Abstract
Studies of non-elite distance runners suggest that men are more likely than women to slow their pace in the marathon.
Purpose
This study determined the reliability of the sex difference in pacing across many marathons, and after adjusting women’s performances by 12% to address men’s greater maximal oxygen uptake and also incorporating information on racing experience.
Methods
Data was acquired from 14 U.S. marathons in 2011, and encompassed 91,929 performances. For 2,929 runners, we obtained experience data from a race-aggregating website. We operationalized pace maintenance as percentage change in pace observed in the second half of the marathon relative to the first half. Pace maintenance was analyzed as a continuous variable and as two categorical variables: “maintain the pace,” defined as slowing < 10%; and “marked slowing,” defined as slowing ≥30%.
Results
The mean change in pace was 15.6% and 11.7% for men and women, respectively (P<0.0001). This sex difference was significant for all 14 marathons. The odds for women were 1.46 (95% CI: 1.41 to 1.50, P<0.0001) times higher than men to maintain the pace and 0.36 (95% CI: 0.34–0.38; P<0.0001) times that of men to exhibit marked slowing. Slower finishing times were associated with greater slowing, especially in men (interaction, P<.0001). However, the sex difference in pacing occurred across age and finishing-time groups. Making the 12% adjustment to women’s performances lessened the magnitude of the sex difference in pacing but not its occurrence. Although greater experience was associated with lesser slowing, controlling for the experience variables did not eliminate the sex difference in pacing.
Conclusions
The sex difference in pacing is robust. It may reflect sex differences in physiology, decision making, or both.
Keywords: gender, distance running, risk taking, endurance exercise, experience, substrate utilization
Introduction
Pacing in endurance races has long been of interest to sports scientists, but research has been traditionally limited to elite or sub-elite athletes (1, 13, 28, 37). The recent availability of data from mass-participation events, however, has permitted pacing to be investigated in a broader range of athletes, including non-elite distance runners. Several notable patterns have emerged, including that men are more likely than women to slow their pace in the marathon (42.195 km) (26, 35).
The sex difference in pacing could reflect physiological differences, such as men’s greater susceptibility to muscle glycogen depletion (7, 31, 34) or women’s skeletal muscle showing lesser fatigability (21). An alternative, but compatible, possibility is that pacing differences may reflect decision making (2). In particular, men may be more likely to adopt a “risky” pace, where an individual begins the race with a fast early pace (relative to their ability) and this increases their likelihood of slowing later. If either of these possibilities proved correct, they would constitute a novel source of pacing variation, one that has not been considered in studies of elite or sub-elite athletes.
Here we addressed the robustness of the sex difference in marathon pacing by extending previous studies in three ways. First, rather than focusing on one marathon, we evaluated a sample of 14 marathons, which encompasses data from over 91,000 performances. This allowed us to generalize our findings to a range of ages and a variety of races.
Second, we obtained information on racing experience for a sub-sample of participants (> 2,900) and tested whether experience was related pacing and whether it might contribute to the (possible) sex difference in pacing. A previous study reported that older runners showed more even marathon pacing and suggested that this reflected their greater racing experience (26). However, no previous study has directly tested whether race experience is associated with better pacing.
Third, we made a 12% adjustment or correction to women’s performances to address the fact that they are roughly 10–12% slower than men’s even when training and talent are similar (8, 19, 20, 24, 32). This sex difference reflects men’s greater maximal oxygen uptake (VO2 max), which is mediated by their larger hearts, greater hemoglobin concentration, lesser body fat, and greater muscle mass per unit of body weight (8, 19, 20, 24, 32). Making this adjustment allows the alignment of relative performance between the sexes. This is potentially important because, at least in some populations (e.g., older runners), a larger proportion of men than women achieve relatively fast performances (11, 19, 20), and relatively fast performances may, in turn, be associated with more even pacing (26). In other words, the previously reported sex difference in pacing (26, 35) might merely reflect that more men than women run relatively fast.
Although most of our analyses are based on making a 12% adjustment to women’s performances, we also explored other adjustments. We used a 10% adjustment to address the commonly-mentioned lower bound for the sex difference in VO2 max (8, 21, 22, 26, 34) and the 10% difference we observed in the mean finishing times for men and women in our sample. We used a 16% adjustment because the sex difference in endurance performance, although typically about 10% for world records and other leading performances, is often observed to be 14–16% for near-elite performers (11, 20, 32). Finally, to allow direct comparisons with previous studies (2, 28, 37), we conducted analyses without any adjustment.
The purpose of this study, therefore, was to compare the pacing or pace maintenance (duration of first half of the marathon relative to the second half) of men and women across different age groups and abilities (finishing times) in a large sample of marathons. A secondary purpose was to determine whether racing experience was related to pacing and if it might contribute to the sex difference in pacing. A third purpose was to test whether adjusting women’s performances to account for their lower VO2 max would affect the sex difference in pacing.
Methods
This study did not require formal approval by Grand Valley State’s Institutional Review Board (IRB) or the other IRBs (Marquette University; Mayo Clinic). The Grand Valley State University IRB determined the protocol (reference # 14-078-H) was exempt under federal category 45 CFR 46.101(b) (4) because all data was pre-existing and public.
