Abstract
This overview attempts to synthesise current understandings of the neuroendocrine basis of parenting. The parent–infant bond is central to the human condition, contributes to risks for mood and anxiety disorders, and provides the potential for resiliency and protection against the development of psychopathology. Animal models of parenting provide compelling evidence that biological mechanisms may be studied in humans. This has led to brain imaging and endocrine system studies of human parents using baby stimuli and concerted psychological and behavioural measures. Certain brain circuits and related hormonal systems, including subcortical regions for motivation (striatum, amygdala, hypothalamus and hippocampus) and cortical regions for social cognition (anterior cingulate, insula, medial frontal and orbitofrontal cortices), appear to be involved. These brain circuits work with a range of endocrine systems to manage stress and motivate appropriate parental caring behaviour with a flexibility appropriate to the environment. Work in this field promises to link evolving models of parental brain performance with resilience, risk and treatment toward mother–infant mental health.
Keywords: parent–child relationships, brain imaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging, attachment, parenting, caregiving, cortisol, dopamine, opioids
Introduction
Parenting is critical for an infant’s survival and development across mammalian species. Despite increased levels of stress as a result of physiological, emotional and economical demands from the baby (1), parents typically find themselves highly motivated to take care of their infants’ needs and also find the interactions with infants rewarding (2,3). The present review draws upon recent human and animal brain research using infant stimuli to examine psychological and biological aspects of maternal motivations and behaviours. Using the example of selected maternal brain and endocrine responses to the hunger cry of an infant, we examine the modulatory roles of dopamine (DA), oxytocin (OT), cortisol (CORT) and endogenous opioids (EO). Thus, we propose to build on current biobehavioural parental brain models (4,5) for a simplified and integrated homeostatic neuroendocrine model.
Neuroimaging methodology primer
Functional neuroimaging methods relevant to the present review include magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and positron emission tomography (PET). MRI techniques combine non-invasive magnetic fields and radiofrequency signals for millimetre resolution. Voxel-based morphometry detects changes in brain structure as a result of development or the environment. Functional MRI (fMRI) involves blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) measurements that reflect changes in neural activity over a time resolution of seconds.
Typically, BOLD signals during different events are measured throughout the brain. Subsequent analyses identify brain regions that show greater activity in response to a condition of interest (e.g. own-baby-cry versus other-baby-cry) and allow correlation of such brain activity with measures of parental behaviours. This technique can also be carried out in rats using such stimuli as nipple suckling, along with direct neurohormone sampling. PET represents another functional brain imaging technique for humans, and measures certain receptor-specific changes in radioactive tracers injected into the bloodstream but with the caveats of radiation exposure and longer minute-scale temporal resolution.
Several caveats of current brain imaging must be considered. First, the BOLD signals of the fMRI data are indirect measures of neural activity, such that the data are susceptible to non-neural changes in the participant’s body and environment. Second, imaging data provide information on the association, rather than causality, between brain and behaviour. Third, because of the limitations of space and movement in the scanner, studies must use simple tasks and stimuli to simulate the performance of parenting behaviours. Finally, differences in parenting brain function are subject to many factors that are not typically controlled, including early-life experiences (6) and postpartum timing and environment (7–9), as is the case for hormonal measures in parents (10,11).
Neuroendocrinology of parenting: responses to baby-cry
An archetypal parenting scenario for a new parent involves perceiving baby-cry, approaching baby, appraising the situation, determining needs such as milk, providing appropriate care such as breastfeeding, and some resolution. We suggest a neuroendocrinological model of parenting, with initial engagement of DA and OT systems, CORT to regulate the closely following stress response, and EO systems for subsequent parenting and reward (Fig. 1). Given the significant gaps in the human neuroendocrine parenting literature, although roles for OT and CORT are relatively well established, more studies are needed to clarify specific roles for DA and EO.
Fig. 1.
