 50–138 19 Healthy
Nasralla et al., 1958c 167 6.4 81–259 49 Normal
Gyllenswärd and Malmström, 1962d 176 26 57–292 9 Healthy
Bauer et al., 1965e 187 28 30–1600 70 Various
Bartolozzi et al., 1967f 62 4.3 12–144 59 Normal
Cole et al., 1974g 120 10 9 Normal
Sarff et al., 1976 90 20–170 No meningitis
Statz and Felgenhauer, 1983h 139 77* 68–240 10 No pathology
Mhanna et al., 2008i 180^ 124–270 10 Suspect sepsis
Srinivasan et al., 2012j 104^ 203** 79–131 148 Normal
a

0–4 weeks. No data on birth weights or gestational age.

b

Birth weights 920-2150 g, Table 19, p. 91.

c

Birth weights ≤ 4.5 lbs, Table 2, p. 1404.

d

Birth weights ≤ 2000 g, Table 5, p. 60.

e

Birth weights 800–2620 g, Figure 1, p. 1018.

f

Birth weights ≤ 2500 g, Figure 2, p. 299.

g

Birth weights 1080–1710 g, Table 1, p. 724.

h

27–32 weeks, Table 3, p. 158.

i

27 weeks, values less at younger and older ages.

j

28–35 weeks, Tables II, p730 and III, p731.

*

S.D.

**

95th percentile.

***

CIs.

^

Median.

Similar results, however, have been obtained for numerous animal species where it was possible to obtain CSF and plasma samples from a wide range of stages of fetal and neonatal development collected under good physiological conditions. The results from animal studies have been reviewed by Saunders (1977, 1992) and by Dziegielewska and Saunders (1988). One of the earliest studies was by Klosovskii (1963) in cat embryos in which CSF protein concentration was up to 22 times that in the adult. Klosovskii correctly interpreted this as having some nutritive function in the developing brain rather than ascribing it to barrier immaturity. The concentration of total protein and of individual proteins in fetal CSF is much higher than in the newborn and adult, but if samples were obtained early enough in development the level was lower than a peak that occurred in all species probably coinciding with the period of maximum neurogenesis. Although Adinolfi (1985) and others interpreted the high CSF concentration of protein in CSF in the developing brain as evidence of a leaky blood-brain and/or blood-CSF barrier there is now substantial evidence that it is a consequence of specific transport of proteins from blood to CSF via a subpopulation of choroid plexus epithelial cells (Dziegielewska et al., 1980, 1991; Habgood et al., 1992; Knott et al., 1997; Johansson et al., 2006, 2008; Liddelow et al., 2009, 2011a,b). In addition it should be noted that the concentration of protein in any space is a function of its amounts and volume in which they distribute. As has been shown by Johansson et al. (2008) the amount of protein in the adult CSF is actually greater in the fetus due to a combination of expanding of the ventricular system and increased CSF flow.

Induction of tight junctions in brain barrier interfaces

Another widespread misconception is that astrocytes are essential for the induction of tight junctions in cerebral blood vessels in the developing brain. In adult brain end feet of astrocytes encircle cerebral capillaries (Caley and Maxwell, 1970; Xu and Ling, 1994). They are thought to make significant contributions to blood-brain barrier functions (Abbott et al., 2006). The belief that astrocytes may be involved in tight junction formation in the developing brain seems to stem largely from in vitro experiments. In culture the presence of either astrocytes or conditioned medium from cultured astrocytes produced cells with more complex tight junctions (Tao-Cheng et al., 1987) and was essential for the preparation of cerebral endothelial monolayers with high transendothelial resistance (Dehouck et al., 1990; Rubin et al., 1991). Janzer and Raff (1987) are frequently cited as supporting the notion that astrocytes are essential for tight junction formation in vivo. However, Holash et al. (1993) showed their results were an artifact (see Saunders et al., 2012, for further discussion). It seems to have been overlooked that there are no astrocytes present in the developing brain when it is first vascularized (Caley and Maxwell, 1970; Daneman et al., 2010a). As first shown by Stewart and Wiley (1981) using chick-quail chimeras, tight junction formation in cerebral vessels is induced by some factor in the neural tissue of the developing brain. The vessels are tight to proteins and small molecules from as early as vessels first grow into the neural tissue (Bauer et al., 1993, 1995; Ek et al., 2006). It is not yet clear what the induction factor(s) is/are. However it seems that the pericytes make an important contribution to tight junction formation (Daneman et al., 2010b). The main period of differentiation of astrocytes and their association with capillaries occur in rodents in the first 3 weeks of postnatal life (Caley and Maxwell, 1970). It may be that astrocytes contribute to tight junction-induction during this period of intense vascularization, and subsequent maintenance of tight junctions, but they are not involved in earlier stages of vascularization of the brain.

Why is it important to understand functioning of barrier mechanisms in the developing brain?

It seems unsatisfactory that this belief in the “immaturity” or leakiness of the blood-brain barrier has persisted into the 21st Century in spite of much evidence to the contrary as reviewed above. There are two quite different reasons why it is important to understand the functional status of barrier mechanisms in the developing brain. The first is that they are likely to be important for different features of brain development at different stages in its development. This is suggested for example, by changes in gene expression for different efflux and influx mechanisms in both the cerebral vasculature and choroid plexuses during brain development (Virgintino et al., 2000, 2007; Daneman et al., 2010a; Liddelow et al., 2012; Kratzer et al., 2013).

The other reason is that it is clinically important. At present because of a lack of information on the extent to which drugs enter the developing brain of the embryo following administration to the mother, the general advice given by doctors to pregnant women is to avoid taking drugs. However, this is not very helpful advice in the case of patients with severe medical conditions requiring continued treatment such as epilepsy (Forsberg and Wide, 2011) depression and anxiety (El Marroun et al., 2014; Ornoy and Koren, 2014) psychosis (Galbally et al., 2014) cancer (van Hasselt et al., 2014), or severe hypertension (Brown and Garovic, 2014).

In the US it has been estimated that between 30 and 35% of women have taken psychoactive drugs during pregnancy (Goldberg and Nissim, 1994). In a study of nearly 100,000 pregnancies, Andrade et al. (2004) found that 64% of pregnant women received a drug prescription. In a four nation study in Europe (De Vigan et al., 1999) an average of 36% of women interviewed used at least one drug during the first trimester of pregnancy. There was substantial national variation (22.5% in Glasgow and 50 and 40% in two centers in France). Because drugs are not systematically investigated for possible deleterious effects on the developing brain the clinical management of pregnant women may be a difficult decision between what is best for the mother and limiting potential damage to the developing fetal brain. This lack of knowledge not only makes it difficult for doctors to make rational decisions about when to and when not to medicate, but commonly results in non-compliance to prescribed medication, terminating medication or self-medication, as well as alternative natural remedies being used by mothers (Wood et al., 2014). The only information available to clinicians at present consists of collections of reports of adverse effects or lack of such effects in compendia such as Yaffe's index (Briggs et al., 2008) or national data organizations such as the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) or the Australian Drug Evaluation Committee in pregnancy, which classify drugs into categories A/B/C/D/X, where A are drugs shown to be safe for the human fetus and X known to induce birth defects (see Teratology Society Public Affairs Committee, 1994). The majority of drugs fall into categories B and C, drugs for which there are not enough data in humans to know whether they are safe for the fetus. The consequences of the currently inadequate state of knowledge about the safety of medications administered during pregnancy have been highlighted by Kennedy (2011). This classification has been under sustained criticism from the Teratology Society in the US (Teratology Society Public Affairs Committee, 1994, 2007) primarily on the grounds that the lack of knowledge about drugs used in pregnancy generates difficulties in making clinical decisions and causing considerable anxiety in patients. This classification is now being abandoned in the US, in favor of a new pregnancy and lactation-labeling rule designed to improve risk versus benefit assessment of drugs used in pregnant and nursing mothers (Ramoz and Patel-Shori, 2014).

Drug use during pregnancy is likely to increase in the future due to higher average ages of mothers, increasing the likelihood of pregnancy complications as well as a higher incidence of premature births with significant medical interventions as a consequence. There is also a growing apprehension that environmental toxins may affect the developing brain. One review listed over 200 industrial compounds known to be neurotoxic in humans and experimental studies have shown over 1000 substances (Grandjean and Landrigan, 2006); this list probably only represents a very small fraction of all chemicals that are neurotoxic. It is particularly inappropriate that public bodies such as the Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease registry (ATSDR) at the Centre for Disease Control in the US should perpetuate statements about immaturity of the blood-brain barrier based on out of date information and mythology. This has been discussed in detail by Ek et al. (2012) but this discussion appears to have had no impact on current reports published by the ATSDR. A more hopeful sign is that the CDC has recently convened meetings to discuss developing a more systematic approach to safer medication use in pregnancy (Broussard et al., 2014).

Recent gene expression studies in developing animal brains (Daneman et al., 2010a; Liddelow et al., 2012, submitted; Kratzer et al., 2013) suggest that many of the protective efflux mechanism that prevent entry of drugs and toxins into the adult brain are present and in some cases more highly expressed in blood vessels and choroid plexuses of the developing brain. However, expression does not necessarily equate with functional effectiveness, so there is an urgent need for systematic drug studies in pregnant and newborn animals and people, in order to define clearly the potential hazards or lack of hazards, when drugs are administered to pregnant women.