We began identifying potential marathons using marathon.guide.com, a website that attempts to maintain a comprehensive list. We initially considered all 75 marathons with greater than 1,000 finishers held in the U.S. in 2011. We discovered, however, that many races did not provide halfway times, and, among those races that did, data was often difficult to download efficiently. We therefore focused on the 14 races where halfway and full race data was presented with the active.com timing platform, which permitted efficient data acquisition.
Of the 96,774 individual times available, the final analysis set was comprised of 91,929 (95%) usable finishing times. Criteria for inclusion in the final data set were having timing data for the halfway mark and the full race in proper sequence (e.g., finishing time greater than half marathon time); a net time less than the gun time; and a slowing less than 400%. The number of participants by marathon is presented in Table 1. The sample consisted of 41.5% women (n = 38,130). The largest three races (Chicago, Disney and Philadelphia) constituted 64.3% of the sample (n = 59,140), and each of the remaining 11 races comprised 1.2% to 5.2% of the total participants.
Table 1.
Listing of marathon participants and summary data on percentage change in pace from first half to second half by sex.
Total Sample | Women | Men | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Participants | Percentage Change in pace |
Participants | Percentage Change in pace |
Participants | Percentage Change in pace |
||||||||
Marathon | Count | % <a> |
Mean | SD | Count | % <b> |
Mean | SD | Count | % <b> |
Mean | SD | p-value <c> |
Air Force | 3199 | 3.5 | 19.2% | 14.9% | 1042 | 32.6 | 16.9% | 12.1% | 2157 | 67.4 | 20.3% | 16.0% | <.0001 |
Atlanta | 1109 | 1.2 | 13.8% | 12.1% | 422 | 38.1 | 11.5% | 9.6% | 687 | 62.0 | 15.2% | 13.2% | 0.0006 |
Austin | 4792 | 5.2 | 15.2% | 13.0% | 1941 | 40.5 | 12.1% | 10.2% | 2851 | 59.5 | 17.3% | 14.2% | 0.0003 |
Baltimore | 3188 | 3.5 | 17.7% | 15.1% | 1189 | 37.3 | 14.4% | 12.4% | 1999 | 62.7 | 19.7% | 16.2% | <.0001 |
Chicago | 35571 | 38.7 | 16.0% | 13.1% | 15412 | 43.3 | 13.5% | 10.9% | 20159 | 56.7 | 17.9% | 14.2% | <.0001 |
Cleveland | 2598 | 2.8 | 10.3% | 11.0% | 994 | 38.3 | 9.6% | 10.0% | 1604 | 61.7 | 10.7% | 11.5% | 0.0317 |
Dallas | 4473 | 4.9 | 10.5% | 11.4% | 1702 | 38.1 | 8.6% | 9.4% | 2771 | 62.0 | 11.6% | 12.3% | <.0001 |
Disney | 13431 | 14.6 | 9.4% | 11.7% | 6225 | 46.4 | 8.1% | 10.1% | 7206 | 53.7 | 10.5% | 12.9% | <.0001 |
Georgia | 2201 | 2.4 | 13.7% | 12.0% | 788 | 35.8 | 10.9% | 9.1% | 1413 | 64.2 | 15.2% | 13.1% | <.0001 |
Grand Rapids | 1581 | 1.7 | 11.3% | 13.5% | 697 | 44.1 | 9.5% | 10.1% | 884 | 55.9 | 12.7% | 15.6% | 0.0011 |
Miami | 3498 | 3.8 | 12.3% | 12.5% | 1245 | 35.6 | 10.6% | 10.7% | 2253 | 64.4 | 13.2% | 13.3% | <.0001 |
Philadelphia | 10138 | 11.0 | 13.1% | 12.4% | 4209 | 41.5 | 11.1% | 10.6% | 5929 | 58.5 | 14.6% | 13.3% | <.0001 |
Pittsburgh | 4255 | 4.6 | 10.8% | 11.0% | 1624 | 38.2 | 9.0% | 9.0% | 2631 | 61.8 | 11.9% | 12.0% | <.0001 |
St. Louis | 1895 | 2.1 | 21.6% | 13.2% | 640 | 33.8 | 18.6% | 11.8% | 1255 | 66.2 | 23.1% | 13.6% | <.0001 |
Total | 91929 | 14.0% | 13.0% | 38130 | 41.5 | 11.7% | 10.9% | 53799 | 58.5 | 15.6% | 14.2% |
Percentage of participants in the total sample from the individual race
Percentage of women and men participants within race
P-value from post-hoc comparison of percentage change between sex for each marathon.
To address racing experience, we obtained information from athlinks.com, a website that seeks to aggregate finishers’ data from all races. Although some races included information as far back as the early 1980s, information was more comprehensive for races occurring since the early 2000s. We searched for each finisher’s name and recorded their total number of races, total number of marathons, personal bests for the 5K and marathon, and earliest year with a recorded race.