An integrated neuroendocrine model of parenting. From left to right in time, the baby stimulus, behavioural response, brain activity and associated hormonal shifts of dopamine, endogenous opioids. No strict timeline for events is given. During baby-cry, dopamine may be the first to rise to assist with arousal, motivation and decision-making circuits, including striatum and amygdala. Oxytocin also rises relatively quickly with hypothalamus brain activity to support milk let-down and promote parenting behaviours. Cortisol (CORT) increases with a slower time course to support stress responses and prepare the mother for demanding behaviour depending on the reason for baby-cry and other circumstances. Assuming a simple hunger cry, CORT will start to drop first, with oxytocin next after it has performed lactation-supporting anxiety-reducing functions. Finally, the parent–infant interaction requires regulatory cortical regions of anterior cingulate, medial frontal and orbitofrontal cortices that are modulated by opioids to mediate satisfaction and reinforce caring behaviour. This may be paired with a proposed second rise in dopamine to mediate reinforcement, learning and any extended play or other dyadic interactions. Abnormalities in these neuroendocrine systems, such as in mood, anxiety or substance abuse disorders, contribute to the impaired orchestration of these parental neuroendocrine systems. DA, dopamine; EO, endogenous opioids; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; OT, oxytocin.
Neuroendocrine modulation of parenting motivation and attention
Animal studies using, for example, lesions, electrophysiology and neuroendocrine contributions to behaviours have documented maternal brain circuits that include the hypothalamus, amygdala, striatum, cingulate and prefrontal cortex (12). Such work has encouraged human neuroimaging work with parallel studies, beginning with auditory baby-cues to activate human parental brain circuits. Lorberbaum et al. (13) were the first group to study brain activity in mothers, in which they listened to other-baby-cries, highlighting the importance of a thalamocingulate circuit for an emotional response and regulation. Subsequently, researchers have identified a series of hypothalamic-midbrain-limbic-paralimbic-cortical brain circuits that are selectively activated during times when the maternal brain is ‘switched-on’ by baby-cries (4).
During the early postpartum period, increased levels of anxiety and worries in new parents, typically about their infant’s safety and health, may include thoughts of reciprocity, unity and even idealised perfection of the infant, which may elevate anxiety levels (14–16). Perhaps healthy anxiety-producing thoughts that lead to frequent checking are related to parenting and infant development, (17) via brain systems under the influence of DA (18). In several human fMRI studies, responses to own- versus other-baby stimuli of first-time mothers during this early postpartum period show increased brain activity in several regions, including the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), midbrain, striatum, cingulate, amygdala and insula (4,5,8,9,19,20). In addition to mediating DA-related anxiety and habitual behaviour and learning (21,22), some of these brain regions also serve to focus attention and promote DA-dependent attention and decision-making (23). For example, baby-cry must be analysed to determine whether it is calling for protection or food. Through reinforcement learning, action–outcome contingencies in response to baby-cries and associated environments can be optimised (24,25). Thus, in Fig. 1, we suggest that dopamine increase is among the first key neuroendocrine responses to baby-cry to focus attention and drive parental behaviour.
Neuroendocrine modulation of suckling and lactation
Provided that the baby-cry calls for milk, the mother may quickly position the infant to breastfeed. Pup-suckling triggers OT, which mediates milk ejection (26). Brain imaging in rats when pup-suckling shows activations in the paraventricular nucleus of hypothalamus, olfactory tubercle, anterior olfactory nucleus, piriform cortex, amygdala, medial preoptic area, ventral tegmentum area, insular cortex and prefrontal cortex (27). Furthermore, exogenous OT administration decreases fear behaviours of lactating rats, at the same time as increasing brain activity in emotion and cognition regulation regions, such as the anterior cingulate, and decreasing brain activity in autonomic, visceromotor and skeletomotor areas enhances neural processing in (and also modulates) emotion regulation brain circuits (28).