Summary

In conclusion, it seems that many of the most strongly held beliefs about the early history of the blood-brain barrier are incorrect. The blood-brain barrier was not described first by Ehrlich (1885); indeed he did not believe that there were any differences in permeability of cerebral blood vessels compared to the rest of the body. Lewandowsky (1900) did not coin the term blood-brain barrier (“Blut-Hirnschranke”). The first person to use this term seems to have been Stern (Stern and Gautier, 1921). Goldmann (1909) was not the first person to demonstrate that some dyes (in his experiments trypan blue) do not enter the brain following parenteral injection. This appears to have been first reported by Franke (1905) who used trypan red. Behnsen (1927) although frequently cited as providing evidence for immaturity of the blood-brain barrier did not do so. Other claims of barrier immaturity or leakiness in developing animals in dye injection experiments can be explained by use of excessive volumes or concentrations of material injected into fragile animals. Early induction of tight junctions in cerebral vessels does not involve astrocytes. At least in the choroid plexus the route of transfer from blood to CSF is intracellular and not via the paracellular pathway.

The importance of discarding the notion of blood-brain barrier immaturity or leakiness in the developing brain lies in the fact that this belief has impeded research into these mechanisms in brain development and their possible relevance for normal and abnormal brain development, as well as the need for systematic studies of therapeutic agents used to treat pregnant women. Therapeutic advice to such patients should be based on evidence, not fear of the unknown.

Author contributions

All of the listed authors contributed to the conception, design, research, drafting and final approval of the work. They each agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Glossary

Abbreviations

CNS

central nervous system

CSF

cerebrospinal fluid

HRP

horseradish peroxidase.

Biographies

George Bernays Wislocki (1892–1956)

graphic file with name fnins-08-00404-g0006.gif

1892 Born March 5th in San Jose California

1912 Graduated from Washington University

1916 Graduated from The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine

1916–1917 Postdoc Assistant in Anatomy under Lewis H Weed

1916–1923 Served as First Lieutenant in U.S. Army Medical Corps during WW1

Arthur Tracy Cabot Fellow at the Laboratory of Surgical Research at Harvard University

Associate in Anatomy at Johns Hopkins

1923 Associate Professor of Anatomy at Harvard University

1931–1941 Parkman Professor of Anatomy at Harvard University

1931 Married Florence Clothier

1939–1940 Member scientific advisory board, Wistar Institute of Anatomy and Biology

1941 James Stillman Professor of Comparative Anatomy at Harvard University

1941 Elected to National Academy of Sciences

1941 Honorary Master of Arts degree, Harvard University

1947 Hersey Professor of Anatomy, Harvard Medical School

1950–1951 First President of the Histochemical Society

1951 Honorary Doctor of Science, Washington University

1956 Died October 22nd in Milton, US (aged 64). Survived by his wife Florence, 2 sons (George and Lewis) and 2 daughters (Edith and Johanna)

Scientific contributions: Wislocki is considered one of the pioneers in the use of histochemistry in anatomical studies. He greatly contributed to the description of the anatomy and function of the endocrine system as well as the comparative anatomy of the placental and brain-barriers. He was the first to publish an image of a dye experiment showing that dye (trypan blue) did not enter the brain of a guinea pig fetus (Figure 1A) except in regions known to have a barrier in the adult brain.

National Academy of Sciences (http://www.nasonline.org/member-directory/deceased-members/20001486.html)

J. Histochem Cytochem. 1957 Jan;5(2):96. “GEORGE BERNAYS WISLOCKI, 1892–1956”

The Harvard Crimson http://www.thecrimson.com/article/1956/10/23/wislocki-dies-here-was-noted-scientist/

Obituary JAMA. 1956;162(18):1642-1644. doi: 10.1001/jama.1956.02970350058020

Paul Ehrlich (1854–1915)

graphic file with name fnins-08-00404-g0007.gif

1854 Born 14th March in Strehlen in Upper Silesia (then Prussia). Educated at the Gymnasium at Breslau and subsequently at the Universities of Breslau, Strassburg, Freiburg-im-Breisgau and Leipzig

1876–1877 State examination as a medical doctor

1878 Obtained doctorate at University of Leipzig

Dissertation on theory and practice of staining animal tissues, in particular as a means of studying oxygenation of the brain

1878–1887 Physician at Berlin Medical Clinic (Charité)

1882 Ehrlich published method of staining tubercle bacillus. Gram method of staining bacteria derived from this

1882 Appointed Titular Professor

1887 Faculty of Medicine, University of Berlin, qualified as Privatdozen/Lecturer based on Habilitation thesis “Sauerstoffbedurfnis des Organismus.” Later appointed Associate Professor and Senior House Physician to Charite Hospital in Berlin

1887–1890 Travels in Egypt and establishment of a private laboratory

1891–1895 Worked with Robert Koch at the Institute of Infectious Diseases in Berlin

1896 Director of the specially established Institute for Serum Research and Testing

1899 Moved to Frankfurt am Main (director of the Institute of Experimental Therapy)

1908 Nobel prize in Physiology or Medicine 1908 (together with Ilya Mechnikov) “in recognition of their work on the theory of immunity”

1914 Professor of Pharmacology at Frankfurt University

1915 Died from a Stroke (Bad Homburg, Germany)

Scientific contributions: Ehrlich made an impressive number of contributions to several research fields including hematology, immunology, chemotherapy and cancer research: the discovery of mast cells (in his Ph.D. thesis), dye experiments that were interpreted as some as early evidence for a mechanism preventing some molecules from entering the brain, although he himself did not accept this explanation (see text), major contributions to the diagnosis of blood diseases, discoveries about immunity to plant poisons and inheritance of acquired immunity via breast-milk, development of quantitative standardization methods for antiserum, the formulation of the Side-chain theory and the discovery of Salvarsan (first pathogen-targeted drug) for treatment of syphilis, the first successful chemotherapy.

See also Dale (1954) and Hüntelmann (2010)

The official website of the Nobel prize (http://www.nobelprize.org)

The Paul-Ehrlich-Institut (http://www.pei.de/EN/home/node.html)

Max Lewandowsky (1876–1918)

graphic file with name fnins-08-00404-g0008.gif

1876 Born 26 June in Berlin, Germany

1893 Awarded his Matriculation Certificate from the Friedrichs Gymnasium school in Berlin

1893 Began his studies at the University of Marburg under Hermann Cohen

1898 State examination as a medical doctor from Halle-Wittenberg University

1902 Habilitation at University of Berlin

1902–1904 Privat Docent in Berlin

1904 Studied Clinical Psychiatry with Nissl and Bonhofer in Heidelberg

1904–1905 Studied psychiatry with Pierre Mari at the Bicętre Clinic in Paris

1905 Physician consultant at the City Hospital in Berlin-Friedrichshain

1907 Publication of Die Funktionen des zentralen Nervensystems

1908 Appointed full Professor

1910 Together with Alzheimer, founded the journal “Zeitschrift fur die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie” and a series “Monographien aus dem Gesamtgebiete der Neurologie und Psychiatrie”

1910–1914 Publication of textbook “Handbuch der Neurologie“ (5 volumes) of neurology Written by Lewandowsky himself (parts of each chapter) together with 40 contemporary European neurologists

1912 Publication of Praktische Neurologie für Ärzte. Later edition in 1919

1914–1917 Served at war hospitals during this period

1918 Died on 4th April in a Berlin sanatorium, probably committed suicide

Scientific contributions: His scientific inheritance contains definitive studies of neurophysiology of breathing, CSF, neuroanatomy of breathing centers, function of the sympathetic nervous system, cerebellum, discalculia and war psychosis. He was active in the Berlin neurological centers. He is frequently credited with having coined the term Blut-Hirnschranke” (blood-brain barrier) but as discussed in this review there is no evidence for this. He was also the author of the textbook Praktische Neurologie für Ärzte.

Lina Stern (1878–1968)

graphic file with name fnins-08-00404-g0009.gif

Part I. Geneva, Switzerland (1898–1925)

1878 Born August 26th in Latvia

1898–1904 Student in the Medical Faculty at the University of Geneva. Presented M.D. thesis in the field of physiology in 1904

1904–1914 Assistant, then first assistant in physiology. Research on biological oxidation, results published in German Journal of Biochemistry

1917 First studies on a barrier that slows down the penetration into the brain of substances dissolved in blood plasma

1918 First ever woman promoted to Associate Professor (in Physiological Chemistry) at the Department of Physiology

1921–1923 Coins the term barrier hemato-encephalic. This was probably the first use of the term “blood-brain barrier,” although she mainly studied what was later called the blood-cerebrospinal fluid-barrier

1925 On receipt of an invitation to move her laboratory to Moscow, an attempt by the local government to promote her to full professor fails because of her leftish pro-revolutionary views and the financial crisis in Geneva

Part II. Rise, fall and rehabilitation in the USSR (1925–1953)

1925 Professor at Moscow Medical University and affiliation to several medical institutions

1925–1934 Difficult and slow adaptation to life in the Soviet Union and several failed attempts to recover her previous position in Geneva. Numerous reports on the blood-brain barrier (BBB) and on the effects of blood solutes following their parenteral or intra-ventricular injection

1929–1948 Director of the new Physiological Institute built and financed by the People's Commissariats of Education of Health, After 1939 the institute became part of the USSR Academy of Sciences. It was dismantled in 1948 (see below)

1934–1935 To mark 30 years of her scientific and educational work, Stern received an honorary doctorate of Biological Sciences and was awarded the title of “honored worker of science”

