Abstract
The conduct problems of children with callous-unemotional traits (i.e., lack of empathy, guilt/lack of caring behaviors) (CU) are particularly resistant to current behavioral interventions, and it is possible that differential sensitivities to punishment and reward may underlie this resistance. Children with conduct problems and CU (CPCU) are less responsive to behavioral punishment techniques (e.g., time-out), however reward techniques (e.g., earning points for prizes or activities) are effective for reducing conduct problems. This study examined the efficacy of modified behavioral interventions, which de-emphasized punishment (condition B) and emphasized reward techniques (condition C), compared to a standard behavioral intervention (condition A). Interventions were delivered through a Summer Treatment Program over seven weeks with an A-B-A-C-A-BC-A design to a group of eleven children (7–11 years; 91% male). All children were diagnosed with either oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder, in addition to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Results revealed the best treatment response occurred during the low punishment condition, with rates of negative behavior (e.g., aggression, teasing, stealing) increasing over the seven weeks. However, there was substantial individual variability in treatment response, and several children demonstrated improvement during the modified intervention conditions. Future research is necessary to disentangle treatment effects from order effects, and implications of group treatment of CPCU children (i.e., deviancy training) are discussed.
Keywords: callous-unemotional traits, conduct problems, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, summer treatment program
Approximately 10% of school-aged children have significant conduct problems, including aggression, disruptive behavior, and verbal conflict (Nock, Kazdin, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2006, 2007). In the absence of intervention, these problems tend to persevere and are linked to poorer outcomes in adolescence (e.g., substance abuse) and adulthood (e.g., incarceration) (Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, Silva, & Stanton, 1996; Robins & Price, 1991). Behavioral interventions have received strong empirical support for improving the current conduct problems (Wilson, Lipsey, & Derzon, 2003) and long-term trajectories of these children (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2011). However, nearly one-third of treated children do not benefit from these interventions (Masi et al., 2011; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1997). In an effort to better understand treatment response, recent research has focused on identifying characteristics that may account for this difference.
One constellation of child characteristics that may be relevant in this regard are callous-unemotional (CU) traits, which refers to an uncaring, unemotional, and callous affective style and a conning, manipulative, self-serving interpersonal style (Frick et al., 2003; Lynam, Caspi, Moffitt, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2007). Research published over the last two decades demonstrates that CU traits explain significant variance within the population of children with conduct problems. Children with conduct problems (CP) and CU traits (CPCU) as compared to children with conduct problems without CU traits (CP-only) are more likely to exhibit severe and violent antisocial behavior (Frick & Ellis, 1999; McMahon, Witkiewitz, & Kotler, 2010). Based on these and many other studies, with limited prosocial emotions (i.e., CU traits) has been added as a specifier of conduct disorder in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Importantly, there is some evidence that behavioral treatment for conduct problems – which is considered a “best practice” approach (Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs, 2008) – may be insufficiently effective for children with elevated levels of CU traits (Haas et al., 2011; Hawes & Dadds, 2005; Kimonis & Armstrong, 2012; Masi et al., 2011; Waschbusch, Walsh, Andrade, King, & Carrey, 2007). That is, the conduct problems of children with CU traits are more likely to persist following behavioral treatment compared to children with CP-only, and this results has been reported for both parent- and child-based interventions (e.g., Haas et al., 2011; Hawes & Dadds, 2005). The relation between CU traits and poor treatment response has been found even after controlling for pre-existing conduct problems, suggesting that it is CU traits themselves rather than the associated conduct problem severity that contribute to poor treatment response (Waschbusch et al., 2007).
If CU traits are associated with decreased response to behavioral interventions, what might account for this finding? One possibility is that this pattern may reflect differences in punishment and/or reward sensitivity. In particular, it has been hypothesized that children with CU traits are under responsive to punishment and over focused on reward (Dadds & Salmon, 2003). This hypothesis is supported by results from studies using controlled experimental tasks, which have found that children with CPCU are less likely to change their behavior in response to punishment (typically operationalized as a loss of points), especially when they are first primed with a reward (Blair, Colledge, & Mitchell, 2001; O’Brien & Frick, 1996). Further, fMRI research shows these behavioral differences are accompanied by differences in brain activation, suggesting that children with CPCU process punishment and reward decisions in qualitatively distinct ways (Finger et al., 2008).
There also is evidence, albeit indirect evidence, from treatment studies that suggests children with CPCU are under responsive to punishment, overfocused on reward, or both. Timeout procedures, arguably a punishment technique, may be less effective for children with CPCU than for children with CP-only (Haas et al., 2011; Hawes & Dadds, 2005). Haas and colleagues examined response to intensive behavioral treatment among elementary school age children with CP and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and reported that higher CU traits were associated with more negative behavior during time-out. On the other hand, reward-based strategies may effective for reducing problematic behavior among children with CPCU (Hawes & Dadds, 2005).