In humans, OT plays key roles in social bonding in general (29), as well as specific aspects of maternal brain function (30), including: (i) vaginal delivery (31); (ii) breastfeeding (32); and (iii) parental sensitivity (33–36). First, human mothers who gave birth by vaginal versus caesarean delivery were more sensitive to own- versus other-baby-cry in emotion regulation and limbic brain regions at 2–4 weeks postpartum (37). Furthermore, in response to own-baby-cry, maternal brain activity correlated with depressive symptoms in the left frontal cortex, which is a region of importance to mood dysregulation in psychiatric illness (38). This supports the controversial link between caesarean delivery and depression risk in mothers that may involve OT (39). Second, breastfeeding mothers were reported to be more sensitive to their own infants cry compared to formula-feeding mothers at 2–4 weeks postpartum (40) in emotion response and regulation brain regions, including the superior frontal gyrus, insula, precuneus, striatum and amygdala. Finally, brain activity in the superior frontal gyrus and amygdala predict maternal sensitivity at 3–4 months postpartum among these mothers (40). This fits the findings that intranasal administration of OT increases the links between social memories and reward in mothers (41), increases infant-directed sensitive play behaviours (36). Furthermore, intranasal oxytocin seems to both decrease anxiety and increase empathy circuit activity in parents responding to baby cry (42).
These studies suggest that OT may enhance parenting motivation by enhancing positive emotions and care-related brain circuits, at the same time as also attenuating negative fear and aversion circuits. This may be similar to the way in which exposure to baby faces is associated with maternal hypothalamus activation that correlates with serum OT (43) to contribute to increased reward circuitry and decrease negative social assessment circuits in maternal brains (44). This is also agreement with decreasing amygdala activation to fearful faces in healthy (45), as well as anxious (46), humans with OT administration.
Further work is required to understand the coordinated responses of structures such as the insula and amygdala (47) with oxytocin (48), which may together orchestrate competing systems toward sensitive baby-care in the initial phases of the baby-cry response. In Fig. 1, we show OT rising soon after baby-cry to regulate milk let-down and related parenting behaviours during the potentially stressful episode.
Neuroendocrine modulation parental stress
Prolonged exposure to baby-cry is stressful, placing demands on the mother to act, and triggers the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (49). Subsequent CORT has a complex association with maternal behaviours in humans (50). During the first few months postpartum in normal mothers, high salivary CORT levels relate to responsive behaviours, positive attitude and vocalisation toward infants during the mother–infant interaction (51). Accordingly, high CORT levels are also found among new mothers who rated infant odours more favourably (50).However, at 6 months postpartum, maternal CORT is inversely associated with sensitive maternal behaviours, including gaze to infant’s face and vocalisation toward infants and OT (33,52). Along the same lines, chronically-elevated CORT levels as a result of high early-life stress may interrupt the activation of maternal brain circuits in response to infant cues. Indeed, high CORT levels among mothers who had frequent changes in their caregivers as children were associated with negative emotions and less responsive parenting (10).
Cortisol is also in complex relationships with OT-related responses. When suckling rapidly, adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) and corticosteroid secretion is stimulated in rats (53), On the other hand, plasma CORT decreases in human mothers (54), potentially through the suckling-induced OT (55) that can inhibit ACTH secretion (56). In Fig. 1, we suggest that CORT will rise after dopamine to assist with the stress responses needed for immediate concerns, as well as extended periods of caring behaviours.
Neuroendocrine modulation of post-cry parental care
Once baby-cry ceases, and lactation has occurred, the infant–mother dyad may enjoy a moment of rewarding happiness. In animal models, components of the reward system are critical for parenting. This can be probed in humans by showing happy baby visual stimuli in human neuroimaging studies. Indeed, human mothers showed a greater activity of response in the midbrain, periaqueductal grey and striatum, OFC, medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula and precuneus to their own- versus other-baby pictures (20,44) or videos (19,57). Furthermore, orbitofrontal cortex responses to own-baby visual stimuli correlate with ratings of pleasant mood, excitement and love that mothers may experience when viewing the stimuli (19,58).