1939 First ever women elected full member of the USSR Academy of Sciences

1942 Publication in Nature and in the British Medical Journal of her article on the treatment of shock by direct action upon the vegetative nervous centers Another article printed in the West during WWII deals with the treatment of tetanus. During the war, Stern was a member of the Jewish Anti-Fascist Committee (JAC) organized in order to mobilize Jewish support for the USSR's war effort against Nazi Germany

1943–1945 Stalin Prize. Orders of the Red Banner and of the Red Star. Elected as full member of the USSR Academy of Medical Sciences

1947 Publication in a widely read medical journal of an unfounded attack against research on the blood-brain barrier, apparently an authorized signal from Stalin geopolitical reorientation to start persecution of Jews

1948 Closure of the Institute of Physiology and Stern “unmasked” as an “anti-Pavlovian” in 1949 she was accused of being a member of a Zionist organization, arrested and detained for 3 years and 10 months

1952 Secrete military trial of prominent members of the JAC. Except Stern, all the accused were executed on August 12, 1952. She was sentenced to five years internal exile in a small town in Central Asia

1953 Shortly after the death of Stalin, Stern was set free and allowed to return to Moscow. The same year she was reinstated in the Academy. She died on March 7, 1968

See also Vein (2008); Dreifuss and Sigrist (2009); Wiesendanger (2010)

Edwin Goldmann (1862–1913)

graphic file with name fnins-08-00404-g0010.gif

1862 Born 11th November in Burghersdorp, South Africa, Moved to Breslau (Wroclaw) in Europe with his parents, sister and brothers at age 14. Studied in Breslau, Freiburg and London

1887 Presented his dissertation “Experimental contributions to the teaching of the cystinuria and sulfur excretion in the urine.” in Freiburg

1888 Received his Medical degree in London, Assistant under Prof. Weigert at the Pathological-Anatomical Institute in Frankfurt-on-Main

1888 Assistant at the University Surgical Hospital in Freiburg

1889 Practicing surgeon in Freiburg and Basel

1891 Received his Habilitation and qualified as a privat-docent

1895 Appointed Extraordinary Professor and became surgeon to the Deaconess's Hospital in Freiburg

1898–1913 Chief Physician at the hospital of the Deaconess's in Freiburg in 1898

1906 On 27th February married Lorna Lawrence Bosworth Smith (daughter of a British peer) at Bingham's Melcombe in Dorset. Associate under Paul Ehrlich at the Institute for Experimental Therapeutics, Frankfurt

1913 Ehrlich canvassed support from William Welch to establish an Institute for Vital Staining that he wished Goldmann to head. Abandoned following Goldmann's death and outbreak of 1st World War

1913 Died from cancer in Freiburg

Scientific contributions: During his time in Freiburg, in addition to his role as a physician, Goldmann worked in the field of “physiological chemistry” investigating sulfur-containing compounds in urine and later at Evangelical Sisters in the field of cancer research. He significantly contributed to the blood-brain barrier field by his often-cited experiments showing the azo-dye trypan blue did not stain the brain when injected parenterally (Goldmann's “First experiment”), but did when injected intrathecally (Goldmann's “Second experiment”).

Singer und F. T. Haneman: Goldmann, Edwin Ellen In: Jewish Encyclopedia

http://de.wikipedia.org/wiki/Edwin_Goldmann

Footnotes

1Where possible we have worked from original references. Where these were not in English we have given citations of key points in the original language, together with a translation (indicated by italics). Readers will be able to decide for themselves if the translations are accurate and if our deductions from the citations are appropriate.