The implication of these findings is clear; standard behavior therapy, in which punishment and reward techniques are used in a balanced fashion to shape behavior, may not be as effective for children with CPCU. If so, implementing behavior therapy in a manner that deemphasizes punishment and emphasizes reward techniques techniques may be a more effective approach for children with CPCU. In theory, this latter approach may align more closely with the unique punishment insensitive/reward-focused learning style exhibited by children with CPCU. The limited available treatment research to indicate that punishment techniques used in standard behavioral treatments (i.e., time-out, loss of privileges) are not only ineffective (Hawes & Dadds, 2005) but may actually induce escalations in negative behaviors among children with CU traits (Haas et al., 2011). Therefore, reducing (as much as possible) punishment by ignoring as much inappropriate behavior as possible, while recognizing that not all negative behavior can be ignored, may be an optimal approach for children with CPCU. Further, to the extent that CU traits are associated with goal-directed/reward-driven behavior regardless of the negative consequences, and to the extent that CU traits are associated with being self-serving, then it also may be effective to use reward-based strategies to shape the behaviors of children with CPCU. In other words, strong and frequent rewards may be a tool that can align the child’s heightened self-interest with the interests of those around him or her.
The purpose of this study was to use single-case research methods to examine the response of children with CPCU to four behavioral treatment conditions: (1) standard treatment in which punishment and reward were delivered in a balanced manner; (2) low punishment treatment, in which negative consequences for undesirable/negative behaviors were minimized; (3) high reward treatment, in which positive consequences for positive or non-negative behavior were maximized; and (4) combined treatment, in which punishments were minimized and rewards were maximized. We chose a single-case design for its versatility, as we would be implementing several different treatment conditions over a short period of time to a small group of children. We hypothesized that rates of negative behavior (i.e., noncompliance, conduct problems, negative verbalizations) would be significantly lower in the low punishment, high reward, and combined conditions compared to standard treatment. Further, we predicted that negative behavior would be lowest in the combined condition, and that the low punishment and high reward conditions would perform equally well. As a secondary aim, we were interested in whether children’s behavior during time-out would change across conditions, as time-out has been identified in previous research as an area of particular difficulty for behavioral interventions with this population. Parallel to our primary hypotheses, we expected a similar pattern of treatment response across day-to-day activities and time-out behavior.
Method
Participants
Participants were 11 children (1 female) between the ages of 7 and 11 (M = 9.5, SD = 1.2). Participants were recruited between April and June 2011 from a larger treatment program for children with ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD). Families were recruited to the larger program using radio and television advertisement, postings in public places (e.g., university websites, billboards), fliers distributed to pediatrician offices and elementary schools, and word of mouth from families enrolled in previous years. Of the 76 participants in the larger treatment program, 35 met eligibility criteria for this research study (i.e., IQ of 80 or above, between 7 and 11 years old, diagnosed with ODD or CD, and had clinically significant levels of CU traits). The remaining children were not in the appropriate age range (n = 22), did not meet criteria for ODD or CD (n = 15), or did not have a sufficiently elevated CU score (n = 4). Families first completed an assessment for the larger treatment program and eligibility to participate in this research study was determined based on the information gathered at the initial assessment. Eligible families were contacted and given information about the nature and purpose of the research program and they were offered the chance to enroll their child in the research program in exchange for receiving the treatment free of charge. This process was conducted on a first-come-first-served basis until 12 participants were enrolled in the study. In total, 15 potentially eligible families were approached with three declining participation. Two families declined for pragmatic reasons, and one family declined because they were not interested in the project. One child, who was enrolled in the study, was dropped from data analyses due to noncompliance with the research protocol (parents administered psychoactive medication to the child, which was prohibited). Children were predominantly Hispanic-Caucasian (83%). Average household income ranged from $15,000 to $59,000 (M = $36,000, SD = $14,433). Eighty-two percent of the children lived with their biological parents (45% married/common law; 36% divorced/separated/single), and 18% lived with relatives or adoptive parents. Intellectual abilities, as assessed using the WISC-IV, ranged from full scale IQ of 88 to 123 (M = 104, SD = 12).
All children met criteria for conduct problems, defined as ODD or CD, as well as ADHD. Diagnoses were made by Ph.D. clinicians using several sources of information, including parent and teacher ratings on the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (DBDRS; Pelham, Gnagy, Greenslade, & Milich, 1992) and the Impairment Rating Scale (IRS; Fabiano et al., 2006) and parent report on the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Structured Parent Interview (DBDSPI; Pelham, Greiner, & Gnagy, 2006). Following DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), diagnoses were assigned if a sufficient number of symptoms were endorsed and if the child evidenced clinically significant impairment. Of the 11 participants, 6 (55%) were diagnosed with ODD/ADHD and 5 (45%) were diagnosed with CD/ADHD. Rating scale data for the participants is summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Measure | Parent | Teacher |
---|---|---|
DBDSPI symptom counts | ||
Hyperactivity/impulsivity | 5.0 (1.4) | -- |
Inattention | 6.6 (2.3) | -- |
Oppositional defiant disorder | 5.0 (1.6) | -- |
Conduct disorder | 2.2 (1.9) | -- |
DBDRS symptom counts | ||
Hyperactivity/impulsivity | 5.5 (2.4) | 5.5 (3.0) |
Inattention | 7.6 (2.5) | 6.4 (3.3) |
Oppositional defiant disorder | 6.3 (2.3) | 4.5 (3.2) |
Conduct disorder | 2.3 (2.5) | 1.0 (1.2) |
ICU | 37.5 (5.0) | 40.9 (12.4) |
APSD | 80.9 (7.1) | 74.5 (7.1) |
CPS - R | 35.9 (5.0) | 31.8 (10.1) |
Note. Values in table are means with standard deviations in parentheses.