These hedonic maternal response and behaviour-mediating brain circuits behaviours may be modulated by EO in a narrow physiological range by providing satisfaction feedback to augment reinforcement (59). Furthermore, DA (as discussed above) and EO reward systems may subserve dissociable components of rewards that may be applicable for understanding the maternal brain: a behavioural reinforcement learning component (‘wanting’) and a hedonic experience component (‘liking’), respectively (60). In an animal study, microinjections of an opioid-stimulating drug to nucleus accumbens increased the hedonic liking impact of sucrose on behaviour and incentive salience signals in firing to the cue associated with the reward, although they did not alter firing signals to the learned reward prediction signals as part of reinforcement learning; conversely, microinjection of a DA-stimulating drug enhanced only the motivation component but did not alter hedonic impact or the learned prediction signal (61). Thus, the EO system may play a central role in the hedonic experience of a mother who, perhaps after breastfeeding, sees her baby smile, such that subsequent sensitive interactions may then be reinforced.
Although the association between the EO and maternal brain responses remains unknown in humans using direct hormone measurements, neuroimaging studies suggest that central opioid system activations (62) overlap with central maternal activations (4,5) in the mesocorticolimbic circuitry. Using PET techniques, μ-opioid neurotransmission induced by the analgesic cognitive expectations, known as placebo effects, are enhanced in the orbital frontal cortex, dorsal and rostral anterior cingulate cortex, nucleus accumbems and midbrain (63,64). Furthermore, analgesic placebo effects are mediated by the co-activation of DA and opioid neurotransmission in the ventral striatum (including the nucleus accumbens) (63), highlighting the potential interactions between DA and EO in the mesolimbic reward circuitry.
Additionally, EO contributes to maternal euphoria and the inhibition of stress-related HPA reactivity to protect the foetus in the late-gestation (65) and lactation phases (65). Overall, EO serve as mediator of social rewards, affiliation, motivation and attenuation of separation-induced pain and anxiety (66), such that a phenomenological resemblance between narcotic addiction and social attachment has been proposed: both display an initial attachment phase, a tolerance-development phase and similar symptoms upon withdrawal (67). This is consistent with the idea that exogenous opiates may commandeer maternal motivation to seek and care for their baby (68). Indeed, the sensitive and powerful interactions between EO and DA in central reward circuitry render narcotic addictions very difficult to treat, potentially by intertwining addiction with primal social attachment needs. In Fig. 1, we illustrate EO as rising with the satisfaction of attending to baby, presuming the cessation of baby-cry, breastfeeding behaviours. We speculate that EO levels may remain elevated to support the pro-social sensitive parenting behaviours that may follow.
Summary, discussion and new directions
Human parent brain responses to baby stimuli were reviewed to be largely in agreement and to build upon largely subcortical animal models, including reward circuits (midbrain, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, OFC, anterior cingulate cortex, insula) and social cognition regions (medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal gyrus, OFC). A selection of the emerging complex literature is discussed to support modulatory roles for the key neurohormones (DA, OT, CORT and EO). Evidence for the importance for OT and CORT in human parental response to baby-cry is well established in human mothers. However, there are many gaps in the literature with respect to DA and EO in humans. Future approaches combining brain imaging and endocrine measurements (e.g. plasma or salivary CORT and OT) and/or acute endocrine manipulation [e.g. exogenous CORT and OT (intranasal OT spray)] may reveal how these hormones interact with each other to modulate parental brain and behaviour. In addition, more ethologically valid stimuli are required to better simulate a real parenting situation, such as interactive audiovisual stimuli that may be personally tailored or involve decision-making, and such studies may prove to be enlightening.
Also, joint consideration of brain circuit and endocrine regulation may be particularly important for understanding special populations of parents, such as mothers under the influence of exogenous endocrine modulators or suffering from endocrine dysregulation that comprises part of a psychiatric condition. For example, mothers with substance abuse problems may have alterations in their brain circuitry and EO systems (69) that render them vulnerable to maladaptive maternal behaviours in the post-partum period. This could be studied in a somewhat controlled fashion in women with opiate addictions who are treated with controlled doses of methadone or buprenorphine maintenance to prevent miscarriage, with the aim of understanding how these substances affect striatum-dependent maternal responses, reward and affiliation processes, as well as how any adverse effects can be mitigated.