References

  1. Abbott N. J., Rönnbäck L., Hansson E. (2006). Astrocyte-endothelial interactions at the blood-brain barrier. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 41–53. 10.1038/nrn1824 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adinolfi M. (1985). The development of the human blood-brain barrier. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 27, 532–537. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1985.tb04581.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adinolfi M., Beck S. E., Haddad S. A., Seller M. J. (1976). Permeability of the blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier to plasma proteins during foetal and perinatal life. Nature 259, 140–141. 10.1038/259140a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adinolfi M., Haddad S. A. (1977). Levels of plasma proteins in human and rat fetal CSF and the development of the blood-CSF barrier. Neuropädiatrie 8, 345–353. 10.1055/s-0028-1091530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ahlfors C. E., Wennberg R. P. (2004). Bilirubin-albumin binding and neonatal jaundice. Semin. Perinatol. 28, 334–339. 10.1053/j.semperi.2004.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ahmed A., Hickey S. M., Ehrett S., Trujillo M., Brito F., Goto C., et al. (1996). Cerebrospinal fluid values in the term neonate. Pediatr. Infect. Dis. J. 15, 298–303. 10.1097/00006454-199604000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Allen F. H., Jr., Diamond L. K., Vaughan V. C. (1950). Erythroblastosis fetalis. VI. Prevention of kernicterus. Am. J. Dis. Child. 80, 779–791. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Al-Sarraf H. (2002). Transport of 14C-gamma-aminobutyric acid into brain, cerebrospinal fluid and choroid plexus in neonatal and adult rats. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 139, 121–129. 10.1016/S0165-3806(02)00537-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Al-Sarraf H., Preston J. E., Segal M. B. (1997). Changes in the kinetics of the acidic amino acid brain and CSF uptake during development in the rat. Dev. Brain Res. 102, 127–134. 10.1016/S0165-3806(97)00089-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Amtorp O., Sørensen S. C. (1974). The ontogenetic development of concentration differences for protein and ions between plasma and cerebrospinal fluid in rabbits and rats. J. Physiol. 243, 387–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Andrade S. E., Gurwitz J. H., Davis R. L., Chan K. A., Finkelstein J. A., Fortman K., et al. (2004). Prescription drug use in pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 191, 398–407. 10.1016/j.ajog.2004.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. ATSDR. (2014). Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry Home Page. Available online at: http://www.atsdr.cdc.gov (retrieved August 29 2014).
  13. ATSDR. Tp46. (1999). Available online at: http://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/ToxProfiles/tp46.pdf (retrieved August 29 2014).
  14. ATSDR. Tp13. (2007). Available online at: http://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/ToxProfiles/tp13.pdf (retrieved August 29 2014).
  15. Bakay L. (1953). Studies on blood-brain barrier with radioactive phosphorus. III. Embryonic development of the barrier. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 70, 30–39. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1953.02320310036002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bakay L. (1956). “Development of the blood-brain barrier, in The Blood-Brain Barrier, with Special Regard to the use of Radioactive Isotopes, ed Bakay L. (Springfield, IL: Thomas; ), 77–149. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bakay L. (1968). Changes in barrier effect in pathological states. Prog. Brain Res. 29, 315–339. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)64165-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Baldrick P. (2010). Juvenile animal testing in drug development – Is it useful? Regul. Toxicol. Pharmacol. 57, 291–299. 10.1016/j.yrtph.2010.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Baños G., Daniel P. M., Pratt O. E. (1978). The effect of age upon the entry of some amino acids into the brain, and their incorporation into cerebral protein. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 20, 335–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Barcroft J. (1938). The Brain and its Environment 1. The Activity of the Brain in Mid-Foetal Life. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Barrett K. E., Barman S. M., Boitano S., Brooks H. L. (2012). Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology. 24th Edn. New York, NY: McGraw Hill Lange. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bartolozzi G., Ciampolini M., Franchini F., Marianelli L. (1967). Studio delle protein liquorate nel prematuro. I Richerche lettroforetiche. Riv. Clin. Pediatr. 80, 296–309. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bass N. H., Lundborg P. (1973). Postnatal development of bulk flow in the cerebrospinal fluid system of the albino rat: clearance of carboxyl-(14C)inulin after intrathecal infusion. Brain Res. 52, 323–332. 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90668-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Bauer C. H., New M. I., Miller J. (1965). Cerebrospinal fluid protein values of premature infants. J. Pediatr. 66, 1017–1022. 10.1016/S0022-3476(65)80086-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bauer H., Sonnleitner U., Lametschwandtner A., Steiner M., Adam H., Bauer H. C. (1995). Ontogenic expression of the erythroid-type glucose transporter (Glut 1) in the telencephalon of the mouse: correlation to the tightening of the blood-brain barrier. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 86, 317–325. 10.1016/0165-3806(95)00044-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bauer H. C., Bauer H., Lametschwandtner A., Amberger A., Ruiz P., Steiner M. (1993). Neovascularization and the appearance of morphological characteristics of the blood–brain barrier in the embryonic mouse central nervous system. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 75, 269–278. 10.1016/0165-3806(93)90031-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Bauer H.-C., Kritzbai I. A., Bauer H., Traweger A. (2014). “You Shall Not Pass” – Tight junctions of the Blood Brain Barrier. Front. Neurosci. 8:392 10.3389/fnins.2014.00392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Behnsen G. (1926). Farbstoffversuche mit Trypanblau an der Schranke zwischen Blut und Zentralnervensystem der wachsenden Maus. Münch. Med. Wochenschrift 73, 1143–1147. [Google Scholar]
  29. Behnsen G. (1927). Über die Farbstoffspeicherung im Zentralnervensystem der weissen Maus in verschiedenen Alterszuständen. Z. Zellforsch. Mikrosk. Anat. 4, 515–572 10.1007/BF00637105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ben-Zvi A., Lacoste B., Kur E., Andreone B. J., Mayshar Y., Yan H., et al. (2014). Mfsd2a is critical for the formation and function of the blood–brain barrier. Nature 509, 573–590. 10.1038/nature13324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Bernard C. (1865). Introduction a l'étude de la Médecine Expérimentale. Paris: Balliere. [Google Scholar]
  32. Bertossi M., Mancini L., Favia A., Nico B., Ribatti D., Virgintino D., et al. (1992). Permeability-related structures in developing and mature microvessels of the chicken optic tectum. Biol. Struct. Morphog. 4, 144–152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Bertossi M., Ribatti D., Nico B., Mancini L., Lozupone E., Roncali L. (1988). The barrier systems in the choroidal plexuses of the chick embryo studied by means of horseradish peroxidase. J. Submicrosc. Cytol. Pathol. 20, 385–395. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Biedl A., Kraus R. (1898). Über eine bisher unbekannte toxische Wirkung der Gallensäuren auf das Centralnervensystem. Centralblatt. Inn. Med. 19, 1185–1200. [Google Scholar]
  35. Birge W. J., Rose A. D., Haywood J. R., Doolin P. F. (1974). Development of the blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier to proteins and differentiation of cerebrospinal fluid in the chick embryo. Dev. Biol. 41, 245–254. 10.1016/0012-1606(74)90303-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Bito L. Z., Myers R. E. (1970). The ontogenesis of haematoencephalic cation transport in the rhesus monkey. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 208, 153–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Bouffard G. (1906). Injection des couleurs de benzidine aux animaux normaux. Ann. d. l'lnst. Pasteur. Paris 20, 539–548. [Google Scholar]
  38. Bradbury M. W. B. (1979). The Concept of a Blood-Brain Barrier. Chichester: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  39. Bradbury M. W. B. (1995). “Developing views of the blood-brain barrier, in New Concepts of a Blood-Brain Barrier, eds Greenwood J., Begley D. J., Segal M. B. (New York, NY: Plenum; ), 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  40. Bradbury M. W. B., Crowder J., Desai S., Reynolds J. M., Reynolds M., Saunders N. R. (1972). Electrolytes and water in the brain and cerebrospinal fluid of the foetal sheep and guinea pig. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 227, 591–610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Braun L. D., Cornford E. M., Oldendorf W. H. (1980). Newborn rabbit blood–brain barrier is selectively permeable and differs substantially from the adult. J. Neurochem. 34, 147–152. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1980.tb04633.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Briggs G. G., Freeman R. K., Yaffe S. J. (2008). Drugs in Pregnancy and Lactation. Philadelphia, PA: Walters Kluwer. [Google Scholar]
  43. Brightman M. W., Reese T. S. (1969). Junctions between intimately apposed cell membranes in the vertebrate brain. J. Cell. Biol. 40, 48–77. 10.1083/jcb.40.3.648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Broman T. (1941). The possibilities of the passage of substances from the blood to the central nervous system (Is there a blood-brain-barrier and a blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier?). Acta Psychol. Scand. 16, 1–25 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1941.tb07398.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Broussard C. S., Frey M. T., Hernandez-Diaz S., Greene M. F., Chambers C. D., Sahin L., et al. (2014). Developing a systematic approach to safer medication use during pregnancy: summary of a centers for disease control and prevention-convened meeting. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 211, 208–214. 10.1016/j.ajog.2014.05.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Brown C. M., Garovic V. D. (2014). Drug treatment of hypertension in pregnancy. Drugs 74, 283–229 10.1007/s40265-014-0187-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Caley D. W., Maxwell D. S. (1970). Development of the blood vessels and extracellular spaces during postnatal maturation of rat cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 138, 31–47. 10.1002/cne.901380104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Chadwick S. L., Wilson J. W., Levin J. E., Martin J. M. (2011). Cerebrospinal fluid characteristics in infants who present to the Emergency Department with fever. Pediatr. Infect. Dis. J. 30, e63–e67. 10.1097/INF.0b013e31820ad2ba [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Clarke C. A. (1989). Preventing rhesus babies: the Liverpool research and follow up. Arch. Dis. Child. 64, 1734–1749. 10.1136/adc.64.12.1734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Cohen H., Davies S. (1938). The Morphology and permeability of the roof of the fourth ventricle in some mammalian embryos. J. Anat. 72, 430–455. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Cole V. A., Durbin G., Olaffson A., Reynolds E. O. R., Rivers R., Smith J. (1974). Pathogenesis of intraventricular haemorrhage in newborn infants. Arch. Dis. Child. 49, 722–728. 10.1136/adc.49.9.722 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Cornford E. M., Braun L. D., Oldendorf W. H. (1982). Developmental modulations of blood-brain barrier permeability as an indicator of changing nutritional requirements in the brain. Pediatr. Res. 16, 324–328. 10.1203/00006450-198204000-00017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Costa L. G., Aschner M., Vitalone A., Syversen T., Soldin O. P. (2004). Developmental neuropathology of environmental agents. Ann. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 44, 87–110. 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.44.101802.121424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Cotran R. S., Kumar V., Robbins S. L., Schoen F. J. (1994). Robbins Pathologic Basis of Disease, 5th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders. [Google Scholar]