Children also were also selected based on the presence of elevated levels of CU traits. Our primary measure of CU traits was parent and teacher ratings on the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2001). This measure was selected because it is currently the only measure of CU traits with established psychometric properties, including normative data, when used to assess elementary school age children. Norms were developed for the APSD using a large community sample of school-aged children recruited from referrals to mental health clinics (Frick & Hare, 2001). All participants had T scores at or above 65 on the CU scale as rated by parent, teacher, or both, which indicates a clinically significant level. We also measured CU traits using the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU; Frick, 2004) and the Child Psychopathy Scale – Revised (CPS-R; Lynam, 1997), which have been used in several studies but do not yet have published norms available. Means and standard deviations for these measures also are reported in Table 1.
Procedure
Overview
All procedures used in this study were approved by a university institutional review board and parent consent and child assent were obtained prior to initiating the research. Children were enrolled in the 2011 Summer Treatment Program (STP) conducted in the southeast United States. The STP is an eight-week intensive treatment program for children with clinically significant attention or behavior problems (Pelham, Greiner, & Gnagy, 1998). The STP was conducted from 8:00 AM until 5:00 PM on weekdays. During the STP day children participated in the following activities: two academic classes and an art class, three group recreational activities, swimming, lunch, and recess. Children were placed in a single group of 12 children who remained together throughout treatment. A lead counselor supervisor and four undergraduate counselors (outside of the classroom), and a teacher and teacher aide (in the classroom) implemented treatment. During recreational periods, children played soccer, softball, or basketball, and during classroom periods children worked on individualized academic assignments. A more extensive description of the STP is available elsewhere (Pelham et al., 2010).
The first week of the STP was considered a warm-up week, in which both the counselors and children were learning procedures; data from this week was not used in this study. After the first week, treatment was implemented in one-week blocks for seven consecutive weeks. Treatment was evaluated in a group setting using an alternating treatment manipulation that followed an A-B-A-C-A-BC-A design, with conditions designated as follows: A = standard treatment; B = low punishment treatment; C = high reward treatment; BC = combined treatment (low punishment and high reward). The experimental design follows earlier research that used reversal designs to examine treatment effects in small group settings (Ayllon & Azrin, 1967). Order of the non-standard treatments (B, C, BC) was determined randomly. Prior to treatment, counselors were intensively trained and tested on STP procedures (see Pelham et al., 2010 for details). During treatment, counselors were observed for at least one hour everyday by clinical supervisors and given feedback on treatment implementation to maintain treatment integrity. To facilitate the transition between treatment conditions, counselors had weekly meetings with clinical supervisors to prepare for and practice changes to treatment protocol and new procedures were explained and discussed with children at the onset of each new condition. Counselors were blinded to children’s conduct problem diagnoses and CU levels, but were not blinded to treatment condition as they were implementing them. Data analyses also were conducted by individuals who were not blinded to treatment condition.
Standard treatment
In standard treatment, children’s behavior was shaped through daily and weekly reinforcers for appropriate behaviors, and daily and weekly punishment for inappropriate behaviors. The primary daily reinforcement was the chance to participate in recess at lunch and again at the end of the day. The primary weekly reinforcer was the chance to participate in “Fun Friday,” which was a different fun activity each week (video game time, swim time, etc.). Punishers included detention in place of recess, during which children were required to write sentences, and detention in place of a Fun Friday, during which children were required to complete chores (cleaning desks, sweeping floors, etc.). In addition, a ten minute time-out was assigned for intentional aggression, intentional destruction of property, and repeated noncompliance. Whether children earned reward, punishment, or neither (a neutral outcome) each day and week was based on their performance on: (a) a daily behavior report card (DRC), and (b) the comprehensive token economy that awarded children points for positive and non-negative behaviors and took points away for negative behaviors. The DRC consisted of list of treatment goals that were individualized to each child and were evaluated throughout the day. Children earned daily rewards if they met a high percent (> 80%) of their treatment goals, or received punishment if they met a low percent (< 50%) of their treatment goals. Parents were instructed to continue with a normal evening routine (i.e., no reward or punishment) when children met a moderate percent (50–80%) of treatment goals. Weekly consequences (reward, punishment, or neutral) were based on DRC performance and on the number of points they earned during the week. Parents were given feedback about their child’s behavior at the end of each day and instructed to deliver the same daily or weekly outcome (reward, punishment, neutral) at home that their child received at the STP for that day or week.
Low punishment treatment
In the low punishment condition, the same procedures were followed as in standard treatment but with the following modifications. First, time-out was reduced from 10 minutes to two minutes. Second, daily and weekly punishments were dropped such that daily and weekly consequences consisted of two levels (reward or neutral). That is, children either earned recess as a daily reward or they did not (i.e., children sat out of recess but sentence writing during recess was discontinued as a punishment) and children either earned Fun Friday as a weekly reward or they did not (i.e., children participated in a typical STP day and chores were discontinued as a punishment). Third, children did not lose points for inappropriate behavior, although we continued to identify inappropriate behavior (e.g., “You’re interrupting me” rather than “You lose 20 points for interruption”), and DRCs were revised to include only positive treatment goals. At home parents were encouraged to either reward children for achieving most of their DRC goals (> 80% goals met), or continue with the normal evening routine (< 80% goals met).