Searches for some finishers’ names yielded race data from more than one individual (e.g., several people named Jane Jones). In some cases, we were able to determine which races were completed by the marathon finisher of interest because of unique identifiers (e.g., age, hometown). Also, some runners “claimed” a series of races that they had finished and these performances thus became identified with a unique member profile. In cases with ambiguities and/or no member profile, we excluded the marathoner of interest. Obtaining experience and ability data in this manner was highly time-consuming so we only investigated a sample of finishers. In particular, we generated a randomized list of approximately 6,000 marathoners from the full sample of 91,929 performances; we were able to obtain experience data for 2,929 of these.
Three research assistants participated in the acquisition of the experience data, and we investigated the reliability of their work by assigning two of them to independently code 210 marathon finishers who had been previously assigned to other coders. Inter-coder reliability for identifying finishers was modest (Cohen’s κ = 0.51): in 85 cases both coders identified the marathon finisher in athlinks.com, in 75 cases neither identified the finisher, and in 52 cases one coder but not the other identified the finisher. In cases where both coders identified the finisher, their agreement was good: total previous races, r (83) = 0.97, P < 0.0001; previous marathons, r (83) = .97, P < 0.0001; fastest previous marathon, r (83) = 0.96, P < 0.0001; fastest 5K, r (40) = 0.99, P < 0.0001; and earliest year with a recorded race, r (83) = 0.91, P < .0001.
Statistical analysis
The overarching goal of the analysis was to test for sex differences in patterns of pace maintenance over the marathon while adjusting for men’s generally faster performances. Furthermore, we sought to control for the potential confounding effects of age and running ability through the use of stratification. Total finishing time, first half time, and second half time were determined from chip times (i.e., when each individual crossed the starting line). The finishing times for men were categorized into < 3 hr 00 min up to 5 hours or more in 30 minute increments. Women’s categories were shifted up by 12% to account for the sex difference in VO2 max. For example, the <3 hr 00 min boundary for men would equate to a 3 hr 22 min boundary for women. For analyses using pacing as a continuous variable, the chip times of women were divided by 1.12 to introduce the 12% adjustment.
Pace maintenance was operationalized by calculating the percentage change in pace observed in the second half of the marathon relative to the first half pace (% change = (second half time − first half time)/first half time). This percentage change was used as a continuous variable as well as for two categorizations. For the first categorization, percentage changes less than 10% were considered “maintaining the pace.” For example, a runner who completed the first half of the marathon in 2 hr 0 min and completed the second half in 2 hr 11 min or faster maintained the pace; 2 hr 12 min or slower failed to maintain the pace. For the second categorization, percentage changes greater than 30% were considered as “marked slowing.” This classification was created to represent a transition from running in the first half to considerable walking during the second half of the race. For example, a runner who completed the first half of the marathon in 2 hr 0 min and the second half in 2 hr 36 min or slower exhibited marked slowing. Mantel-Haenszel techniques for stratified data were used to summarize the odds for maintaining, or not, the pace during the race when controlled for age, first half pace and race. In instances where the odds ratio was not defined due to a zero cell, the empirical logit was used for computation. This estimator for the odds ratio added 0.5 to each of the cell prior to the calculation of the stratum-specific odds ratio and the overall pooled estimate.
These categorizations enabled ease of interpretation and graphical display. To ensure statistical efficacy, regression analyses were also used to test for co-incidence of regression lines for men vs. women using the continuous percentage change in pace by finishing time. Also, multiple linear regression techniques were used to explore the relationship of experience variables with the percentage change in pace.
All statistical analyses were conducted using the SAS system version 9.3 (Cary, NC). Two-sided p-values less than 0.05 were taken as statistically significant.
Results
A total of 91,929 runners (41.5% women) were included in this analysis. The mean ± SD ages for men and women were 38.9 ± 10.8 and 35.5 ± 9.7 years. The mean finishing times for men and women were 4 hrs 28 min ± 53 min and 4 hrs 54 min ± 52 min, a difference of 10%. The mean ± SD change in pace was 15.6% ± 14.2% and 11.7% ± 10.9% for men and women, respectively (P < 0.0001). Although there was variability across marathons, women consistently showed less change in pace (P < 0.05 for all 14 marathons, Table 1).
Pacing and finishing time
Table 2 reports the distribution of percentage change in pace (duration of first half of the marathon relative to the second half) by finishing time group separately for men and women. The finishing time groups for women have been adjusted by 12% so they are comparable with the performances of men. The percentage change in pace varied across finishing time group for each sex (P < 0.0001 for both sexes). Table 2 shows that slower groups had lower percentages of participants who maintained the pace (< 10% slowing) and more participants (%) who exhibited marked slowing (≥ 30% slowing). This pattern of greater slowing with greater finishing times was more pronounced in men than women (see below).
Table 2.
Percentage slowing by sex and finishing time group. Finishing time groups for women have been adjusted 12% to account for VO2max differences.