  55. Dale H. (1954). Paul Ehrlich. Br. Med. J. 1, 659–663. 10.1136/bmj.1.4863.659 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Damkier H. H., Brown P. D., Praetorius J. (2013). Cerebrospinal fluid secretion by the choroid plexus. Physiol. Rev. 93, 1847–1892. 10.1152/physrev.00004.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Daneman R., Zhou L., Agalliu D., Cahoy J. D., Kaushal A., Barres B. A. (2010a). The mouse blood-brain barrier transcriptome: a new resource for understanding the development and function of brain endothelial cells. PLoS ONE 5:e13741. 10.1371/journal.pone.0013741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Daneman R., Zhou L., Kebede A. A., Barres B. A. (2010b). Pericytes are required for blood-brain barrier integrity during embryogenesis. Nature 468, 562–566. 10.1038/nature09513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Davies P. A., Rudd P. T. (1994). Neonatal Meningitis. Cambridge: MacKeith Press. [Google Scholar]
  60. Davis J. E., Garrison N. E. (1968). Mean weights of chick embryos correlated with the stages of Hamburger and Hamilton. J. Morph. 124, 79–82. 10.1002/jmor.1051240105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Davson H. (1956). Physiology of the Ocular and Cerebrospinal Fluids. London: Churchill. [Google Scholar]
  62. Davson H. (1967). Physiology of the Cerebrospinal Fluid. London: Churchill. [Google Scholar]
  63. Davson H. (1976). Review lecture: the blood-brain barrier. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 255, 1–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Davson H., Welch K., Segal M. B. (1987). Physiology and Pathophysiology of the Cerebrospinal Fluid. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. [Google Scholar]
  65. Davson H., Segal M. B. (1996). Physiology of the CSF and Blood-Brain Barriers. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  66. Dehouck M.-P., Meìresse S., Delorme P., Fruchart J.-C., Cecchelli R. (1990). An easier, reproducible, and mass-production method to study the blood–brain barrier in vitro. J. Neurochem. 54, 1798–1801. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1990.tb01236.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Delorme P. (1972). Differenciation ultrastructurale des jonctions intercellulaires de l'endothelium des capillaires télencéphaliques chez l'embryon de poulet. Z. Zellforsch. Mikrosk. Anat. 133, 571–582. 10.1007/BF00307137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Delorme P., Gayet J., Grignon G. (1970). Ultrastructural study on transcapillary exchanges in the developing telencephalon of the chicken. Brain Res. 22, 269–283. 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90471-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Dempsey E. W., Wislocki G. B. (1955). An electron microscopic study of the blood-brain barrier in the rat, employing silver nitrate as a vital stain. J. Biophys. Biochem. Cytol. 1, 245–256. 10.1083/jcb.1.3.245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Dermietzel R., Krause D., Kremer M., Wang C., Stevenson B. (1992). Pattern of glucose transporter (Glut 1) expression in embryonic brains is related to maturation of blood-brain barrier tightness. Dev. Dyn. 193, 152–163. 10.1002/aja.1001930207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Dermietzel R., Meller K., Tetzlaff W., Waelsch M. (1977). In vivo and in vitro formation of the junctional complex in choroid epithelium. A freeze-etching study. Cell Tissue Res. 181, 427–441. 10.1007/BF00221766 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. De Vigan C., De Walle H. E. K., Cordier S., Goujard J., Knill-Jines R., Aymé S., et al. (1999). Therapeutic drug use during pregnancy: a comparison in four European countries. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 52, 977–982. 10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00091-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Diamond J. M. (1974). Tight and leaky junctions of epithelia: a perspective on kisses in the dark. Fed. Proc. 33, 2220–2224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Dobbing J. (1961). The blood-brain barrier. Physiol. Rev. 41, 130–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Donahue S., Pappas G. D. (1961). The fine structure of capillaries in the cerebral cortex of the rat at various stages of development. Am. J. Anat. 108, 331–347. 10.1002/aja.1001080307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Dreifuss J.-J., Sigrist N. T. (2009). Lina Stern, l'académicienne genevoise persécutée en URSS. Rev. Med. Suisse 5, 593–597 Available online at: http://rms.medhyg.ch/article.php?ID_ARTICLE=RMS_194_0593 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Dziegielewska K. M., Evans C. A., Lai P. C., Lorscheider F. L., Malinowska D. H., Møllgård K., et al. (1981). Proteins in cerebrospinal fluid and plasma of fetal rats during development. Dev. Biol. 83, 193–200. 10.1016/S0012-1606(81)80024-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Dziegielewska K. M., Evans C. A. N., Malinowska D. H., Møllgård K., Reynolds M. L., Saunders N. R. (1980). Blood-cerebrospinal fluid transfer of plasma proteins during fetal development in the sheep. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 300, 457–465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Dziegielewska K. M., Evans C. A. N., Malinowska D. H., Møllgård K., Reynolds J. M., Reynolds M. L., et al. (1979). Studies of the development of brain barrier systems to lipid insoluble molecules in fetal sheep. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 292, 207–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Dziegielewska K. M., Habgood M. D., Møllgård K., Stagaard M., Saunders N. R. (1991). Species-specific transfer of plasma albumin from blood into different cerebrospinal fluid compartments in the fetal sheep. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 439, 215–237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Dziegielewska K. M., Hinds L. A., Møllgård K., Reynolds M. L., Saunders N. R. (1988). Blood-brain, blood-cerebrospinal fluid and cerebrospinal fluid-brain barriers in a marsupial (Macropus eugenii) during development. J. Physiol. (Lond) 403, 367–388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (1988). “The development of the blood-brain barrier: proteins in fetal and neonatal CSF, their nature and origins, in Handbook of Human Growth and Developmental Biology. Vol 1. Neural, Sensory, Motor and Integrative Development, eds Meisami E., Timiras P. S. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ), 169–191. [Google Scholar]
  83. Ehman K. (2010). “Development of the mammalian nervous system, in Reproductive Toxicolgy, 3rd Edn., eds Kapp R. W., Tyl R. W. (New York, NY: Informa; ), 51–67. [Google Scholar]
  84. Ehrlich P. (1885). Das Sauerstoffbedürfnis des Organismus. Eine Farbenanalytische Studie. Berlin: Hirschwald. [Google Scholar]
  85. Ehrlich P. (1906). “The relations existing between chemical constitution, distribution and pharmacological action, in Collected Studies on Immunity New York: John Wiley & Sons, translated by C Bolduana from Ch XXXIV of Gesammelte Arbeiten zur Immunitätsforschung, ed Ehrlich P. (Berlin: Hirschwald; ), 1904. [Google Scholar]
  86. Ek C. J., Dziegielewska K. M., Habgood M. D., Saunders N. R. (2012). Barriers in the developing brain and neurotoxicology. Neurotoxicology 33, 586–604. 10.1016/j.neuro.2011.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ek C. J., Habgood M. D., Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (2003). Structural characteristics and barrier properties of the choroid plexuses in developing brain of the opossum (Monodelphis domestica). J. Comp. Neurol. 460, 451–464. 10.1002/cne.10661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Ek C. J., Habgood M. D., Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (2006). Functional effectiveness of the blood-brain barrier to small water-soluble molecules in developing and adult opossum (Monodelphis domestica). J. Comp. Neurol. 496, 13–26. 10.1002/cne.20885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Ek J., Habgood M. D., Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (2001). Permeability of the blood-brain and blood-CSF barriers to small molecular weight lipid insoluble markers during postnatal development in the opossum, Monodelphis domestica. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 536, 841–853. 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.00841.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. El Marroun H., White T., Verhuist F. C., Tiemeier H. (2014). Maternal use of antidepressant or anxiolytic medication during pregnancy and childhood neurodevelopmental outcomes: a systematic review. Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 23, 973–992. 10.1007/s00787-014-0558-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Engelhardt B. (2003). Development of the blood-brain barrier. Cell Tissue Res. 314, 119–129. 10.1007/s00441-003-0751-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Engelhardt B. (2006). “Development of the blood-brain interface, in Blood-Brain Barriers, Vol. 1, eds Dermietzel R., Spray D. C., Nedergaard M. (Weinheim: Wiley-VCH; ), 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  93. Ernster L., Herlin L., Zetterstrom R. (1957). Experimental studies on the pathogenesis of kernicterus. Pediatrics 20, 647–652. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Evans C. A. N., Reynolds J. M., Reynolds M. L., Saunders N. R., Segal M. B. (1974). The development of a blood-brain barrier mechanism in foetal sheep. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 238, 371–386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Ferguson R. K., Woodbury D. M. (1969). Penetration of 14C-inulin and 14C-sucrose into brain, cerebrospinal fluid, and skeletal muscle of developing rats. Exp. Brain Res. 7, 181–194. 10.1007/BF00239028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Fishman R. A. (1992). Cerebrospinal Fluid in Diseases of the Nervous System, 2nd Edn. Philadelphia, FL: WB Saunders. [Google Scholar]
  97. Flexner L. B. (1938). Changes in the chemistry and nature of the cerebrospinal fluid during fetal life in the pig. Am. J. Physiol. 124, 131–135. [Google Scholar]
  98. Forsberg L., Wide K. (2011). Long-term consequences after exposure to antiepileptic drugs in utero. Ther. Adv. Drug Saf. 2, 227–234. 10.1177/2042098611419003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Fossan G., Cavanagh M. E., Evans C. A. N., Malinowska D. H., Møllgård K., Reynolds M. L., et al. (1985). CSF-brain permeability in the immature sheep fetus: a CSF-brain barrier. Dev. Brain Res. 18, 113–124. 10.1016/0165-3806(85)90255-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Franceschini P. (1929). Sulla presenza di elementi connettivali nel sistema nervoso centrale e sopra alcune particlarità di struttura dele meningi molli die plessi coroidei. A proposito della cosi detta barriera “emato-encefalica”. Sperimentale 83, 419–445. [Google Scholar]