High reward treatment
The high reward condition matched the standard condition except that children had the opportunity to earn material rewards in addition to fun activities. Material rewards (toys) were earned through a ticket system in which children were awarded tickets every 15 minutes that they did not exhibit negative behavior. They also were awarded tickets for exhibiting positive behaviors. Children exchanged their tickets for toys at the end of the week, with better toys requiring more tickets. Toys were identified using a reinforcement interview with parents and with the child. Children were given frequent reminders of the reinforcement associated with tickets both verbally (during group discussion and individual conversations with counselors) and by allowing them to visit the toy store multiple times per day.
Combined treatment
The combined treatment condition consisted of simultaneously implementing the procedures used in the low punishment and high reward treatments. Conceptually, children were given high incentive to not misbehave while simultaneously given as little attention as feasible for misbehavior.
Diagnostic Measures
DBDRS
The DBDRS consists of 45 questions designed to measure DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD, ODD and CD using a four-point Likert scale (0 = not at all to 3 = very much; Pelham et al., 1992). Validity of the DBDRS has been supported by research showing significant convergent and discriminant validity as compared to other measures of psychopathology (Massetti, Pelham, & Gnagy, June 2005) and as compared to observations of children’s behavior (Wright, Waschbusch, & Frankland, 2007). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) for our sample ranged from .78 to .90 and .86 to .95 across parent- and teacher-reported symptom scales respectively.
DBDSPI
The DBDSPI is a structured interview designed to measure DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and CD (Pelham et al., 2006). The DBDSPI was administered by trained interviewees (PhD level clinicians or advanced graduate students) to parents in a one-on-one setting. Clinicians introduced each symptom by asking parents to describe their child’s typical behavior over the past year using situational prompts (e.g., at home, at school) provided in the interview. Based on parental responses, clinicians rated how much the symptom was a problem, in each situation and overall, using operationally defined scales that ranged from “not a problem” to “severe problem”. Symptoms were counted as present if the overall rating indicated it was a moderate or severe problem within the last six months. The internal consistency ranged from .84 to .93 (Cronbach’s alpha) across symptom subscales.
APSD
The APSD consists of 20 questions designed to measure CU traits, narcissism, and impulsivity (Frick & Hare, 2001). Only the six items from the CU scale were used in the current study, and these were rated on a three-point Likert scale (0 = not at all true to 2 = definitely true). The validity of the CU scale on the APSD has been supported in numerous studies (Kotler & McMahon, 2010). Internal consistency within our sample was consistent with past research (Cronbach’s alpha = .56 and .50 for parent- and teacher-report) (e.g., Bijttebier & Dacoene, 2009), however, these values are likely an underestimate of internal consistency because of our small sample size.
ICU
The ICU consists of 24 items designed to measure the extent to which children are uncaring, callous, and unemotional (Frick, 2004). Items were rated using a four-point Likert scale (0 = not at all true to 3 = definitely true). Although norms for the ICU have not yet been established for elementary school age samples, preliminary research has supported its validity and reliability in this age range (Houghton, Hunter, & Crow, 2012). The internal consistency for this measure in our sample was good for parent- and teacher-report (Cronbach’s alpha = .82 and .89).
CPS-R
The CPS-R consists of 55 items designed to measure numerous facets of psychopathic personality, including callous-unemotional traits (Lynam, 1997). Items were rated as present or absent using a yes/no response format and, after reverse coding appropriate items, summed into a total score. Although norms for the CPS-R are not available for elementary school age samples, preliminary research has supported the reliability and validity of the total score in youth as young as 12 years old (Kotler & McMahon, 2010). This measure had good internal consistency for our sample (parent-report alpha = .73; teacher-report alpha = .82).
Dependent Measures
Overview
The dependent measures were frequency counts of participant behavior as recorded by counselors throughout the day in the context of implementing the comprehensive token economy (i.e., a point system). The current study used data from the three sports activity periods and from time-out. We excluded data from transitions (i.e., walking in line from one location to another), lunch, and swimming because treatment was most consistently implemented during the sports periods (i.e., children spent a considerable portion of transitions and swimming in locker rooms and bathrooms, where it was difficult to accurately monitor them, and rates of behavior during lunch were low, when they were occupied by eating). Point-system data was not collected during the three classroom periods, as another form of behavioral management (response-cost) was used in the classroom (for details refer to the STP manual; Pelham et al., 1998). Finally, this study used data from Monday through Thursday because children spent Friday afternoon receiving their weekly contingency (reward, neutral, or punishment activity, depending on their behavior and treatment condition), which necessitated a non-standard schedule on Fridays.
Negative behavior during sport activities
Negative behavior during sport activities was operationalized using the following point-system behavioral categories: aggression, destruction of property, noncompliance, stealing, lying, verbal abuse to staff, teasing peers, and swearing (see Pelham et al., 1998 for operational definitions). These behaviors were combined in a single negative behavior score for parsimony. Frequency counts of these behaviors were summed across the three sports periods for each day. If a child was missing for a portion of the day, his/her daily behavior totals were pro-rated based on the time they were present. Using these totals, we calculated a daily average of negative behavior for each week of treatment for each child. Past research using STP point-system data has found high interrater reliability and strong correlations between point-system categories and other measures of behavioral problems, such as parent-report or clinical interview (Wright et al., 2007).