Maintained the Pace (< 10% Slowing) |
Marked Slowing (≥30% Slowing) |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | Finishing Time | N | Mean | SD | N | % | N | % |
Women | <3 hr 22 min | 677 | 5.0% | 6.0% | 575 | 84.9 | 2 | 0.3 |
<3 hr 55 min | 4041 | 5.7% | 6.5% | 3184 | 78.8 | 13 | 0.3 | |
<4 hr 29 min | 8504 | 8.7% | 8.4% | 5192 | 61.1 | 125 | 1.5 | |
<5 hr 02 min | 9824 | 11.3% | 10.1% | 4785 | 48.7 | 436 | 4.4 | |
<5 hr 36 min | 6912 | 14.6% | 11.5% | 2506 | 36.3 | 585 | 8.5 | |
5 hr 36 min or more | 8172 | 16.5% | 12.5% | 2461 | 30.1 | 930 | 11.4 | |
Men | <3 hr 00 min | 1355 | 5.6% | 6.6% | 1096 | 80.9 | 12 | 0.9 |
<3 hr 30 min | 5534 | 7.6% | 8.1% | 3809 | 68.8 | 85 | 1.5 | |
<4 hr 00 min | 11439 | 10.3% | 10.4% | 6386 | 55.8 | 529 | 4.6 | |
<4 hr 30 min | 11732 | 15.0% | 12.4% | 4474 | 38.1 | 1283 | 10.9 | |
<5 hr 00 min | 10413 | 18.6% | 14.3% | 3109 | 29.9 | 2021 | 19.4 | |
5 hr 00 min or more | 13326 | 22.7% | 16.5% | 2727 | 20.5 | 3586 | 26.9 |
Finishing time groups are listed as non-mutually exclusive categories for brevity in presentation. For example, "<3 hr 55 min" corresponds to the stratum of finishers having finishing times of 3 hr 22 mins or greater but less than 3 hr 55 mins.
We calculated the common (pooled) odds ratios for maintaining the pace (Figure 1A) and marked slowing (Figure 1B) for women relative to men across the finishing-time groups. Overall, women were 1.46 times (95% CI: 1.41 to 1.50; P < 0.0001) more likely to maintain the pace (< 10% slowing) compared to men. There were, however, differences among the finishing time groups (P < 0.0001, indicated by a Breslow-Day test for homogeneity). The largest sex differences occurred in the sub-elite category (3:00 – 3:30 for men, 3:22 – 3:55 for women; OR = 1.68, 95% CI: 1.53 to 1.85) and those with the slowest finishing times (5:00+ for men, 5:36+ for women; OR = 1.67, 95% CI: 1.57 to 1.78).
Figure 1. Odd Ratios of Maintaining Pace and Marked Slowing.
Forest plot for the odds of maintaining the pace (< 10% change in pace) (A) and marked slowing (≥ 30% change in pace) (B) for women relative to men. The error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The common estimate is the pooled odds ratio as estimated by the Mantel-Haenszel estimator. ‘Favors women’ indicates that women were less likely to slow than men. Conversely, ‘favors men’ means that men were less likely to slow than women.
Overall, 5% of women and 14% of men experienced marked slowing (≥ 30% slowing), which resulted in women having 64% lower odds for marked slowing than men (OR = 0.36, 95% CI: 0.34 to 0.38; Table 2, Fig. 1B). This pattern of women showing lesser odds of marked slowing did not differ significantly across finishing time groups (Breslow-Day test, P = 0.11). However, there were only 14 participants (12 women and 2 men) in the elite group (< 3:00 men; 3:22 women) so that the precision of estimated odds ratio was poor (wide confidence interval).
Pacing and age
Figures 2 and 3 display the odds ratios of maintaining the pace and marked slowing for women relative to men, with stratification for finishing time group and age group. The overall (pooled) estimate of the common odds ratio for maintaining the pace with adjustment for finishing time and age was 1.54 (95% CI: 1.50 to 1.59; P < 0.001) for women relative to men, although there was significant variation across groups (Breslow-Day test, P < 0.001; Fig. 2). For marked slowing, the common odds ratio with adjustment for finishing time and age was 0.32 (95% CI: 0.31 to 0.34; P < 0.001) for women relative to men. Similar to the results presented in Figure 1B, patterns were similar across finishing time and age groups (P = 0.19, Breslow-Day test; Fig. 3).
Figure 2. Odd Ratios of Maintaining Pace.
Forest plot for the odds of maintaining the pace (< 10% change in pace) for women relative to men with stratification for finishing time group and age group. The error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The common estimate is the pooled odds ratio as estimated by the Mantel-Haenszel estimator. The empirical logit estimator for the odds ratio was used when at least one sex category had no (or all) events (e.g., all men and women in the < 3 hour marathon finishing time group).
Figure 3. Odd Ratios of Marked Slowing.
Forest plot for the odds of marked slowing (≥ 30% change in pace) for women relative to men with stratification for finishing time group and age group. The error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The common estimate is the pooled odds ratio as estimated by the Mantel-Haenszel estimator. The empirical logit estimator for the odds ratio was used when at least one sex category had no (or all) events (e.g., all men and women in the < 3 hour marathon finishing time group).