Thus, combining brain imaging approaches with psychological measures and interventions in parents may begin to address significant public health issues of other peripartum mental health problems, such as postpartum depression, anxiety and low socioeconomic status, by developing specific mechanistic models that could provide more effective management strategies. In the first study of brain activity in clinically depressed mothers, frontal executive function regions were less sensitive to a standard face-matching task (70). Furthermore, there was an inverse relationship between depression and the amygdala response to negative faces, as well as a significantly reduced left dorsomedial prefrontal cortical face-related activity in response to fear and anger faces. Also, very recently and in the first study of clinically depressed mothers using own-baby-cry, the brain response was essentially eliminated by depression in a distributed network of paralimbic and prefrontal regions, which displayed an inverse correlation between response and depression severity, including function in prefrontal areas and the striatum (71). Taken together with known variations in frontal cortex activity in mothers as a function of depression symptoms (37), it appears that postpartum depression affects the cortical–amygdala connections that are important for mood regulation and motivation, thus impairing parenting. Neuroendocrine brain systems under the influence of anxiety and other adverse circumstances around parenthood, such as poverty (72,73), also remain to be studied.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Institute for Research on Unlimited Love (http://unlimitedloveinstitute.org); the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (http://narsad.org); the Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation (http://ktgf.org); Yale Center for Risk, Resilience, and Recovery, Associates of the Yale Child Study Center; the Department of Psychiatry of the University of Michigan, MICHR High Tech grant UL1RR024986; the Science of Generosity Initiative (http://generosityresearch.nd.edu/); and the NICHD (IRC2MD004767-01). Drs P. Kim and S. S. Ho contributed to this work equally. Dr J. E. Swain would especially like to acknowledge the inestimable collegial support of many individuals, including Drs Ruth Feldman, James F. Leckman Jeffrey Lorberbaum, Linda C. Mayes, Robert T. Schultz and Lane Strathearn.
References
- 1.Barclay L, Everitt L, Rogan F, Schmied V, Wyllie A. Becoming a mother – an analysis of women’s experience of early motherhood. J Adv Nurs. 1997;25:719–728. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.t01-1-1997025719.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mercer RT. The process of maternal role attainment over the first year. Nurs Res. 1985;34:198–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Goodman JH. Becoming an involved father of an infant. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2002;34:190–200. doi: 10.1177/0884217505274581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Swain JE. The human parental brain: in vivo neuroimaging. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1242–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barrett J, Fleming AS. Annual research review: all mothers are not created equal: neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:368–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kim P, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Newman MA, Feldman R, Swain JE. Perceived quality of maternal care in childhood and structure and function of mothers’ brain. Dev Sci. 2010;13:662–673. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00923.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim P, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Wang X, Swain JE. The plasticity of human maternal brain: longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:695–700. doi: 10.1037/a0020884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Swain JE, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Hoyt E, Kang H, Kim P, David D, Nguyen S, Constable RT, Schultz RT. Functional brain activations of parents listening to their own baby-cry change over the early postpartum. Dev Psychobiol. 2011 In Press. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Swain JE, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Constable RT, Schultz RT. The neural circuitry of parent-infant attachment in the early postpartum. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology; Puerto Rico: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Krpan KM, Coombs R, Zinga D, Steiner M, Fleming AS. Experiential and hormonal correlates of maternal behavior in teen and adult mothers. Horm Behav. 2005;47:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Gonzalez A, Jenkins JM, Steiner M, Fleming AS. The relation between early life adversity, cortisol awakening response and diurnal salivary cortisol levels in postpartum women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Newman JD. Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals. Behav Brain Res. 2007;182:155–165. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lorberbaum JP, Newman JD, Horwitz AR, Dubno JR, Lydiard RB, Hamner MB, Bohning DE, George MS. A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:431–445. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01284-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Feldman R, Weller A, Leckman JF, Kuint J, Eidelman AI. The nature of the mother’s tie to her infant: maternal bonding under conditions of proximity, separation, and potential loss. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40:929–939. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Leckman JF, Mayes LC. Preoccupations and behaviors associated with romantic and parental love. Perspectives on the origin of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 1999;8:635–665. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Evans DW, King RA, Cohen DJ. Early parental preoccupations and behaviors and their possible relationship to the symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1999;396:1–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10951.