Negative behavior during time-out
As noted earlier, time-out was assigned following every instance of intentional aggression, intentional destruction or property, or repeated noncompliance. Negative behavior was measured using the same definition described earlier (a sum of the frequency counts of lying, stealing, destruction of property, etc.) but applying this definition to times the child was in time-out. However, because the duration of time-out was manipulated as a function of treatment condition, we did not use a total frequency count of negative behavior (nor did we use duration or frequency of time-out) as the dependent variable. Instead, we used the rate of negative behavior during time-out, computed by dividing the frequency of negative behavior during time-out by the total minutes of time-out. This was computed each day for each participant.
Data Analyses
For both sport activity and time-out data, we followed earlier research (Ayllon & Azrin, 1967) by first examining group-level treatment effects and then examining individual differences in treatment response. Group effects were examined using repeated measures ANOVAs with treatment condition as the independent variable. For the purpose of the ANOVAs, standard treatment was separated into two treatment conditions: the mean of negative behavior during the first two weeks of standard treatment (standard12), and the mean of negative behavior during the second two standard weeks (standard34). This was justified because negative behavior was appreciably lower during the first two weeks of standard treatment (activity M = 10.23, SD = 10.50; time-out M = .11, SD = .19) compared to the latter two standard weeks (activity M = 14.43, SD = 13.94; time-out M = .51, SD = .44) on both activity and time-out measures, t(10) = −2.83, p =.02, and t(8) = −2.71, p = .03 respectively. Thus, the ANOVA compared the following conditions: standard12 vs. standard 34 vs. low punishment vs. high reward vs. combined treatment. Assumption testing was conducted with Mauchley’s test of sphericity, and analyses violating this assumption were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geiser degrees of freedom adjustment. Significant differences were followed up with multiple comparisons using t-tests based on a least significant difference. After examining these group effects, individual differences in treatment response were explored by plotting data separately for each participant. All analyses were conducted using SPSS (Version 20.0).
Results
Sports Activities
Group effects
Using repeated measures one-way ANOVA, there was a significant main effect of condition on levels of negative behavior during activities, F(2, 16) = 5.28, p = .02, ηp2 = .37. The assumption of sphericity was violated therefore degrees of freedom were adjusted. Examination of means (see Table 2) and post hoc multiple comparisons showed that negative behavior was highest during the combined condition, with rates of negative behavior that were significantly higher than any other condition (ps < .03; Hedge’s g for combined condition with: standard12 = .69, low punishment = .92, high reward = .92, standard34 = .41). The standard34 and high reward conditions followed, and the standard34 condition had significantly higher rates of negative behavior than the standard12 condition (p = .01; Hedge’s g = .33), and marginally significantly higher than the low punishment condition (p = .09; Hedge’s g = .63). However, levels of negative behavior during the high rewards condition were not significantly different from any other condition excluding the combined condition. Finally, negative behavior was lowest during the low punishment and standard12 conditions, which did not significantly differ from one another.
Table 2.
S12 | LP | HR | S34 | LPHR | F-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Activities | 10.23a (10.49) | 7.43ac (5.24) | 13.60ac (16.63) | 14.43c (13.94) | 21.74b (20.03) | 5.28* |
Time-out | 0.09c (0.17) | 0.05ac (0.11) | 0.30ad (0.24) | 0.51bd (0.44) | 0.68b (0.57) | 8.68*** |
Note. LP = low punishment, HR = high reward, LPHR = combined, S12 = standard12, and S34 = standard34. Means with differing subscripts are significantly different at p < .05 based on least significant difference post hoc comparisons.
p < .05
p < .01
p< .001
Individual differences
Descriptive analyses of individual differences in response to treatment (see Figure 1) suggested a number of interesting patterns. First, a handful of children demonstrated a similar response pattern to the group-level pattern, albeit at varying levels of negative behavior severity (i.e., participants 1, 4, and 10). On the other hand, some children responded well to both low punishment and high reward in comparison to the standard treatment conditions (i.e., participants 6, 8, and 9). Finally, some children demonstrated consistently low levels of negative behavior throughout the summer (i.e., participants 2, 5, 7, and 11), whereas others demonstrated consistently high levels of negative behavior throughout the summer (e.g., participant 3 [absent during low punishment due to physical injury that occurred outside of treatment]).
Time-out
Group effects
Repeated measures one-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of condition on levels of negative behavior, F(4, 20) = 8.68, p < .001, ηp2 = .64. Examination of means (see Table 2) and post hoc multiple comparisons showed that time-out data largely paralleled the sports activities data. First, the combined condition had significantly higher rates of negative behavior compared to any other condition (ps < .04; Hedge’s g for combined condition with: standard12 = 1.35, low punishment = 1.44, high reward = .84) excluding standard34. The second highest rates of negative behavior occurred during the standard34. Negative behavior during the standard34 condition was significantly higher than the standard12 and low punishment conditions (ps < .02; Hedge’s g for standard34 with: standard12 = 1.21, low punishment = 1.35), and marginally significantly higher than the high reward condition (p = .09; Hedge’s g = .57). Negative behavior during the high rewards condition was significantly higher than during the standard12 condition (p = .04; Hedge’s g = .97), but did not significantly differ from rates of negative behavior during the low punishment condition. Finally, the lowest rates of negative behavior occurred during the standard12 and low punishment conditions, both of which were close to zero and did not significantly differ.
Individual differences
Individual differences during time-out also paralleled individual differences during sport activities (see Figure 2). Again, participants 1 and 10 demonstrated a similar response pattern as group-level performance. However, relative to participant 4’s performance in other conditions, this child demonstrated relatively uniform and high levels of negative behavior across the high reward, combined, and standard34 conditions. Further similarities between time-out and activity performance were noted among children with low base rates of negative behavior during activities, who were rarely in time-out and never demonstrated any negative behaviors at any point during their time-outs. Interestingly, there was no consistent response pattern within time-out among children who responded well to both low punishment and high reward conditions during activities (i.e., participants 6, 8, and 9). Only participant 9 replicated his/her activity response pattern during time-out, whereas participant 6 performed relatively poorly during the high reward condition and participant 8 performed relatively poorly during the low punishment condition on time-out measures. Finally, participant 3 maintained relatively high levels of negative behavior during time-out across all conditions for which this child was present.
Discussion
The current study examined the treatment response of 11 children with CPCU to four modifications of behavioral therapy. Group-level analyses indicated the best treatment response occurred during the low punishment and standard12 conditions, whereas the worst response occurred during the combined condition. Frequencies of negative behaviors during the high reward and standard34 conditions fell between these points. These results were similar across both activity and time-out measures. Thus, our hypothesis that children would demonstrated less negative behavior during the modified treatment conditions relative to the standard treatment conditions was partially supported in that negative behavior was lowest during the low punishment condition relative to the latter standard treatment condition. In contrast, we did not find evidence in support of the efficacy of either the high reward or combined treatment conditions, as levels of negative behavior during both the high reward and combined conditions were equal to or greater than levels of negative behavior during standard treatment. Unexpectedly, the highest rates of negative behavior actually occurred during the combined condition.
These results support the findings of previous research suggesting that children with CU traits are less sensitive to punishment while engaging in goal-directed behavior (Dadds & Salmon, 2003), and consequently less likely to change their behavior in the face of punishment. First, excluding the combined condition, we found relatively higher rates of negative behavior both during activities and in time-out when standard punishment methods were used (i.e., high reward and standard conditions). Also, negative behavior during the standard conditions worsened over time, suggesting that punishment strategies were doing little to ameliorate negative behavior. In contrast, we observed the lowest rates of negative behavior during activities and time-out in the low punishment condition. Indeed, none of the children demonstrated any aggression, destruction of property, stealing, or lying during time-out in the low punishment condition. This is a promising finding, given that previous research has found that time-out is particularly difficult to implement among children with CU traits (Haas et al., 2011). It is possible that standard punishment methods (e.g., point loss, 10 minute time-out, detention during recess and Fun Friday) attenuated children’s motivation to behave appropriately as punishment interfered with their ability to earn rewards. In the face of seemingly impossible goals, other types of reinforcers, such as peer attention or counselor negative attention, may have become more salient and enticing. Consequently, children may have engaged in higher rates of negative behavior as a means to these ends. Furthermore, the increasing rates of negative behavior during the standard conditions also are interesting when compared to past STP research of children with ADHD-only, which report significant reductions in negative behavior over eight weeks of standard treatment (i.e., standard condition; Pelham et al., 2000). Therefore, our results suggest that worsening treatment response may be an effect of CU traits – although this is speculative as we did not have a comparison ADHD-only group.
These interpretations also are tempered by the wide variability of response patterns across participants. Indeed, three response patterns were evident in our inspection of the individual treatment response graphs. Parallel to the group analysis, some children appeared to respond best to the low punishment and standard12 conditions, in contrast other children seemed to respond best to the high rewards condition. Finally, a handful of children demonstrated rates of negative behavior similar to those of normative samples across all treatment conditions (Pelham et al., 1998). Although all children in our sample shared the same clinical profiles (i.e., CPCU with ADHD), it is curious how variable their treatment responses were. It is likely that other environmental and child characteristics moderated each child’s response pattern. For instance, in our opinion children who responded well were more motivated than others to bond with counselors, consequently child-counselor rapport may have been an important factor in treatment responsiveness. Further, some children demonstrated greater self-control and presumably were able to manage urges to misbehave when these urges ran counter to their goals. On the other hand, other children reacted impulsively to challenging situations (e.g., trying to sit in classroom quietly while peers were being disruptive), meaning they had fewer opportunities to be reinforced/rewarded for appropriate behavior, and more opportunities to be reinforced for inappropriate behavior via peer attention (e.g., other children cheer when child runs around classroom). Obviously these suggestions are speculative, and future research is necessary to understand the wide variability of treatment responsiveness of children with CPCU.
In spite of this heterogeneity, two consistencies were noted: (1) the combined condition had the highest rates of negative behavior, and (2) there was a trend for children’s behavior to worsen over time (i.e., rates of negative behavior during the first two weeks of standard treatment were significantly less than the latter 2 weeks on most behavioral measures). Given these consistencies, it is likely that ordering effects influenced treatment response patterns. It may be that the low punishment condition outperformed the other conditions because it appeared earlier in the STP (during the third week), whereas the combined treatment may have underperformed relative to other conditions because it occurred later in the STP (during the seventh week). That is, even without manipulating treatment children’s behavior may have differed earlier versus later in treatment as peers and counselors become more familiar with each other and as peers and counselors become more fatigued by the demanding nature of the treatment. These and other factors cannot be disentangled from the treatment effects in this study. To do so, order of treatment would have to be counterbalanced across participants, but the small sample size used in this pilot study did not allow for this possibility.