Change in pace as a continuous measure
To assess the sensitivity of categorizing finishing time and percentage slowing, we conducted regression analyses. Men and women slowed differentially (F(2,91925) = 993; P < 0.0001), so that the sex difference widened among slower runners (adjusted for women relative to men by 12%). For instance, percentage slowing for a 3hr 00 min male finisher and 3 hr 26 min female finisher (compared because of the 12% adjustment for finishing time) were 6.9% and 5.5%, respectively (25% difference). However, for a 5 hr 00 min male finisher and a 5 hr 36 min female finisher 12% adjusted), the slowing widened to 18.8% and 14.5%, respectively (30% difference). This regression model explained 20% (R-Square = 0.197) of the variation in percentage change in pace, but the relationships were not strictly linear.
To examine the non-linear relationship more fully, figure 4A illustrates a locally-weighted (LOESS smoother) plot relating finishing times with percentage change in pace for the entire sample. A scatter plot consisting of a random sample of 5000 finishers has been superimposed onto the figure to add context to these regression fits. The percentage slowing increases more rapidly in men than women as the finishing time increases.
Figure 4. Percentage Change in Pace as a function of Finishing Time for Men and Women.
A. LOESS smoothed curves modeling the relationship of percentage change in pace as a function of finishing times and for men and women who finished under 6 hours (n = 84,277). The points plotted are a random sample of 5000 finishers to provide context to the LOESS fitted lines. Women’s finishing times have been divided by 1.12 to account for VO2max differences.
B. LOESS smoothed curves modeling the relationship of percentage change in pace as a function of finishing times under 6 hours (n = 84,277) that are unadjusted and adjusted for physiological sex differences. Women, 10% adjustment indicates that women’s finishing times were divided by 1.10; Women, 16% adjustment indicates that women’s finishing times were divided by 1.16.
Sensitivity analysis for the 12% adjustment
We next investigated whether our results were sensitive to the 12% adjustment we made to women’s performances. Figure 4B displays locally-weighted plots relating finishing times to change in pace for the entire sample using no adjustment, a 10% adjustment, and a 16% adjustment. No adjustment produced a larger estimate of the sex difference in pacing than when using an adjustment (10% or 16%). Further, the sex difference in pacing was smaller with a 16% adjustment than with the 12% adjustment used in our earlier analyses (Fig. 4A). Nonetheless, even with a 16% adjustment, the sex difference in pacing remained apparent in all analyses as non-coincident regression lines (i.e., regression lines of women generally indicating lesser slowing for any given finishing time).
Race experience
Finally, we explored the effect of experience on pacing in a subgroup of 2,929 runners. These runners had a mean finishing time of 4 hrs 36 min and included 42.8% female participants. The overall mean ± SD percentage change in pace was 15.0% ± 13.5%. As with the entire sample, men’s percentage change in pace was greater than the women’s (16.8% vs 12.5%; P < 0.0001). Similar to the entire sample, 12.4% of the racers showed marked slowing, with men showing a higher frequency than women (16.7% vs. 6.6% respectively; P < 0.0001).
The next step was assessing which experience variables were significantly related to uneven pacing. We found greater slowing among those individuals with their earliest recorded race after 2008 (≤ 3 years of experience) (ρ = 0.08; P < 0.001), those with fewer prior marathons (ρ = −0.08; P < 0.001) and those with fewer total races at all distances (ρ = −0.07; P < 0.001). There was also greater pace slowing among those whose fastest previous marathons were slower (ρ = 0.19; P < 0.001, in 1856 participants that ran more than one marathon).
Consistent with the findings with the entire data set, the relationship of the 12% adjusted finish time and percentage slowing differed between men and women (test for coincidence, F(2,2925) = 38.6; P < 0.001). This model explained 20% of the variation in changes in pacing (R-Square = 0.197). With the addition of participant age, and the experience variables of total number of prior marathons and recent introduction to marathons (≤ 3 years of experience), the r-square increased to 20.8% (n = 2910; R-Square = 0.208). There was no evidence that the experience variables affected pacing differently in men and women (F(2,2901) = 2.06, P = 0.13). In a final model, consisting of the 12% adjusted finishing time, age, total marathons run, recent introduction to the marathon, and sex (the main effects of sex and its interaction with finishing time) the coefficients of partial determinations were as follows: 17.7%, 0.1%, < 0.1%, < 0.1%, and 2.9% respectively. Thus, finishing time and the differences between men and women explained the largest percentages of the variation in the change in pace. The estimated percentage point difference in pace change, men relative to women, for relative finish times of 2 hr 30 min (i.e., female finish time of 2 hr 48 min), 3 hr 00 min and 5 hr 00 min were 1.6 (p = 0.15), 2.2 (P = 0.001) and 4.7 (P < 0.001) percentage points after adjusting for these experience variables.