  101. Franke E. (1905). Therapeutische Versuche bei Trypanosomenerkrankung. Dissertation, Giessen. [Google Scholar]
  102. Frömter E., Diamond J. (1972). Route of passive ion permeation in epithelia. Nat. New Biol. 235, 9–13. 10.1038/newbio235009a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Friedemann U. (1942). Blood-brain barrier. Physiol. Rev. 22, 125–145. [Google Scholar]
  104. Friedemann U., Elkeles A. (1931). Über die Permeabilität der Blut-Hirnschranke für Bakteriengifte. I. Mitteilung. 1. Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die Diphtherie-Vergiftung. Z. Exp. Med. 74, 293–319. [Google Scholar]
  105. Friedemann U., Elkeles A. (1934). The blood-brain barrier in infectious diseases: its permeability to toxins in relation to their electrical charges. Lancet 7, 719–724 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)92558-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Galbally M., Snellen M., Power J. (2014). Antipsychotic drugs in pregnancy: a review of their maternal and fetal effects. Ther. Adv. Drug Saf. 5, 100–107. 10.1177/2042098614522682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Ganong W. B. (1993). Review of Medical Physiology. 16th Edn. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton & Lange. [Google Scholar]
  108. Ganong W. B. (1999). Review of Medical Physiology. 19th Edn. Stamford, CT: Appleton & Lange. [Google Scholar]
  109. Gerrard J. (1952). Kernicterus. Brain 75, 526–570. 10.1093/brain/75.4.526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Ghersi-Egea J.-F., Gazzin S., Strazielle N. (2009). Blood-brain interfaces and bilirubin-induced neurological diseases. Curr. Pharm. Des. 15, 2893–2907. 10.2174/138161209789058147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Goldberg H. L., Nissim R. (1994). Psychotropic drugs in pregnancy and lactation. Int. J. Psychiatr. Med. 24, 129–147. 10.2190/2BF1-0718-WE7F-A9F7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Goldmann E. E. (1909). Die äussere und innere Sekretion des gesunden und kranken Organismus im Lichte der ‘vitalen Färbung.’ Beiträg Klinische Chirurgie 64, 192–265. [Google Scholar]
  113. Goldmann E. E. (1913). Vitalfärbung am Zentralnervensyatem. Beitrag zur Physio-Pathologie des plexus chorioideus und der Hirnhäute. Abh preuss, Akad Wiss Phys-Math Kl 1, 1–60. [Google Scholar]
  114. Grandjean P., Landrigan P. J. (2006). Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals. Lancet 368, 2167–2178. 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69665-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Grazer F. M., Clemente C. D. (1957). Developing blood-brain barrier to trypan blue. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 94, 758–760. 10.3181/00379727-94-23077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Gröntoft O. (1954). Intracranial haemorrhage and blood-brain barrier problems in the newborn. Acta Pathol. Microbiol. Scand. Suppl. 100, 1–109. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Gyllenswärd Å., Malmström S. (1962). The cerebrospinal fluid of newborn infants. Acta Paediatr. 135, 54–62 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1962.tb08658.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Habgood M. D., Knott G. W., Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (1993). The nature of the blood-CSF barrier permeability decrease during postnatal brain development in the rat. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 468, 73–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Habgood M. D., Sedgwick J. E. C., Dziegielewska K. M., Saunders N. R. (1992). A developmentally regulated blood-cerebrospinal fluid transfer mechanism for albumin in immature rats. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 456, 181–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Hawkins B. T., Davis T. P. (2005). The Blood-brain barrier/neurovascular unit in health and disease. Pharmacol. Rev. 57, 173–185. 10.1124/pr.57.2.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Heine W., Hobusch D., Drescher U. (1981). Eiweissgehalt des Liquors und Blut-Liquor-relation der Glucose und Electrolyte im Säuglings-und Kindersalter. Helv. Paediatr. Acta 36, 217–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Himwich W. A., Petersen J. C., Allen M. L. (1957). Hematoencephalic exchange as a function of age. Neurology 7, 705–710. 10.1212/WNL.7.10.705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Holash J. A., Noden D. M., Stewart P. A. (1993). Re-evaluating the role of astrocytes in blood-brain barrier induction. Dev. Dyn. 197, 14–25. 10.1002/aja.1001970103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Hulsebosch C. E., Fabian R. H. (1989). Penetration of IgGs into the neuraxis of the neonatal rat. Neurosci. Lett. 98, 13–18. 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90365-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Hüntelmann A. C. (2010). Legend of science. External constructions by the extended family - the biography of Paul Ehrlich. Interdisciplines 2, 13–36 10.2390/indi-v1-i2-17 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Iskander I., Gamaleldin R., El Houchi S., El Shenawy A., Seoud I., El Gharbawi N., et al. (2014). Serum bilirubin and bilirubin/albumin ratio as predictors of bilirubin encephalopathy. Pediatrics 134, e1330–e1339. 10.1542/peds.2013-1764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Janzer R. C., Raff M. C. (1987). Astrocytes induce blood-brain barrier properties in endothelial cells. Nature 325, 253–257. 10.1038/325253a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Järup L. (2003). Hazards of heavy metal contamination. Br. Med. Bull. 68, 167–182. 10.1093/bmb/ldg032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Johanson C. E. (1989). “Ontogeny and Phylogeny of the blood-brain barrier, in Implications of the Blood Brain Barrier and its Manipulation, Vol. 1. Basic aspects, ed Neuwelt E. A. (New York, NY: Plenum; ), 157–198. [Google Scholar]
  130. Johanson C. E., Woodbury D. M. (1974). “Changes in CSF flow and extracellular space in the developing rat, in Drugs and the Developing Brain, eds Vernadakis A., Weiner N. (New York, NY: Plenum; ), 281–287. [Google Scholar]
  131. Johansson P. A., Dziegielewska K. M., Ek C. J., Habgood M. D., Liddelow S. A., Potter A. M., et al. (2006). Blood–CSF barrier function in the rat embryo. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 65–76. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04904.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Johansson P. A., Dziegielewska K. M., Liddelow S. A., Saunders N. R. (2008). The blood-CSF barrier explained: when development is not immaturity. Bioessays 30, 237–248. 10.1002/bies.20718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Joó F. (1987). Current aspects of the development of the blood-brain barrier. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 5, 369–372. 10.1016/0736-5748(87)90013-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Kennedy D. S. (2011). A to X: the problem of categorisation of drugs in pregnancy—an Australian perspective. Med. J. Aust. 195, 572–574. 10.5694/mja11.11119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. King L. S. (1939). The hematoencephalic barrier. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatr. 41, 51–72 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1939.02270130061003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  136. Knott G. W., Dziegielewska K. M., Habgood M. D., Li Z. S., Saunders N. R. (1997). Albumin transfer across the choroid plexus of South American opossum (Monodelphis domestica). J. Physiol.(Lond.) 499, 179–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Klosovskii B. N. (1963). The Development of the Brain and its Disturbance by Harmful Factors Translated by Haigh, B. Oxford: Pergamon. [Google Scholar]
  138. Kratzer I., Liddelow S. A., Saunders N. R., Dziegielewska K. M., Strazielle N., Ghersi-Egea J. F. (2013). Developmental changes in the transcriptome of the rat choroid plexus in relation to neuroprotection. Fluid Barriers CNS 10:25. 10.1186/2045-8118-10-25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Kuttner R., Sims J. A., Gordon M. W. (1961). The uptake of a metabolically inert amino acid by brain and other organs. J. Neurochem. 6, 311–317. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1961.tb13481.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Lajtha A., Toth J. (1961). The brain barrier system-II. Uptake and transport of amino acids by the brain. J. Neurochem. 8, 216–225. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1961.tb13545.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Lee J. C. (1971). Evolution of the concept of the blood-brain barrier concept. Prog. Neuropathol. 1, 84–145. [Google Scholar]
  142. Lefauconnier J.-M., Trouvé R. (1983). Developmental changes in the pattern of amino acid transport at the blood-brain barrier in rats. Brain Res. 283, 175–182. 10.1016/0165-3806(83)90095-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Lewandowsky M. (1900). Zur Lehre von der Cerebrospinalflüssgkeit. Z. Clin. Med. 40, 480–494. [Google Scholar]
  144. Liddelow S., Dziegielewska K. M., Ek C. J., Johansson P. A., Potter A., Saunders N. R. (2009). Cellular transfer of macromolecules across the developing choroid plexus of Monodelphis domestica. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 253–266. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06571.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Liddelow S., Dziegielewska K. M., Noor N., Potter A. M., Saunders N. R. (2011a). Modification of choroid plexus protein transfer from blood to cerebrospinal fluid in response to altered plasma protein composition during development. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 391–400. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07509.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Liddelow S. A., Dziegielewska K. M., Ek C. J., Habgood M. D., Bauer H., Bauer H.-C., et al. (2013). Mechanisms that determine the internal environment of the developing brain: a transcriptomic, functional and ultrastructural approach. PLoS ONE 8:e65629. 10.1371/journal.pone.0065629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Liddelow S. A., Dziegielewska K. M., Møllgård K., Phoenix T. N., Temple S., VandeBerg J. L., et al. (2011b). Sparc/osteonectin, an endogenous mechanism for targeting albumin to the blood-CSF interface during brain development. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1062–1073. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07821.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Liddelow S. A., Temple S., Møllgård K., Gehwold R., Wagner A., Bauer H., et al. (2012). Molecular characterisation of transport mechanisms at the developing mouse blood-CSF interface: a transcriptome approach. PLoS ONE 7:e33554. 10.1371./journal.pone.0033554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Lossinsky A. S., Vorbrodt A. W., Wisniewski H. M. (1986). Characterization of endothelial cell transport in the developing mouse blood-brain barrier. Dev. Neurosci. 8, 61–75. 10.1159/000112242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Lucey J. F. (1971). Changing concepts regarding exchange transfusions and neonatal jaundice. Clin. Obstet. Gynecol. 14, 586–593. 10.1097/00003081-197106000-00020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Malamud W., Fuchs D. M., Malamud N. (1928). Barrier between the blood and the cerebrospinal fluid. 1. Changes in permeability in mental disease. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 20, 780–798 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1928.02210160121010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  152. Mazariegos M. R., Tice L. W., Hand A. R. (1984). Alteration of tight junctional permeability in the rat parotid gland after isoproterenol stimulation. J. Cell Biol. 98, 1865–1877. 10.1083/jcb.98.5.1865 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Meek W. J. (1907). A study of the choroid plexus. J. Comp. Neurol. 17, 286–306. [Google Scholar]