Related to this point, there is anecdotal evidence that experience within each treatment modification played an important role in children’s treatment response. Children appeared to show a more positive response to treatment procedures when they were first introduced as compared to when they had gained some experience with them. For instance, the lowest rates of negative behavior occurred the first day the children were exposed to new treatment procedures. Interestingly, this pattern did not hold for across the standard treatment weeks, suggesting it was the novelty of a new system rather than the start of a new week that influenced treatment response. Consistent with this observation, children’s behavior was often at its worst when they had experience with treatment procedures and gained knowledge of how to exploit those procedures. For instance, during the combined treatment condition counselors were not allowed to take points for negative behaviors but they were allowed to give commands to stop exhibiting negative behaviors for two minutes when children were being so disruptive that the group could not function. Because children earned points for compliance when they followed this command, they quickly learned to exhibit high rates of negative behavior so their counselors would issue a command for them to stop, which in turn earned them points for compliance. Thus, the poor response to treatment in the combined condition was likely due to the fact that high rates of negative behavior had no appreciable negative consequence (point loss) but did have relatively predictable positive consequences (points for complying with a command to stop misbehaving). More generally, these observations suggest that novelty of treatment may be an especially important to consider in designing treatments for children with CU traits, and we suspect that boredom and familiarity with treatment protocols (and their weaknesses) contributed to increasing negative behavior.
Deviancy training, in which children’s misbehavior is reinforced by the social reactions of peers, is thought to be especially problematic in group-based treatments for children with antisocial behavior (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011). This may have been evidence in the present study as well in that we observed one child frequently encourage other children to behave aggressively towards counselors while they were in time-out. This appeared to occur most often in the combined treatment condition which inadvertently delivered the least effective treatment. If this observation is confirmed in future research, it would suggest that deviancy training may be important to consider when treatment children with CU traits in group settings.
This study was the first (that we are aware of) to modify behavioral treatment procedures for use with children with CPCU in a group-based format. Although this is a novel study, results should be interpreted with consideration of the limitations of this study. First, as previously mentioned, treatment conditions were presented in a single order, making it impossible to parse ordering effects and practice/time effects from treatment effects. Second, our sample was small, and it is possible that we did not have the power to detect certain effects. Further, because of the small sample size and the need to take Type II error into account, we did not correct for family-wise error in our post hoc testing and our comparisons are be vulnerable to Type I error. It also was impossible to control for participant characteristics that may have moderated treatment response such as intelligence, age, or CU trait severity. Nonetheless, we tried to limit these effects by restricting the range of these variables (e.g., recruiting children with clinically significant CU traits only; limiting age range to school-aged children). Third, this data did not control for counselor behavior. Although the counselors were implementing a very structured treatment and were routinely observed, it is possible that their behavior towards the children changed as they developed relationships with the children. For example, one counselor became a favorite of the children and over the course of the summer it became increasingly difficult for her to implement time-out effectively because the children found any time with her to be reinforcing. Fourth, the DRC goals were changed weekly and may have accounted for some variance in treatment response, and we were unable to formally assess parents’ implementation of these goals. Also, the weekly changes in treatment condition may have been difficult for children to adjust to and longer treatment periods would have been more desirable. Finally, our treatment manipulations were behaviorally based and did not focus on the relationships among children or between children and staff. Recent research suggests that children with CU traits tend to have an insecure attachment style (Pasalich, Dadds, Hawes, & Brennan, 2012) and consequently may benefit from emotion-focused interactions with positive role models.
Future research should address these limitations in several ways. Of most importance, these results should be replicated in studies using multiple treatment groups that randomly assign children to treatment and that counter-balance the order of treatment conditions. Incorporating both behavioral and attachment perspectives into a treatment model may improve treatment response and possibly reduce deviancy training effects. Future research also should examine how STP procedures could be translated into parent-child treatment models, as it is a more accessible form of psychological treatment.
Highlights.
We examined effects of behavioral interventions on children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits
Children demonstrated improvement when punishments (e.g., time-out) were reduced
There was substantial variability in treatment response across children
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (7R34MH085796) awarded to Daniel Waschbusch. The authors also wish to thank the staff at the Center for Children and Families, and the families who participated in the study.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Natalie V. Miller, Email: nvmiller@psych.ubc.ca.
Sarah M. Haas, Email: smhaas@buffalo.edu.
Daniel A. Waschbusch, Email: dwaschbusch@hmc.psu.edu.
Michael T. Willoughby, Email: willoughby@unc.edu.
Sarah A. Helseth, Email: shelseth@fiu.edu.
Kathleen I. Crum, Email: kcrum@fiu.edu.
Erika K. Coles, Email: ecoles@fiu.edu.
William E. Pelham, Jr., Email: wpelham@fiu.edu.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 2000. text rev. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. Washington, DC: Author; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Ayllon T, Azrin NH. The measurement and reinforcement of behavior of psychotics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1967;8:357–383. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1965.8-357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijttebier P, Dacoene S. Assessment of psychopathic traits in children and adolescents: Further validation of the Antisocial Process Screening Device and the Childhood Psychopathy Scale. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2009;25:157–163. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.25.3.157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJR, Colledge E, Mitchell DGV. Somatic markers and response reversal: Is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:499–511. doi: 10.1023/a:1012277125119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. The effects of the Fast Track preventive intervention on the development of conduct disorder across childhood. Child Development. 2011;82:331–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01558.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dadds MR, Salmon K. Punishment insensitivity and parenting: Temperament and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2003;6:69–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1023762009877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Tipsord JM. Peer contagion in child and adolescent social development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2011;62:189–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Boggs SR. Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2008;37:215–237. doi: 10.1080/15374410701820117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fabiano GA, Pelham JWE, Waschbusch DA, Gnagy EM, Lahey BB, Chronis AM, et al. A practical measure of impairment: Psychometric properties of the Impairment Rating Scale in samples of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and two school-based samples. [doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3] Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:369–385. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finger EC, Marsh AA, Mitchell DG, Reid ME, Sims C, Budhani S, et al. Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:586–594. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ. Inventory of callous-unemotional traits. University of New Orleans; New Orleans, LA: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Cornell AH, Bodin SD, Dane HE, Barry CT, Loney BR. Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:246–260. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Ellis M. Callous-unemotional traits and subtypes of conduct disorder. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1999;2:149–168. doi: 10.1023/a:1021803005547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Hare RD. The Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Haas SM, Waschbusch DA, Pelham WE, Jr, King S, Andrade BF, Carrey NJ. Treatment response in CP/ADHD children with callous/unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:541–552. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9480-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawes DJ, Dadds MR. The treatment of conduct problems in children with callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:737–741. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.4.737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houghton S, Hunter SC, Crow J. Assessing callous-unemotional traits in children aged 7- to 12-years: A confirmatory factor analysis of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2012;35:215–222. doi: 10.1007/s10862-012-9324-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis ER, Armstrong K. Adapting parent-child interaction therapy to treat severe conduct problems with callous-unemotional traits: A case study. Clinical Case Studies. 2012;11:234–252. [Google Scholar]
- Kotler JS, McMahon RJ. Assessment of child and adolescent psychopathy. In: Salekin RT, Lynam DT, editors. Handbook of Child and Adolescent Psychopathy. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. pp. 79–109. [Google Scholar]
- Lynam DR. Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:425–438. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.106.3.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynam DR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:155–165. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.116.1.155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masi G, Manfredi A, Milone A, Muratori P, Polidori L, Ruglioni L, et al. Predictors of nonresponse to psychosocial treatment in children and adolescents with disruptive behavior disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2011;21:51–55. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0039. doi:10.10891cap.2010.0039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massetti GM, Pelham WE, Gnagy EM. Situational variability of ADHD, ODD, and CD: Psychometric properties of the DBD interview and rating scale for an ADHD sample. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology; New York, NY.. Jun, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- McMahon RJ, Witkiewitz K, Kotler JS. Predictive validity of callous-unemotional traits measured in early adolescence with respect to multiple antisocial outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:752–763. doi: 10.1037/a0020796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva P, Stanton W. Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct problems in males: Natural history from ages 3 to 18 years. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:399–424. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007161. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nock MK, Kazdin AE, Hiripi E, Kessler RC. Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:699–710. doi: 10.1017/s0033291706007082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nock MK, Kazdin AE, Hiripi E, Kessler RC. Lifetime prevalence, correlates, and persistence of oppositional defiant disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:703–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien BS, Frick PJ. Reward dominance: Associations with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1996;24:223–240. doi: 10.1007/bf01441486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J. Attachment and callous-unemotional traits in children with early-onset conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:838–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02544.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Jr, Gnagy EM, Greenslade KE, Milich R. Teacher ratings of DSM-III-R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1992;31:210–218. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Jr, Gnagy EM, Greiner AR, Hoza B, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, et al. Behavioral versus behavioral and pharmacological treatment in ADHD children attending a summer treatment program. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2000;28:507–525. doi: 10.1023/A:1005127030251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Jr, Gnagy EM, Greiner AR, Waschbusch DA, Fabiano GA, Burrows-Maclean L. Summer treatment programs for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Weisz JR, Kazdin AE, editors. Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents. 2. New York, NY US: Guilford Press; 2010. pp. 277–292. [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Jr, Greiner AR, Gnagy EM. Children’s summer treatment program manual. Buffalo: CTADD; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Pelham WE, Jr, Greiner AR, Gnagy EM. Disruptive behavior disorders clinical interview. Buffalo: CTADD; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Robins LN, Price RK. Adult disorders predicted by childhood conduct problems: Results from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area project. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes. 1991;54:116–132. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1991.11024540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waschbusch DA, Walsh TM, Andrade BF, King S, Carrey NJ. Social problem solving, conduct problems, and callous-unemotional traits in children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2007;37:293–305. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0033-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M. Treating children with early-onset conduct problems: A comparison of child and parent training interventions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65:93–109. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.65.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson SJ, Lipsey MW, Derzon JH. The effects of school-based intervention programs on aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:136–149. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright KD, Waschbusch DA, Frankland BW. Combining data from parent ratings and parent interview when assessing ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2007;29:141–148. doi: 10.1007/s10862-006-9039-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]