Discussion
This study shows that the sex difference in marathon pacing among non-elite runners is robust in several respects. First, the sex difference was significant in all 14 marathons in our sample. These marathons differed considerably in their size and extent of slowing (Table 1), indicating that the sex difference occurs generally. Second, we showed that, although age (i.e., being an older runner) and faster performances are associated with more even pacing (26), the sex difference generally persists across ages and abilities. There was evidence, however, for an interaction, with a less pronounced sex difference among faster runners. Third, we demonstrated that adjusting women’s performances by 12%—to address men’s greater VO2max—does not eliminate the sex difference in pacing. Fourth, we found that, although inexperience is associated with greater pace slowing, controlling for experience does not eliminate the sex difference. Finally, our study is notable in its exploration of pace slowing as both a continuous and as a categorical variable. Thus, rather than merely stating that men slowed more than women on average, we showed that, after controlling for age and ability and making the 12% adjustment, women had approximately one-third the odds as men to experience marked slowing (running the second half of the race more than 30% slower than the first half).
Besides demonstrating the robustness of the sex difference in pacing, our study yielded other notable results. One finding is that, making a 12% adjustment to women’s performances did affect the magnitude of the sex difference in pacing. In particular, the sex difference was larger without an adjustment, indicating that previous studies that did not make an adjustment may have overestimated the sex difference in pacing (26, 35). Nonetheless, even with the 12% adjustment, evidence for the sex difference in pacing remained reliable. We also showed that although the estimated sex difference in pacing is generally insensitive to the exact adjustment used (between 10 and 16%), the sex difference in pacing decreases as the adjustment increases.
Our study also provided the first results directly bearing on the hypothesis that experience is related to pacing in the marathon (26). We showed that four experience variables were significantly associated with pacing, namely years of experience, prior marathons, prior races at all distances, and fastest previous marathon. Thus, more experienced runners paced more evenly and inexperience was associated with greater pace slowing. Moreover, at least two of the experience variables (total marathons run and recent introduction to the marathon) explained variation in pacing independent of the effects of age, finishing time, and sex. The amount of variation explained by the experience variables, however, was modest (> 0.1% for each) compared to sex (2.9%) and finishing time (17.7%). Consequently, controlling for experience does not eliminate the sex difference.
What causes the sex difference in pacing?
Various physiological factors might contribute to the sex difference in marathon pacing, although we did not measure these. For example, men are more susceptible to muscle glycogen depletion, which can contribute to greater fatigability and “hitting the wall” (HTW) or dramatic marathon slowing (4, 9, 30, 33). Men may be more susceptible to slowing because, during endurance exercise, women generally have a lower respiratory exchange ratio indicating they utilize proportionately more fat and less carbohydrate at a given intensity of exercise (7, 25, 34, 36). In general, women possess larger proportional areas of type I muscle fibers which are more fatigue resistant especially for long duration exercise (21). Supporting this hypothesis are studies finding that men are more likely than women to report dramatic slowing or HTW (4, 33).
Another possibility is that men might be more susceptible to hyperthermia (35), which is believed to be another frequent contributor to marathon slowing (16, 27). Supporting this is the finding that the sex difference in marathon pacing in non-elites was larger in the 2007 Chicago marathon, which was hot (27 °C), than in the 2009 Chicago marathon, which was cool (3 °C) (35). Challenging this hypothesis, however, are studies indicating that men typically enjoy advantages in thermoregulation (14).
Decision making could also contribute to the sex difference in marathon pacing. Specifically, if men are more likely to adopt a “risky” or competitive pace relative to their ability, this could increase their likelihood of slowing later. This is because a fast pace has numerous physiological consequences, including increasing the risk of hyperthermia and glycogen depletion (3, 9, 13, 37). Evidence for a sex difference in pacing decisions comes from a study showing that marathon performances among non-elites tend to cluster at round numbers (e.g., just under 4 hours) due to some runners speeding up in the final 2.2 km of the race; this pattern was substantially more pronounced among men than women (2). There is also evidence that male distance runners are more likely to adopt a competitive rather than a recreational orientation towards the sport: 1) more male than female runners report that competition motivates them to run (6, 22, 29); 2) roughly three times as many men as women run fast relative to sex-specific world class standards, and this may reflect that that more men engage in the training necessary for faster performances (11); and 3) when men and women have the option of entering a single-sex competitive road race or a single-sex non-competitive road race, men are more likely than women to select the competitive race (15). In addition, there is much evidence for sex differences in risk taking in other contexts, including health, recreation, and finance (5, 10, 18).
Does the sex difference narrow among faster runners?
Another interesting question is whether the sex difference in pacing is lesser among faster runners. Addressing this question is important because it could provide clues about the cause(s) of the sex difference. Within financial domains, for example, studies report that the sex difference in risk taking substantially weakens or disappears when financial knowledge is controlled (12, 17, 23).
In the present study, despite the large overall sample size, there was very little data on genuinely elite or even near-elite runners. Moreover, faster runners, both men and women, generally maintain even paces (Table 2; Fig. 4A). These considerations notwithstanding, we did have some relevant results, although they provide an ambiguous answer to the question of whether the sex difference in pacing narrows among faster runners. On the one hand, the sex difference in pacing occurred among all finishing time groups (Table 2; Fig. 1), and among runners in our two fastest groups (men running faster than 3:00 or 3:30; women running faster than 3:22 or 3:55), the magnitude of the sex difference based on categorical variables (i.e., odds-ratios) was similar to the sex difference across all finishing time groups (Table 2; Fig. 1). On the other hand, when using continuous measures, the significant interaction of sex with adjusted finishing time indicates that the sex effect was significantly less pronounced in faster runners (Fig. 4A). Specifically, we estimated that among faster runners (3hr 00 min male finisher and corresponding 3 hr 26 min female finisher), men slowed, on average 25% more than women, whereas among slower runners (5 hr 00 min male finisher and corresponding 5 hr 36 min female finisher), men slowed, on average, 30% more than women.
We note that there have been several studies of pacing among elite men and women in other endurance sports. These typically indicate no pacing differences (28) or else subtle differences that are difficult to interpret due to sex-specific aspects of the competition, such as the depth of competitors (37).
Are women more effective pacers?
There is consensus that even (or nearly even) pacing is best for optimizing performance in races that take several hours to complete, such as the marathon (1). Furthermore, dramatic pace slowing in the marathon is associated with considerable discomfort (4, 33). Our results therefore imply that women are generally more effective than men in their marathon pacing. Nonetheless, this conclusion should be considered provisional because an evenly paced race does not automatically indicate a well-paced one. This is because an individual may be overly conservative given their training and ability. Similarly, although dramatically slowing in a race almost certainly indicates a poor outcome for that race, a risky initial pace may be desirable in a broader context. For instance, if a runner’s goal is to achieve at least one exceptional performance during the year (e.g., setting a personal record, winning their age group), they may have the best chance of achieving it if they often begin their races with a fast, risky pace. Thus, a definitive answer to question of whether particular groups or individuals are better pacers requires a comprehensive assessment of goals.
Insights from non-elites
Much has been gained by studying the pacing of elite and sub-elite endurance athletes (1), and investigating the pacing of non-elite or recreational athletes may provide further insights. For example, besides the sex difference, our study confirms that older runners and runners with faster initial paces tend to pace more evenly (26), and, to our knowledge, none of these effects were predicted by studies of elite pacing. Moreover, the variation in pacing among non-elites shown in our study (Fig. 4A) seems far greater than among elites (13). This is probably due to the tremendous diversity of non-elites’ goals, training, and abilities (2, 11). We suggest that future studies of pacing in non-elites assess these factors and others (e.g., coaching, fluid and carbohydrate ingestion, use of pace groups, and targeted pace) in order to illuminate the interactions among physiological and psychological factors in pacing. This research would seem of interest to many scientists and perhaps also to the public. For example, although marathon training handbooks, magazines, and websites generally contain sound advice, their discussions of pacing might be improved considerably as the factors related to uneven pacing are identified.
Limitations
The current study is not without limitations. First, we evaluated pacing only by assessing first and second half split times, so we were unable to model when the slowing typically began. Other studies indicate that slowing becomes pronounced in the final 10–15 km of the marathon (4, 13, 26). Second, we did not account for pacing variation associated with race courses or weather conditions, and this variation may be substantial (13, 35). Third, although we succeeded in identifying experience factors associated with pacing variation, other experience factors that we did not assess may also be important. Similarly, as noted above, we had no individual data on other relevant factors, including training, carbohydrate ingestion, and targeted pace. Fourth, some runners may have participated in more than one race in our sample. We were unable to “cluster” performances under a given participant, and this could have led to confidence intervals being smaller than they would be if clustering was modeled explicitly.
Conclusions
This study demonstrates that the sex difference in pacing among non-elite marathon runners is robust and greater among slower runners. It occurs consistently across marathons, persists across age groups and ability levels, and remains when experience is controlled and when women’s performances are adjusted to address men’s greater VO2max. The causes of the sex difference in pacing are presently unknown, but they likely involve the interplay of physiological and psychological factors. Studying this sex difference and related pacing phenomena should yield performance insights for scientists, coaches, and athletes.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Tyler Esser, Joseph Fleming, Alyssa Stevens, Jonathon Senefeld and Marissa Miars for assistance with data collection.
This publication was partially supported by Grant Number UL1 TR000135 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.
The results of the present study do not constitute endorsement by the American College of Sports Medicine.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Abbiss CR, Laursen PB. Describing and understanding pacing strategies during athletic competition. Sports Med. 2008;38(3):239–252. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200838030-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Allen EJ, Dechow PM. The “rationality” of the long distance runner: Prospect theory and the marathon. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network; 2013. [cited 2013 Dec 15]. Available from: http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2342396. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Brooks GA. Lactate: Link between glycolytic and oxidative metabolism. Sports Med. 2007;37(4–5):341–343. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200737040-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Buman MP, Brewer BW, Cornelius AE, Van Raalte JL, Petitpas AJ. Hitting the wall in the marathon: Phenomenological characteristics and associations with expectancy, gender, and running history. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2008;9(2):177–190. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Byrnes JP, Miller DC, Schafer WD. Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1999;125(3):367–383. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Callen KE. Mental and emotional aspects of long-distance running. Psychosomatics. 1983;24(2):133–151. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(83)73239-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Carter SL, Rennie C, Tarnopolsky MA. Substrate utilization during endurance exercise in men and women after endurance training. Am. J. Physiol.-Endocrinol. Metab. 2001;280(6):E898–E907. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.2001.280.6.E898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cheuvront SN, Carter R, DeRuisseau KC, Moffatt RJ. Running performance differences between men and women: An update. Sports Med. 2005;35(12):1017–1024. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200535120-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Coyle EF. Physiological regulation of marathon performance. Sports Med. 2007;37(4–5):306–311. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200737040-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Croson R, Gneezy U. Gender differences in preferences. J. Econ. Lit. 2009;47(2):448–474. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Deaner RO. Distance running as an ideal domain for showing a sex difference in competitiveness. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2013;42(3):413–428. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9965-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dwyer PD, Gilkeson JH, List JA. Gender differences in revealed risk taking: Evidence from mutual fund investors. Econ. Lett. 2002;76(2):151–158. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ely MR, Martin DE, Cheuvront SN, Montain SJ. Effect of ambient temperature on marathon pacing is dependent on runner ability. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2008;40(9):1675–1680. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181788da9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gagnon D, Kenny GP. Does sex have an independent effect on thermoeffector responses during exercise in the heat? J. Physiol.-Lond. 2012;590(23):5963–5973. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.240739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Garratt RJ, Weinberger C, Johnson N. The State Street Mile: Age and gender differences in competition aversion in the field. Econ. Inq. 2013;51(1):806–815. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gonzalez-Alonso J, Crandall CG, Johnson JA. The cardiovascular challenge of exercising in the heat. J. Physiol.-Lond. 2008;586(1):45–53. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.142158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Halko M-L, Kaustia M, Alanko E. The gender effect in risky asset holdings. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2012;83(1):66–81. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Harris CR, Jenkins M, Glaser D. Gender differences in risk assessment: Why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgm. Decis. Mak. J. 2006;1(1):48–63. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hunter SK, Stevens AA, Magennis K, Skelton KW, Fauth M. Is there a sex difference in the age of elite marathon runners? Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2011;43(4):656–664. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181fb4e00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hunter SK, Stevens AA. Sex differences in marathon running with advanced age: Physiology or participation? Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2013;45(1):148–156. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31826900f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hunter SK. Sex differences in human fatigability: Mechanisms and insight into physiological responses. Acta Physiol. 2014;10(4):768–789. doi: 10.1111/apha.12234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Johnsgard K. The motivation of the long-distance runner: 1. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness. 1985;25(3):135–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Johnson JE, Powell PL. Decision making, risk and gender: Are managers different? Br. J. Manag. 1994;5(2):123–128. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Joyner MJ. Physiological limiting factors and distance running: influence of gender and age on record performance. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 1993;21:103–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lamont LS. Gender differences in amino acid use during endurance exercise. Nutr. Rev. 2005;63(12):419–422. doi: 10.1301/nr.2005.dec.419-422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.March DS, Vanderburgh PM, Titlebaum PJ, Hoops ML. Age, sex, and finish time as determinants of pacing in the marathon. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2011;25(2):386–391. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181bffd0f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Maughan RJ, Watson P, Shirreffs SM. Heat and cold: What does the environment do to the marathon runner? Sports Med. 2007;37(4–5):396–399. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200737040-00032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Muehlbauer T, Panzer S, Schindler C. Pacing pattern and speed skating performance in competitive long-distance events. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2010;24(1):114–119. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181c6a04a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ogles BM, Masters KS. A typology of marathon runners based on cluster analysis of motivations. J. Sport Behav. 2003;26(1):69–85. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rapoport BI. Metabolic factors limiting performance in marathon runners. [cited 2013 Dec 18];PLoS Comput. Biol. 2010 6(10):e1000960. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Roepstorff C, Steffensen CH, Madsen M, et al. Gender differences in substrate utilization during submaximal exercise in endurance-trained subjects. Am. J. Physiol.-Endocrinol. Metab. 2002;282(2):E435–E447. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00266.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sparling PB, O’Donnell EM, Snow TK. The gender difference in distance running performance has plateaued: An analysis of world rankings from 1980 to 1996. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1998;30(12):1725–1729. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199812000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Stevinson CD, Biddle SJH. Cognitive orientations in marathon running and “hitting the wall.”. Br. J. Sports Med. 1998;32(3):229–234. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.32.3.229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tarnopolsky MA. Sex differences in exercise metabolism and the role of 17-beta estradiol. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2008;40(4):648–654. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31816212ff. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Trubee NW. The effects of age, sex, heat stress, and finish time on pacing in the marathon [Internet] [cited 2013 Jan 4];2011 Available from: http://etd.ohiolink.edu/view.cgi/Trubee%20Nicholas.pdf?dayton1303833182. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Venables MC, Achten J, Jeukendrup AE. Determinants of fat oxidation during exercise in healthy men and women: a cross-sectional study. J. Appl. Physiol. 2005;98(1):160–167. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00662.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Vleck VE, Bentley DJ, Millet GP, Buergi A. Pacing during an elite Olympic distance triathlon: Comparison between male and female competitors. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2008;11(4):424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2007.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]