  154. Mhanna M. J., Alesseh H., Gori A., Aziz H. F. (2008). Cerebrospinal fluid values in very low birth weight infants with suspected sepsis at different ages. Pediatr. Crit. Care Med. 9, 294–298. 10.1097/01.PCC.0b013e31816c6e12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Millen J. W., Hess A. (1958). The blood-brain barrier: an experimental study with vital dyes. Brain 81, 248–257. 10.1093/brain/81.2.248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Mitchell W., Kim C. S., O'Tuama L. A., Pritchard J. B., Pick J. R. (1982). Choroid plexus, brain and kidney Na+,K+-ATPase: comparative activities in fetal, newborn and young adult rabbits. Neurosci. Lett. 31, 37–40. 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90050-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Mizee M. R., Wooldrik D., Lakeman K. A., van het Hof B., Drexhage J. A., Geerts D., et al. (2013). Retinoic acid induces blood-brain barrier development. J. Neurosci. 33, 1660–1671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1338-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Møllgård K., Lauritzen B., Saunders N. R. (1979). Double replica technique applied to choroid plexus from early foetal sheep: completeness and complexity of tight junctions. J. Neurocytol. 8, 139–149. 10.1007/BF01175557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Møllgård K., Malinowska D. H., Saunders N. R. (1976). Lack of correlation between tight junction morphology and permeability properties in developing choroid plexus. Nature 264, 293–294. 10.1038/264293a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Møllgård K., Saunders N. R. (1975). Complex tight junctions of epithelial and of endothelial cells in early foetal brain. J. Neurocytol. 4, 453–468. 10.1007/BF01261375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Møllgård K., Saunders N. R. (1986). The development of the human blood-brain and blood-CSF barriers. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 12, 337–358. 10.1111/j.1365-2990.1986.tb00146.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Møllgård K., Saunders N. R. (1977). A possible transepithelial pathway via endoplasmic reticulum in foetal sheep choroid plexus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 199, 321–326. 10.1098/rspb.1977.0142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Moos T., Møllgård K. (1993). Cerebrovascular permeability to azo dyes and plasma proteins in rodents of different ages. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 19, 120–127. 10.1111/j.1365-2990.1993.tb00416.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Nag S. (2011). Morphology and properties of brain endothelial cells, Ch 1 in the blood-brain and other neural barriers: reviews and protocols. Methods Mol. Biol. 686, 3–35. 10.1007/978-1-60761-938-3_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Naidoo B. T. (1968). The cerebrospinal fluid in the healthy newborn infant. S. Afr. Med. J. 42, 933–935. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Nasralla M., Gawronska E., Hsia D. Y.-Y. (1958). Studies on the relation between serum and spinal fluid bilirubin during early infancy. J. Clin. Invest. 37, 1403–1412. 10.1172/JCI103730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Nattie E. E., Edwards W. H., Marin-Padilla M. (1984). Newborn puppy cerebral acid-base regulation in experimental asphyxia and recovery. J. Appl. Physiol. 56, 1178–1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Nitta T., Hata M., Gotoh S., Seo Y., Sasaki H., Hashimoto N., et al. (2003). Size-selective loosening of the blood-brain barrier in claudin-5-deficient mice. J. Cell Biol. 161, 653–660. 10.1083/jcb.200302070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Oldendorf W. M. (1971). Brain uptake of radiolabeled amino acids, amines, and hexoses after arterial injection. Am. J. Physiol. 221, 1629–1639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Olney J. W., Ho O. L. (1970). Brain damage in infant mice following oral intake of glutamate, aspartate or cysteine. Nature 227, 609–611. 10.1038/227609b0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Olsson Y., Klatzo I., Sourander P., Steinwall O. (1968). Blood-brain barrier to albumin in embryonic new born and adult rats. Acta Neuropathol. 16, 117–124. 10.1007/BF00691305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Ornoy A., Koren G. (2014). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in human pregnancy: on the way to resolving the controversy. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 19, 188–194. 10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Otila E. (1948). Studies on the cerebrospinal fluid in premature infants. Acta Paediatr. 35(Suppl. 8), 9–100. [Google Scholar]
  174. Pardridge W. M., Mietus L. J. (1982). Kinetics of neutral amino acid transport through the blood-brain barrier of the newborn rabbit. J. Neurochem. 38, 955–962. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1982.tb05335.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Penta P. (1932). Sulla colorazione vitale del sistema nervosa negli centrale animali neonati. Riv. Neurol. 5, 62–80. [Google Scholar]
  176. Piliero S. J., Lending M. (1959). Protein studies in normal newborn infants. Paper electrophoresis of cerebrospinal fluid and serum. Am. J. Dis. Child. 97, 785–789. 10.1001/archpedi.1959.02070010787004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Purpura D. P., Carmichael M. W. (1960). Characteristics of blood-brain barrier to gamma-aminobutyric acid in neonatal cat. Science 131, 410–412. 10.1126/science.131.3398.410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Ramey B. A., Birge W. H. (1979). Development of cerebrospinal fluid and the blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier in rabbits. Dev. Biol. 68, 292–298. 10.1016/0012-1606(79)90261-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Ramoz L. L., Patel-Shori N. M. (2014). Recent changes in pregnancy and lactation labeling: retirement of risk categories. Pharmacotherapy 34, 389–395. 10.1002/phar.1385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Ribatti D., Nico B., Bertossi M. (1993). The development of the blood-brain barrier in the chick. Studies with Evans blue and horseradish peroxidase. Ann. Anat. 175, 85–88. 10.1016/S0940-9602(11)80247-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Ribatti D., Nico B., Crivellato E., Artico M. (2006). Development of the blood-brain barrier: a historical point of view. Anat. Rec. B New Anat. 289B, 3–8. 10.1002/ar.b.20087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Risau W., Hallmann R., Albrecht U. (1986). Differentiation-dependent expression of proteins in brain endothelium during development of the blood–brain barrier. Dev. Biol. 117, 537–545. 10.1016/0012-1606(86)90321-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Risau W., Seulberger H. (1990). “Angiogenesis and differentiation of blood-brain barrier endothelium. Ch 1, in Pathophysiology of the Blood-Brain Barrier, Vol. 14, eds Johansson B. B., Owman C. H., Widner H. (Amsterdam: Elsevier: ), 3–9. [Google Scholar]
  184. Risau W., Wolburg H. (1990). Development of the blood–brain barrier. Trends Neurosci. 13, 174–178. 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90043-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Roberts R. B., Flexner J. B., Flexner L. B. (1959). Biochemical and physiological differentiation during morphogenesis. XXIII. Further observations relating to the synthesis of amino acids and proteins by the cerebral cortex and liver of the mouse. J. Neurochem. 4, 78–90. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1959.tb13176.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Rodier P. M. (1994). Vulnerable periods and processes during central nervous system development. Environ. Health Perspect. 102, 121–124. 10.1289/ehp.94102121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Rodier P. M. (1995). Developing brain as a target of toxicity. Environ. Health Perspect. 103, 73–76. 10.1289/ehp.95103s673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Rodier P. M. (2004). Environmental causes of central nervous system maldevelopment. Pediatrics 113, 1076–1083. Available online at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/113/Supplement_3/1076.full.html [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Romanoff A. L. (1967). Biochemistry of the Avian Embryo. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  190. Roncali L., Nico B., Ribatti D., Bertossi M., Mancini L. (1986). Microscopical and ultrastructural investigations on the development of the blood-brain barrier in the chick embryo optic tectum. Acta Neuropathol. 70, 193–201. 10.1007/BF00686072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Roux E., Borrel A. (1898). Tétanos cérébral et immunité contre le tétanus. Ann. Institut Pasteur 12, 225–239. [Google Scholar]
  192. Roy S., Hirano A., Kochen J. A., Zimmerman H. M. (1974). The fine structure of cerebral blood vessels in chick embryo. Acta Neuropath. 30, 277–285. 10.1007/BF00697010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Rubin L. L., Hall D. E., Porter S., Barbu K., Cannon C., Horner H. C., et al. (1991). A cell culture model of the blood-brain barrier. J. Cell Biol. 115, 1725–1735. 10.1083/jcb.115.6.1725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Sarff L. D., Platt L. H., McCracken G. H., Jr. (1976). Cerebrospinal fluid evaluation in neonates: comparison of high-risk infants with and without meningitis. J. Pediatr. 88, 473–477. 10.1016/S0022-3476(76)80271-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Saunders N. R. (1977). Ontogeny of the blood-brain barrier. Exp. Eye Res. 25(Suppl.), 523–550. 10.1016/S0014-4835(77)80046-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Saunders N. R. (1992). “Ontogenetic development of brain barrier mechanisms, in Physiology and Pharmacology of the Blood Brain-barrier. Vol. 103. Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology, ed Bradbury M. W. B. (Berlin: Springer-Verlag; ), 327–369. [Google Scholar]
  197. Saunders N. R., Adam E., Reader M., Møllgård K. (1989). Monodelphis domestica (grey short-tailed opossum): an accessible model for studies of early neocortical development. Anat. Embryol. 180, 227–236. 10.1007/BF00315881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Saunders N. R., Daneman R., Dziegielewska K. M., Liddelow S. A. (2013). Transporters of the blood-brain and blood-CSF interfaces in development and in the adult. Mol. Aspects Med. 34, 742–752. 10.1016/j.mam.2012.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Saunders N. R., Liddelow S. A., Dziegielewska K. M. (2012). Barrier mechanisms in the developing brain. Front. Neuropharmacol. 3:46. 10.3389/fphar.2012.00046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Sedlácek J. (1975). Some basic chemical components of the cerebrospinal fluid in developing chick embryos. Physiol. Bohemoslov. 24, 305–313. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Seta K., Sershen H., Lajtha A. (1972). Cerebral amino acid uptake in vivo in newborn mice. Brain Res. 47, 415–425. 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90649-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Spatz H. (1934). Die Bedeutung der vitalen Färbung für die Lehre vom Stoffaustausch zwischen dem Zentralnervensystem and dem iibrigen Körper. Arch. F. Psychiatr. 101, 267–358 10.1007/BF01789978 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  203. Spiegel-Adolf M., Baird H. W., 3rd., Szekely E. G., Wycis H. T. (1954). Cerebrospinal fluid studies in infant children with cerebral palsy and other neurologic disorders. Pediatrics 14, 215–221. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Srinivasan L., Shah S. S., Padula M. A., Abbasi S., McGowan K. L., Harris M. C. (2012). Cerebrospinal fluid reference ranges in term and preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. J. Pediatr. 161, 729–734. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.03.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Statz A., Felgenhauer K. (1983). Development of the blood-CSF barrier. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 25, 152–161. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1983.tb13738.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Stern L. (1934). A propos de la méthod d'investigation du fonctionnement de la barrière hémato-encéphalique. C. R. Soc. Biol. 115, 1059–1061. [Google Scholar]