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kim P, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Leckman JF, Swain JE. Primary parental preoccupation and the transition from adulthood to parenthood. Infant Mental Health J. 2011 In Press. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Leckman JF, Feldman R, Swain JE, Eicher V, Thompson N, Mayes LC. Primary parental preoccupation: circuits, genes, and the crucial role of the environment. J Neural Transm. 2004;111:753–771. doi: 10.1007/s00702-003-0067-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Noriuchi M, Kikuchi Y, Senoo A. The functional neuroanatomy of maternal love: mother’s response to infant’s attachment behaviors. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Strathearn L, Li J, Fonagy P, Montague PR. What’s in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics. 2008;122:40–51. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ashby FG, Turner BO, Horvitz JC. Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nikolaus S, Antke C, Beu M, Muller HW. Cortical GABA, striatal dopamine and midbrain serotonin as the key players in compulsive and anxiety disorders – results from in vivo imaging studies. Rev Neurosci. 2010;21:119–139. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2010.21.2.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schultz W. Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: basic and recent data. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:24. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Daw ND, Gershman SJ, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron. 2011;69:1204–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brunton PJ, Russell JA. The expectant brain: adapting for motherhood. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:11–25. doi: 10.1038/nrn2280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Febo M, Numan M, Ferris CF. Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother-pup bonding during suckling. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11637–11644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3604-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Febo M, Shields J, Ferris CF, King JA. Oxytocin modulates unconditioned fear response in lactating dams: an fMRI study. Brain Res. 2009;1302:183–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ross HE, Young LJ. Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30:534–547. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Leng G, Meddle SL, Douglas AJ. Oxytocin and the maternal brain. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2008;8:731–734. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kendrick KM. Oxytocin, motherhood and bonding. Exp Physiol. 2000;85:111S–124S. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-445x.2000.tb00014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sala NL, Althabe O. The milk ejecting effect induced by oxytocin during human lactation. Acta Physiol Lat Am. 1968;18:88–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Feldman R, Weller A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Levine A. Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:965–970. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Feldman R, Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O. The cross-generation transmission of oxytocin in humans. Horm Behav. 2010;58:669–676. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R. Oxytocin, cortisol, and triadic family interactions. Physiol Behav. 2010;101:679–684. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Naber F, van Ijzendoorn MH, Deschamps P, van Engeland H, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed responsiveness during play with their children: a double-blind within-subject experiment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1583–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Swain JE, Tasgin E, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Constable RT, Leckman JF. Maternal brain response to own baby-cry is affected by cesarean section delivery. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1042–1052. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01963.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Moise D, Madhusoodanan S. Psychiatric symptoms associated with brain tumors: a clinical enigma. CNS Spectr. 2006;11:28–31. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900024135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Skrundz M, Bolten M, Nast I, Hellhammer DH, Meinlschmidt G. Plasma oxytocin concentration during pregnancy is associated with development of postpartum depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:1886–1893. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kim P, Feldman R, Mayes LC, Eicher V, Thompson N, Leckman JF, Swain JE. Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:907–915. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02406.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bartz JA, Zaki J, Ochsner KN, Bolger N, Kolevzon A, Ludwig N, Lydon JE. Effects of oxytocin on recollections of maternal care and closeness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:21371–21375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012669107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Riem MM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Pieper S, Tops M, Boksem MA, Vermeiren RR, van Ijzendoorn MH, Rombouts SA. Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: a randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Strathearn L, Fonagy P, Amico J, Montague PR. Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2655–2666. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bartels A, Zeki S. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1155–1166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kirsch P, Esslinger C, Chen Q, Mier D, Lis S, Siddhanti S, Gruppe H, Mattay VS, Gallhofer B, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11489–11493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3984-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Labuschagne I, Phan KL, Wood A, Angstadt M, Chua P, Heinrichs M, Stout JC, Nathan PJ. Oxytocin attenuates amygdala reactivity to fear in generalized social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2403–2413. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Swain JE, Konrath S, Brown S, Finegood E, Akce LB, Dayton CJ, Ho SS. Parenting and beyond: common neurocircuits underlying parental and altruistic caregiving. Parent SciPract. 2011 doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.680409. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr Rev. 2000;21:55–89. doi: 10.1210/edrv.21.1.0389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fleming AS, Steiner M, Corter C. Cortisol, hedonics, and maternal responsiveness in human mothers. Horm Behav. 1997;32:85–98. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1997.1407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fleming AS, Steiner M, Anderson V. Hormonal and attitudinal correlates of maternal behavior during the early postparpregnancy. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 1987;5:5193–5205. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R. Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:377–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Walker CD, Lightman SL, Steele MK, Dallman MF. Suckling is a persistent stimulus to the adrenocortical system of the rat. Endocrinology. 1992;130:115–125. doi: 10.1210/endo.130.1.1309321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Amico JA, Johnston JM, Vagnucci AH. Suckling-induced attenuation of plasma cortisol concentrations in postpartum lactating women. Endocr Res. 1994;20:79–87. doi: 10.3109/07435809409035858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Chiodera P, Salvarani C, Bacchi-Modena A, Spallanzani R, Cigarini C, Alboni A, Gardini E, Coiro V. Relationship between plasma profiles of oxytocin and adrenocorticotropic hormone during suckling or breast stimulation in women. Horm Res. 1991;35:119–123. doi: 10.1159/000181886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Legros JJ. Inhibitory effect of oxytocin on corticotrope function in humans: are vasopressin and oxytocin ying-yang neurohormones? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:649–655. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00018-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ranote S, Elliott R, Abel KM, Mitchell R, Deakin JF, Appleby L. The neural basis of maternal responsiveness to infants: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2004;15:1825–1829. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000137078.64128.6a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Nitschke JB, Nelson EE, Rusch BD, Fox AS, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ. Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. Neuroimage. 2004;21:583–592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Panksepp J, Nelson E, Siviy S. Brain opioids and mother-infant social motivation. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 1994;397:40–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13264.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Smith KS, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW. Disentangling pleasure from incentive salience and learning signals in brain reward circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:E255–E264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101920108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zubieta JK, Stohler CS. Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1156:198–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04424.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:220–231. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Zubieta JK, Bueller JA, Jackson LR, Scott DJ, Xu Y, Koeppe RA, Nichols TE, Stohler CS. Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on mu-opioid receptors. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7754–7762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0439-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Brunton PJ, Russell JA, Douglas AJ. Adaptive responses of the maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis during pregnancy and lactation. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20:764–776. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01735.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Nelson EE, Panksepp J. Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1998;22:437–452. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00052-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Panksepp J, Herman BH, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi FG. Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1980;4:473–487. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Swain JE, Mayes LC, Leckman JF. Endogenous and exogenous opiates modulate the development of parent–infant attachment. Behavior and Brain Sci. 2005;28:367–368. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Shi J, Zhao LY, Copersino ML, Fang YX, Chen Y, Tian J, Deng Y, Shuai Y, Jin J, Lu L. PET imaging of dopamine transporter and drug craving during methadone maintenance treatment and after prolonged abstinence in heroin users. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;579:160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Moses-Kolko EL, Perlman SB, Wisner KL, James J, Saul AT, Phillips ML. Abnormally reduced dorsomedial prefrontal cortical activity and effective connectivity with amygdala in response to negative emotional faces in postpartum depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:1373–1380. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Laurent HK, Ablow JC. A cry in the dark: depressed mothers show reduced neural activation to their own infant’s cry. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq091. Epub Ahead of Print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Feldman R, Granat A, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:919–927. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b21651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Biological and environmental initial conditions shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first years of life. Dev Sci. 2009;12:194–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00761.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]