  207. Stern L., Battelli F., Peyrot R. (1924). Fonctionnement de la barrière hémato-encéphalique (H.-E.) chez les divers animaux au cours de leur développement. Schw. Med. Wschr. 5, 313. [Google Scholar]
  208. Stern L., Gautier R. (1918a). Passage simultané des substances dans le liquide céphalo-rachidien et dans les centres nerveux. R. C. R. d. Ia Soc. de Phys. et d'hist. natur. de Genève 35, 58–60. [Google Scholar]
  209. Stern L., Gautier R. (1918b). Le passage dans le liquide céphalo-rachidien de substances introduites dans la circulation et leur action sur le système nerveux central chez les différentes espèces animales. R. C. R. d. Ia Soc. de Phys. et d'hist. natur. de Genève 35, 91–94. [Google Scholar]
  210. Stern L., Gautier R. (1921). Recherches sur le liquide céphalo-rachidien. 1. Les rapports entre le liquide céphalo-rachidien et la circulation sanguine. Arch. Int. Physiol. 17, 138–192 10.3109/13813452109146211 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  211. Stern L., Gautier R. (1922). Recherches sur le liquide céphalo-rachidien. II. Les rapports entre le liquide céphalo-rachidien et les éléments nerveux de l'axe cérébro-spinal. Arch. Int. Physiol. 17, 391–448 10.3109/13813452209146219 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  212. Stern L., Gautier R. (1923). Recherches sur le liquide céphalo-rachidien. II. Rapports entre le liquide céphalo-rachidien et des espaces ventriculaires et cellui des espaces sous-arachnodiens. Arch. Int. Physiol. 20, 403–436 10.3109/13813452309145975 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  213. Stern L., Peyrot R. (1927). Le fonctionnment de la barrière hémato-encéphalique aux divers stades de développement chez les diverses especes animals. C. R. Soc. Biol. 96, 1124–1126. [Google Scholar]
  214. Stern L., Rapoport J. L. (1928a). Les rapports entre l'augmentation de la perméabilité de la barrière hémato-encéphalique et les altérations de son substratum morphologique. C. R. Soc. Biol. 98, 1515–1517. [Google Scholar]
  215. Stern L., Rapoport J. L. (1928b). Les échanges entre le liquide céphalorachidien et les éléments nerveux cérébro-spinaux. C. R. Soc. Biol. 98, 1518–1519. [Google Scholar]
  216. Stern L., Rapoport J. L., Lokschina E.-S. (1929). Le fonctionnement de la barrière hémato-encéphalique chez les nouveau nés. C. R. Soc. Biol. 100, 231–223. [Google Scholar]
  217. Stewart P. A., Hayakawa E. M. (1987). Interendothelial junctional changes underlie the developmental “tightening” of the blood brain barrier. Brain Res. 429, 271–281. 10.1016/0165-3806(87)90107-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Stewart P. A., Hayakawa K. (1994). Early ultrastructural changes in blood-brain barrier vessels of the rat embryo. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 78, 25–34. 10.1016/0165-3806(94)90005-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Stewart P. A., Wiley M. J. (1981). Developing nervous tissue induces formation of blood-brain barrier characteristics in invading endothelial cells: a study using quail-chick transplantation chimeras. Dev. Biol. 84, 183–192. 10.1016/0012-1606(81)90382-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Tao-Cheng J. H., Nagy Z., Brightman M. W. (1987). Tight junctions of brain endothelium in vitro are enhanced by astroglia. J. Neurosci. 7, 3293–3299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Tauc M., Vignon X., Bouchaud C. (1984). Evidence for the effectiveness of the blood–CSF barrier in the fetal rat choroid plexus. A freeze-fracture and peroxidase diffusion study. Tissue Cell 16, 65–74. 10.1016/0040-8166(84)90019-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Tennyson V. (1975). “Ultrastructural characteristics of the telencephalic and myelencephalic choroid plexus in fetus of man and rabbit, and a comparison with the adult choroid plexus in rabbit, in The Choroid Plexus in Health and Disease, eds Netsky M. G., Shuangshati S. (Bristol: John Wright & Sons; ), 36–71. [Google Scholar]
  223. Tennyson V. M., Pappas G. D. (1964). Fine structure of the developing telencephalic and myelencephalic choroid plexus in the rabbit. J. Comp. Neurol. 123, 379–412. 10.1002/cne.901230307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Teratology Society Public Affairs Committee. (1994). FDA Classification of drugs for teratogenic risk. Teratology 49, 446–447. 10.1002/tera.1420490603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Teratology Society Public Affairs Committee. (2007). Teratology public affairs committee position paper: pregnancy labeling for prescription drugs: ten years later. Birth Defects Res. A Clin. Mol. Teratol. 79, 627–630. 10.1002/bdra.20389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Tschirgi R. D. (1950). Protein complexes and the impermeability of the blood-brain barrier to dyes. Am. J. Physiol. 163, 756. [Google Scholar]
  227. van den Bergh H. A. A., Müller P. (1916). Über eine direkte und indirekte Diazoreaktion auf Bilirubin. Biochem. Zeitschrift 77, 90–103. [Google Scholar]
  228. van Hasselt J. G., van Calsteren K., Heyns L., Han S., Mhallem Gziri M., Schellens J. H., et al. (2014). Optimizing anti-cancer drug treatment in pregnant cancer patients: pharmacokinetic analysis of gestation-induced changes for doxorubicin, epirubicin, docetaxel and paclitaxel. Ann. Oncol. 25, 2059–2065. 10.1093/annonc/mdu140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Vein A. A. (2008). Science and fate: Lina Stern (1878-1968), a neurophysiologist and biochemist. J. Hist. Neurosci. 17, 195–206. 10.1080/09647040601138478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Viña J. R., DeJoseph M. R., Hawkins P. A., Hawkins R. A. (1997). Penetration of glutamate into brain of 7-day-old rats. Metab. Brain Dis. 12, 219–227. 10.1007/BF02674614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Virgintino D., Errede M., Girolamo F., Capobianco C., Robertson D., Vimercati A., et al. (2007). Fetal blood-brain barrier P-glycoprotein contributes to brain protection during human development. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 67, 50–61. 10.1097/nen.0b013e31815f65d9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Virgintino D., Errede M., Robertson D., Capobianco C., Girolamo F., Vimercati A., et al. (2004). Immunolocalization of tight junction proteins in the adult and developing human brain. Histochem. Cell Biol. 122, 51–59. 10.1007/s00418-004-0665-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Virgintino D., Robertson D., Benagiano V., Errede M., Bertossi M., Ambrosi G., et al. (2000). Immunogold cytochemistry of the blood-brain barrier glucose transporter GLUT1 and endogenous albumin in the developing human brain. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 123, 95–101. 10.1016/S0165-3806(00)00086-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Volpe J. J. (2008). “Bilirubin and brain injury, in Neurology of the Newborn, 5th Edn., ed Volpe J. J. (Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders; ), 619–651. [Google Scholar]
  235. von Monakow C. (1921). Der Kreislauf des Liquor cerebro-spinalis. Schweiz Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 8, 233–234. [Google Scholar]
  236. Vorbrodt A. W., Lossinsky A. S., Wisniewski H. M. (1986). Localization of alkaline phosphatase activity in endothelia of developing and mature mouse blood-brain barrier. Dev. Neurosci. 8, 1–13. 10.1159/000112236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Wakai S., Hirokawa N. (1978). Development of the blood-brain barrier to horseradish peroxidase in the chick embryo. Cell Tissue Res. 195, 195–203. 10.1007/BF00236719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Wakai S., Hirokawa N. (1981). Development of blood-cerebrospinal fluid barrier to horseradish peroxidase in the avian choroidal epithelium. Cell Tissue Res. 214, 271–278. 10.1007/BF00249211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Walter K. (1934). Die allgemeinen Grundlagen des Stoffaustausches zwischen dem Zentralnervensystem und dem übrigen Körper. Archiv für Psychiatrie 10, 195–230 10.1007/BF01789976 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  240. Watson D. (1964). Modern methods for determining cerebrospinal fluid protein. Clin. Chem. 10, 412–416. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Watson R. E., Desesso J. M., Hurtt M. E., Cappon G. D. (2006). Postnatal growth and morphological Development of the brain: a species comparison. Birth Defects Res. B. Dev. Reprod. Toxicol. 77, 471–484. 10.1002/bdrb.20090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Weed L. H. (1917a). An anatomical consideration of the cerebro-spinal fluid. Anat. Res. 12, 461–496. 10.1002/ar.10901204059789630 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  243. Weed L. H. (1917b). The development of the cerebrospinal fluid spaces in pig and in man. Contrib. Embryol. Carnegie Inst. 5, 3–116. [Google Scholar]
  244. Wennberg R. P. (2000). The blood-brain barrier and bilirubin encephalopathy. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 20, 97–109. 10.1023/A:1006900111744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Wennberg R. P., Ahlfors C. E., Bhutani V. K., Johnson L. H., Shapiro S. M. (2006). Toward understanding kernicterus: a challenge to improve the management of jaundiced newborns. Pediatrics 117, 474–485. 10.1542/peds.2005-0395 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Widell S. (1958). On the cerebrospinal fluid in normal children and in patients with acute abacterial meningoencephalitis. Acta Paediatr. 47(Suppl.), 115. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1958.tb07687.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Wiesendanger M. (2010). Constantin von Monakow (1853-1930) and Lina Stern (1878-1968). Schweiz Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 161, 140–144. [Google Scholar]
  248. Windle W. F. (1969). Brain Damage by asphyxia at birth. Sci. Am. 221, 76–84. 10.1038/scientificamerican1069-76 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Wislocki G. B. (1920). Experimental studies on fetal absorption. I. The vitality stained fetus. Contrib. Embryol. Carnegie Inst. 11, 45–60. [Google Scholar]
  250. Wood K. E., Sinclair L. L., Rysgaard C. D., Strathman F. G., McMillin G. A., Krasowski M. D. (2014). Retrospective analysis of the diagnostic yield of newborn drug testing. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 14:250. 10.1186/1471-2393-14-250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Woodbury D. M., Johanson C., Brønsted M. (1974). “Maturation of the blood-brain and blood-cerebrospinal fluid barriers and transport systems, in Narcotics and the Hypothalamus, eds Zimmerman E., George R. (New York, NY: Raven Press; ), 225–247. [Google Scholar]
  252. Xu J., Ling E.-A. (1994). Studies of the ultrastructure and permeability of the blood-brain barrier in the developing corpus callosum in postnatal rat brain using electron dense tracers. J. Anat. 184, 227–237. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Zheng W. (2002). “Blood-brain barrier and blood-CSF barrier in metal-induced neurotoxicities, in Handbook of Neurotoxicology, Vol. 1, ed Massaro E. J. (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; ), 161–193. [Google Scholar]
  254. Zuelzer W. W., Mudgett R. T. (1950). KERNICTERUS: etiologic study based on an analysis of 55 cases. Pediatrics 6, 453–474. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES