Abstract
We tagged 82 lactating northern fur seals (Callorhinus ursinus) with tri-axial accelerometers and magnetometers on two eastern Bering Sea islands (Bogoslof and St. Paul) with contrasting population trajectories. Using depth data, accelerometer data and spectral analysis we classified time spent diving (30%), resting (~7%), shaking and grooming their pelage (9%), swimming in the prone position (~10%) and two types of previously undocumented rolling behavior (29%), with the remaining time (~15%) unspecified. The reason for the extensive rolling behavior is not known. We ground-truthed the accelerometry signals for shaking and grooming and rolling behaviors—and identified the acceleration signal for porpoising—by filming tagged northern fur seals in captivity. Speeds from GPS interpolated data indicated that animals traveled fastest while in the prone position, suggesting that this behavior is indicative of destination-based swimming. Very little difference was found in the percentages of time spent in the categorical behaviors with respect to breeding islands (Bogoslof or St. Paul Island), forager type (cathemeral or nocturnal), and the region where the animals foraged (primarily on-shelf <200m, or off-shelf > 200m). The lack of significant differences between islands, regions and forager type may indicate that behaviors summarized over a trip are somewhat hardwired even though foraging trip length and when and where animals dive are known to vary with island, forager type and region.
Introduction
Quantifying the time an animal spends in various activities such as locomotion, resting, and foraging can be used to estimate energy expenditure and understand life history strategies. In the case of northern fur seals (Callorhinus ursinus), determining activity budgets could help to explain why the population trajectories of two eastern Bering Sea populations differ. The population of fur seals on St. Paul Island (central Bering Sea) has declined since 1998 at an annual rate of 5.5%, while the population on Bogoslof Island (southern Bering Sea) has increased at an annual rate of 11.7% since 1997 [1–3]. Unfortunately, at-sea observations of fur seal behavior are difficult to obtain.
Telemetry devices carried by northern fur seals since the 1990’s have provided at-sea locations of individuals and recordings of swimming depths by time of day [4,5]. TDR (Time-Depth-Recording) data have also been augmented with acoustic data to differentiate behaviors (locomotion, diving, resting and surface activity) and flipper stroke rates associated with different types of dives [6]. Such telemetry has revealed some differences in foraging habitat and foraging behavior between lactating fur seals breeding at different sites [4,5,7–9]. Most notably, they have shown that fur seals from Bogoslof Island forage almost exclusively in deep basin waters with very little diving occurring during the day, while individuals from St. Paul Island forage over both the shelf and in deep basin waters [9,10]. In addition, they have revealed that some St. Paul animals make substantial numbers of dives during the day—a behavior that is highly associated with animals that forage over the continental shelf [10,11].
The current breakdown of at-sea behaviors of northern fur seals is relatively coarse, and fails to differentiate surface activities. One means to document more specific behaviors is by categorizing movements recorded by tri-axial accelerometers [12–19]. Identifying episodes of feeding, locomotion and other behaviors can be useful in basic physiological studies, bioenergetic modeling, as well as ecological studies identifying foraging hotspots, informing predator prey dynamics, and marine resource management [20–24].
Our study sought to expand the number of previously differentiable behaviors of northern fur seals by analyzing acceleration data recorded during foraging trips. Data were collected from lactating females from St. Paul and Bogoslof Islands using a biologging tag that included tri-axial accelerometers (Daily Diary tag, Wildlife Computers, Redmond, WA). Our goals were to 1) differentiate between common fur seal behaviors, 2) create an at-sea activity budget, and 3) make ecologically relevant comparisons both within and between the two island populations of northern fur seals. We also validated our interpretation of some of the accelerometry data from fur seals in the field by filming instrumented female fur seals in captivity.
Methods
Ethics Statement
All procedures were conducted under the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration permit no. 14329 and the protocol was approved by the Committee on Animal Care at the University of British Columbia (permit no. A09–0345).
Tagging
From July 10th to September 19th 2009, we tagged lactating northern fur seals on St. Paul Island (in the Pribilof archipelago) and on Bogoslof Island (a volcanic pinnacle south of the Pribilof Islands and just north of the Aleutian Islands chain) (Fig. 1). Fur seals were tagged with a combination of Wildlife Computers Mk10-F (with Fastloc GPS), first-generation Daily Diary tags, and VHF transmitters. Both Wildlife Computers tags are about the size of a deck of cards and were mounted dorsally along the centerline of the animal between the shoulder blades. Seals were recaptured between 3 and 28 days after deployment to recover the tags. Further details of the capture, recapture and tagging methodology can be found in Nordstrom et al. [10].
Behavior categorization
Our behavior data were collected using the Daily Diary tag. This tag had tri-axial accelerometers and magnetometers that recorded acceleration and the earth’s magnetic field in the 3 spatial dimensions (from the animal’s frame of reference, anterior-posterior (surge or x-axis), dorsal-ventral (heave or z-axis) and lateral (sway or y-axis)). It also recorded swimming depth, internal temperature of the tag, and conductivity to indicate wet/dry. The acceleration and magnetic field sensors were set to collect data at 16Hz, while the remaining sensors recorded every second (1Hz).
Behavioral analysis was conducted using the depth channel, the lateral accelerometer, and the dorso-ventral accelerometer. Behavioral analysis occurred in two stages. The first stage involved importing the data into Igor Pro wave analysis software (WaveMetrics). The Ethographer [14] software add-in was initially used to identify diving behavior based on dive depths. It was then used to identify commonly occurring, repeated signals in the lateral acceleration data using spectral analysis. These repeated signals (behaviors) were differentiated into 4 distinct movement patterns—subsequently identified as resting, shaking (also identified with pelage grooming) and two types of surface rolling (“sine wave” and “W”) (Fig. 2). Remaining time was placed into an undifferentiated “mixed” category. Analysis occurred over 1 minute bins, and the dominant behavior was assigned to each period. The second stage of the behavioral analysis involved importing the data into R [25] and visually examining the accelerometry data to identify a 6th behavioral category from the “mixed” category, which we termed “prone position” behavior.
“Diving”, the first behavior we differentiated, was identified using the depth channel and defined as excursions below a depth of 5m during the day and below a depth of 0 meters during the night. This definition was primarily designed to distinguish between diving that was most likely foraging, and diving that was most likely other sub surface behaviors (which vary between day and night). We defined night as beginning and ending at nautical twilight, which is a solar elevation angle of 6 degrees below the horizon. Given that night dives tend to be shallow and short, we assigned diving behavior to a given minute if the animal was below the surface for more than 25% of the time in that minute. This definition differs from our “dominant behavior” designation for all other behaviors, and underestimates if diving occurred during a minute but overestimates the overall time an animal was actually diving.
Myctophids are a deep-water species and prey item for northern fur seals from Bogoslof and St. Paul Island [26,27]. These species migrates vertically to the near-surface waters during the night, where it is consumed by fur seals and sea birds. Parades et al. [28] found myctophids to be present in ~90% of sampled Black-legged kittiwakes (Rissa tridactyla), which are plunge divers that are unable to dive below the top meter of water. It is thus possible for fur seals to feed on myctophids while making very short shallow dives—hence our need to establish criteria to capture this relatively brief but important foraging behavior.
We differentiated the remaining time into exclusive behavioral categories using the lateral and dorsal-ventral axes of accelerometer data. Raw data from the accelerometer channel was in mV, with a reading of ~10 mV roughly coinciding with the force of gravity at 9.8 m s-2. We defined resting behavior using a two-step criteria: 1) average lateral acceleration > 5 m s-2 or < -5 m s-2 with 2) a standard deviation < 3 m s-2. These conditions indicated that the fur seals were laying on either lateral side at the water surface and moving very little. Shaking behavior was defined when bursts of strong lateral acceleration (> ±25 m s-2) were recorded.
We identified and defined other behaviors in the accelerometer data based on commonly occurring waves with consistent amplitude and frequency. Frequency analysis can be used to identify cyclical oscillation waves. We thus extracted rolling behaviors from the acceleration data by employing a frequency analysis method based on continuous wavelet transformation with a Morlet mother wavelet as described by Sakamoto et al. [14]. This method enabled us to extract the times of cyclic oscillation that had specific periodicities. The spectrogram examined the periodicity in 8 steps corresponding to 1.0, 1.8, 3.2, 5.6, 10, 18, 32, and 56 seconds, and showed a strong signal between 2–3 seconds corresponding to the periodicity of the rolling behaviors. We then calculated the proportion of the sum of the signal intensity at the 1.8 and 3.2 second cycles to the sum of all cycles where a high proportion indicated that the shape of the acceleration signal was similar to a sine wave. We defined the basic sine wave rolling behavior when this proportion was more than 60%. We also calculated the sum of the signal intensity at the 1.8, 3.2, 5.6 and 10 second cycles and we defined the behavior as a W-roll when the sum of these proportions was greater than 70%. K-means clustering (an unsupervised classification algorithm) was then used to aggregate the behavior spectra over each second by minimizing the within-cluster sum of squared Euclidean distances from the cluster (behavior) centroids, thereby determining the dominant behavior.
While we were unable to quantify swimming effort using flipper strokes, visual inspection of the accelerometer channels indicated that the animals did spend considerable time at the surface in a prone position. This appeared to occur most often near the end of a trip and was hypothesized to indicate swimming. Seals lying on their stomachs in the prone position have a static acceleration of ~0 on the sway axis and ~10 on the heave-axis accelerometer, with some minor variation between tags due to sensor variability. We thus defined an animal to be in the prone position when the mean of the sway-axis acceleration was 0 (± 2) and the mean of the heave-axis acceleration was 10 (+2,-3). This slight negative skewing of the heave-axis acceleration reflected our observation that some individuals tended to roll slightly when in the prone swimming position.
Filmed captive behavior
We filmed a group of captive fur seals (4.5 yr old females at the Vancouver Aquarium, Canada) for approximately 18.5 hours over 7 days between May and December 2012 to validate the at-sea behavior our algorithm identified from the accelerometry data of the wild fur seals. One of the females in the group (NF08ME) was fitted with a specially-designed harness with an attached accelerometer. The instrumented fur seal swam freely with conspecifics in a 15 × 15 × 3.5 m deep research pool for several hours at a time, while we filmed her behavior with a high definition camera mounted above the pool. The anterior-posterior axis of acceleration was used to identify the porpoising behavior.
Speed analysis
Speeds were calculated between GPS fixes (obtained from the Mk-10F tag) via simple point interpolation and dividing distance traveled by time. GPS points associated with unrealistic travel speeds of greater than 3 m s-1 were removed. We then assigned speed to all behaviors exhibited between GPS fixes and average speeds per behavior were calculated on a per trip basis. We then used mixed effects models (with individual fur seal as a random effect) to determine if specific behaviors tended to have faster traveling speeds associated with them. We investigated two models (stratified by island as fur seals from St. Paul tended to have more GPS fixes per time than those from Bogoslof) where the basic model took the form of Speed i = β j Behavior j + seal k b k + e i, where b k and e i are both normally distributed random variables.
Behavior analyses
We sought to determine if there was a difference in the proportion of behaviors exhibited by different subsets of fur seals, and whether behaviors changed based on the length of the foraging trip. We were also interested whether differences existed between the two islands, between animals on St. Paul that used the on-shelf or off-shelf habitats, and between nocturnal or cathemeral foragers (“cathemeral “defined by Nordstrom et al. [10] for animals with > 10% of their dives during the day).
The proportions of each behavior exhibited by fur seals during a foraging trip always summed to one. Such data have some unusual statistical properties in that the proportions are highly correlated (e.g., if diving takes up 50% of the time, there is only 50% of the time that can be allocated to the other 6 behaviors) and the proportions are bounded by 0 and 1. This is a classic “compositions” data set, yet popular methods to analyze these data are not necessarily intuitive and involve taking log ratios of proportions before statistically comparing the ratios. There are at least three 3 R packages available for this type of analysis in addition to numerous textbooks and papers that vastly range in their accessibility for biologists. We relied heavily on van den Boogaart and Tolosana-Delgado [29], while the more mathematically inclined may prefer Aitchison [30]. We primarily used the package Compositions for data exploration and visualization. For the statistical analyses, we used fractional multinomial logistic regression using the fmlogit package in STATA, which uses the untransformed proportions themselves and accounts for multiple observations per animal (as many of our animals took more than one foraging trip). The full model for the fractional multinomial logistic regression included trip date, trip length, island (Bogoslof or St. Paul), forager type (nocturnal or cathemeral) and foraging region (on-shelf or off-shelf). Based on the results of this model, we used a simple linear regression to characterize the relationship between total diving time over the course of a day and total nighttime available.
Results
Tagging
Tags from seals on Bogoslof Island (n = 41 individuals) logged 111 useable foraging trips, while those from seals on St. Paul Island (n = 41) recorded 51 foraging trips. Trip duration for the Bogoslof seals averaged 3.3 days ranging from 3.75 hours to just over 13 days, while trip duration for St. Paul Island seals averaged 7.7 days ranging from just over 4 days to nearly 17 days. Trip distance as calculated by interpolation between GPS locations averaged 189 km and ranged from 0.5 to 525 km, while St. Paul Island trips averaged 582 km and ranged from 243 to 1265 km.
Behavior categorization
Behaviors differentiated from the free-ranging fur seals using acceleration data included resting (Fig. 3), shaking/pelage grooming (Fig. 4), the two types of rolling behavior, the W-roll (based on the wave form) and a sine wave-roll (Figs. 5, 6), and the prone position (Fig. 7). The data indicated the animals spent an unexpectedly large amount of time engaged in the two rolling behaviors (Table 1). Observations of the captive animals confirmed that we had correctly identified the rolling behavior (Fig. 8A) and shaking/grooming (Fig. 8B), as well as successfully identifying and characterizing porpoising behavior (Fig. 9). Thus, we are confident in our ability to interpret these specific behavioral categories represented by changes in acceleration.
Table 1. Mean proportions of time engaged in specific behaviors at-sea by lactating northern fur seals from Bogoslof and St. Paul islands as determined from acceleration signals and depth sensors.
Variable | Behavior | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dive | Rest | Shaking/Grooming | Rolling | Mixed | Prone | |||||||||||
Sine | W | Total | ||||||||||||||
Bogoslof | 0.292 | (0.10) | 0.072 | (0.06) | 0.096 | (0.06) | 0.047 | (0.05) | 0.227 | (0.11) | 0.274 | (0.13) | 0.155 | (0.09) | 0.111 | (0.12) |
St. Paul | 0.284 | (0.07) | 0.081 | (0.04) | 0.073 | (0.02) | 0.086 | (0.07) | 0.245 | (0.10) | 0.331 | (0.11) | 0.143 | (0.07) | 0.087 | (0.09) |
Both Islands | ||||||||||||||||
Cathemeral | 0.295 | (0.07) | 0.087 | (0.04) | 0.084 | (0.03) | 0.060 | (0.03) | 0.261 | (0.09) | 0.321 | (0.09) | 0.146 | (0.06) | 0.067 | (0.06) |
Nocturnal | 0.288 | (0.10) | 0.073 | (0.06) | 0.089 | (0.05) | 0.060 | (0.06) | 0.228 | (0.11) | 0.287 | (0.13) | 0.152 | (0.08) | 0.110 | (0.11) |
Off-shelf | 0.291 | (0.10) | 0.074 | (0.06) | 0.090 | (0.05) | 0.057 | (0.06) | 0.227 | (0.11) | 0.283 | (0.13) | 0.153 | (0.09) | 0.109 | (0.11) |
On-shelf | 0.284 | (0.06) | 0.080 | (0.04) | 0.082 | (0.02) | 0.074 | (0.04) | 0.263 | (0.10) | 0.336 | (0.10) | 0.142 | (0.06) | 0.076 | (0.08) |
St. Paul only | ||||||||||||||||
Cathemeral | 0.295 | (0.07) | 0.087 | (0.04) | 0.084 | (0.03) | 0.060 | (0.03) | 0.261 | (0.09) | 0.321 | (0.09) | 0.146 | (0.06) | 0.067 | (0.06) |
Nocturnal | 0.274 | (0.07) | 0.077 | (0.04) | 0.065 | (0.02) | 0.108 | (0.08) | 0.232 | (0.11) | 0.340 | (0.12) | 0.141 | (0.08) | 0.104 | (0.11) |
Off-shelf | 0.284 | (0.08) | 0.083 | (0.05) | 0.064 | (0.03) | 0.101 | (0.08) | 0.225 | (0.10) | 0.326 | (0.11) | 0.145 | (0.08) | 0.099 | (0.10) |
On-shelf | 0.284 | (0.06) | 0.080 | (0.04) | 0.082 | (0.02) | 0.074 | (0.04) | 0.263 | (0.10) | 0.336 | (0.10) | 0.142 | (0.06) | 0.076 | (0.08) |
Proportions are averaged over foraging trips and stratified by island, forager type (cathemeral or nocturnal), and region (on or off-shelf). Standard deviations are in parenthesis.
Diving was the most common behavior across any of the stratifications at just under 30% (Table 1), followed closely by the W-roll, resting, shaking/grooming and the prone position, which each constituted between 5 and 10% of the daily behavior. The sine-roll was the least exhibited behavior, though if combined with the W-roll (29%) was greater than the proportion of time spent diving (30%). Finally, the mixed, or unknown, category constituted about 15% of the fur seal activity budget.
Behavior analyses
On the whole, there does not seem to be any indication that the fur seal behaviors differ between any of our explanatory variables. The ternary diagrams do not show separation of proportions by explanatory variables (Fig. 10, see S1–S3 Figs. for the full suite of behaviors), and the vast majority of box plots show overlap on the notches (Fig. 11 and S4–S6 Figs.). Similarly, the lowest AICc indicates the best model is simply the constant with only four coefficients of 30 being significantly different from 0 in the most complicated model (Fig. 12, Table 2), one of which is the effect of date on diving. Further examination indicated that total diving time in a day increased with total night time available (as the summer progressed) (n = 162, slope = 0.45, t = 13.1, p<0.001) (Fig. 13).
Table 2. Fractional multinomial logistic regression models investigated.
Model Parameters | K | Log pseudolikelihood | AICc | Δ AICc |
---|---|---|---|---|
Constant | 6 | -283.7 | 579.9 | 0.0 |
Trip Duration + Constant | 12 | -282.0 | 590.1 | 10.2 |
Island + Constant | 12 | -283.0 | 592.1 | 12.2 |
Date + Constant | 12 | -283.1 | 592.3 | 12.4 |
Forager Type + Constant | 12 | -283.4 | 592.9 | 13.0 |
Region + Constant | 12 | -283.4 | 593.0 | 13.1 |
Lunar Phase + Constant | 12 | -283.4 | 593.0 | 13.1 |
Trip Duration + Island + Constant | 18 | -281.4 | 603.7 | 23.8 |
Trip Duration + Forager Type + Constant | 18 | -281.4 | 603.8 | 23.9 |
Trip Duration + Region + Constant | 18 | -281.6 | 604.2 | 24.3 |
Island + Forager Type + Constant | 18 | -282.6 | 606.2 | 26.3 |
Island + Region + Constant | 18 | -282.8 | 606.6 | 26.7 |
Forager Type + Region + Constant | 18 | -283.2 | 607.4 | 27.5 |
Trip Duration + Island + Forager Type + Constant | 24 | -281.1 | 619.2 | 39.3 |
Trip Duration + Island + Region + Constant | 24 | -281.3 | 619.6 | 39.7 |
Trip Duration + Forager Type + Region + Constant | 24 | -281.3 | 619.7 | 39.8 |
Island + Forager Type + Region + Constant | 24 | -282.6 | 622.2 | 42.3 |
Trip Duration + Island + Forager Type + Region + Constant | 30 | -281.0 | 636.7 | 56.8 |
Number of model parameters (K), Akaike Information Criterion corrected for small sample size (AICc) and the difference in AICc between the best model (ΔAICc).
Speed analysis
The comparison of GPS calculated speeds for each of the behaviors (Table 3) indicated that the prone position was significantly faster than all other behaviors from St. Paul Island and most of the other behaviors from Bogoslof Island. However, it was not significantly different from the diving and W-roll behavior for animals from Bogoslof (Fig. 14). Coefficient values (Table 3) are changes in speed relative to the prone position; hence the prone position coefficient is the mean speed for the prone position behavior.
Table 3. Parameter estimates for the effect of behavior on travel speeds.
Bogoslof | β-value | t-value | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Prone | 0.738 | 29.27 | <0.001 |
Dive | -0.022 | -0.75 | 0.451 |
Resting | -0.387 | -12.59 | <0.001 |
Shaking/grooming | -0.107 | -3.65 | <0.001 |
Sine-roll | -0.257 | -8.19 | <0.001 |
W-roll | -0.017 | -0.58 | 0.560 |
Mixed | -0.141 | -4.76 | <0.001 |
Pribilofs | |||
Prone | 1.303 | 38.31 | <0.001 |
Dive | -0.363 | -8.73 | <0.001 |
Resting | -0.953 | -22.90 | <0.001 |
Shaking/grooming | -0.414 | -9.95 | <0.001 |
Sine-roll | -0.897 | -21.56 | <0.001 |
W-roll | -0.280 | -6.72 | <0.001 |
Mixed | -0.504 | -12.13 | <0.001 |
The Prone behavior was used as the relative parameter so that the β’s for the other behaviors are adjustments to the Prone β and significance values indicate that the β’s are significantly different from 0 but can be interpreted as significantly different speeds from the Prone behavior speed.
Discussion
Using spectral analysis on accelerometer data to categorize behavior is a relatively easy way to discover, characterize, and distinguish behavioral types relative to other methods currently available. Not only are there relatively inexpensive software programs readily available for implementation, but the method does not require sophisticated statistical analyses common to other methods [31–33], thereby making spectral analysis readily accessible to most biologists. In our case, it allowed us to construct an activity budget for lactating northern fur seals. We were also able to validate our behavioral interpretations of the accelerometry data by instrumenting and filming fur seals in captivity.
Behavior categorization
The shaking behavior was easily identified by visually inspecting the accelerometry trace, while observing the captive animals made it possible to identify the grooming pattern that was associated with the shaking. The grooming behavior was a low frequency, irregular rolling pattern, typified by the animal staying on one side or the other for 1–10 seconds, turning over abruptly or gradually, remaining on the other side for another 1–10 seconds, and repeating this sequence while shaking every minute or so. Grooming maintains the fur for thermoregulation—and the time categorized as shaking to remove water and maintain the layer of air within the inner fur probably reflected the time (~10% of the time) needed to keep the fur in good thermoregulatory condition.
The most surprising result was the amount of time fur seals spent rolling at the surface (combined W-wave and sine wave), which accounted for nearly one-third of the entire foraging trip. It is unclear what specific purpose rolling at the surface may serve, though it may be used to scan for predators or prey. The rolling occurred in two forms that we named based on the graphic display of the accelerometer data—a sine-wave-roll (a 360° roll) and a W-roll (whereby the animal turns ~300° in one direction, then turns back 300° in the other direction). The rolling behavior, particularly the W-form, often continued among wild fur seals for hours at a time with little break, while captive animals did not sustain either of the rolling behaviors for more than a minute at a time. W-rolls were associated with faster swimming speeds and hence likely associated with transiting, while the sine-rolls appear to be associated with loitering.
The GPS-based speed analysis relative to the behaviors indicated that the prone position was associated with the fastest speeds—making it the most likely behavior associated with transiting. The St. Paul data were more informative as the fur seals from this island averaged a greater number of GPS fixes per day (19.1) than the Bogoslof seals (15.0 fixes per day), so calculated speeds were more likely to be associated with periods of distinct behaviors as the number of GPS fixes increased (Fig. 14). We expected diving to be associated with active swimming and high travel speeds, but did not expect the data to indicate shaking/grooming behaviors to be associated with high travel speeds. Shaking and grooming were associated with diving, but did not occur for extended periods at this time. Rather, shaking and grooming occurred most frequently during the first 24 hours of the fur seals leaving land (which may reflect the seals cleaning the dirt and fecal matter they may have gathered on their fur while on land). Thus, it appears that the speeds associated with shaking and grooming likely reflected speeds emanating from other associated behaviors.
Filmed captive behavior
While it is difficult to unconditionally determine all behaviors from acceleration data, we found that many of the key behaviors that dominated the at-sea activity we recorded can be accurately categorized and successfully ground-truthed using captive animals. Some of the behaviors we classified for northern fur seals were previously known from direct observations, such as the jug handle resting position (where fur seals lie on one side and raise their hind flippers to touch the side flipper that extends out of the water). Shaking and various forms of rolling and grooming behaviors have also been commonly seen at the surface during the summer months in the waters near St. Paul and Bogoslof Islands. We were unable to associate flipper beats or porpoising with swimming at the surface. Flipper beats are easily recognizable during dives and were easily identifiable in the relatively calm pool at the aquarium. We presume that the flipper beats that occur while swimming at or near the surface in the wild are masked by signals from ocean waves, particularly surface chop.
Behavior analyses
Because of the differences between the population trajectories of the two islands, as well as differences in trip lengths and the two different regions and strategies that animals use to forage from St. Paul Island, we expected to find differences in the relative amounts of behaviors between these ecological groupings. Specifically, we expected to find that the increasing population of Bogoslof fur seals that have short feeding trips would have an easier time foraging. However, it’s difficult to predict what an “easier time” would look like in relative amounts of behavior, but one might expect less time swimming and diving, and more time resting on the surface given that a satiated animal might be expected to dive less. The lack of differences may imply that behavioral allocations are more controlled by external factors. It may be, for example, that the amount of time that can be spent diving is limited by the amount of available darkness, (Fig. 13) and thus prey availability. It may also be that the behaviors of lactating northern fur seals as we measured them are not as flexible as we believed they should be, but are hardwired as part of a generalized successful foraging strategy that depends less on other behavioral choices, such as chosen foraging region (on-shelf vs. off-shelf). Given this data set and analysis, it appears that trip length is the only behavioral clue we have to foraging success.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We are indebted to A. Baylis, J. Gibbens, R. Marshall, R. Papish, A. Will, C. Berger, A. Harding, R. Towell, B. Fadley, K. Call, N. Liebsch and C. Kuhn for assistance or advice with animal captures and instrument deployment. Rory Wilson, Nikolai Liebsch and Adrian Gleiss provided significant analytical technical advice. Thanks also to the husbandry staff at the Vancouver Aquarium for work with the captive fur seals, and to the anonymous referees who provided constructive criticism that significantly improved the manuscript. This work was undertaken as part of the Bering Sea Integrated Research Program (BSIERP) Patch Dynamics Study (publication number 151), funded jointly by the US National Science Foundation and the North Pacific Research Board (publication number 524).
Funding Statement
This work was conducted as part of the Bering Sea Integrated Research Program (BSIERP) patch dynamics project, funded jointly by the U.S. National Science Foundation and the North Pacific Research Board. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."
References
- 1. Call KA, Ream RR, Johnson D, Sterling JT, Towell RG. Foraging route tactics and site fidelity of adult female northern fur seal (Callorhinus ursinus) around the Pribilof Islands. Deep Sea Research Part II: Topical Studies in Oceanography. 2008;55(16):1883–96. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Towell RG, Ream R, Bengston J, Sterling J. 2010 northern fur seal pup production and adult male counts on the Pribilof Islands, Alaska Alaska Fisheries Science Center Memorandum, NOAA; 2011. Available: http://www.afsc.noaa.gov/nmml/pdf/2010-nfs-pup-estimates.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Towell RG, Ream R. 2011 northern fur seal pup production estimate on Bogoslof Island, Alaska Alaska Fisheries Science Center Memorandum, NOAA; 2012. Available: http://www.afsc.noaa.gov/nmml/PDF/BogPupMem11_final.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Goebel M, Bengtson J, DeLong R, Gentry R, Loughlin T. Diving patterns and foraging locations of female northern fur seals. Fish Bull (Wash, DC). 1991;89(2):171–9. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Robson BW, Goebel ME, Baker JD, Ream RR, Loughlin TR, Francis RC, et al. Seperation of foraging habitat among breeding sites of a colonial marine predator, the northern fur seal (Callorhinus ursinus). Can J Zool. 2004;82(1):20–9. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Insley S, Robson B, Yack T, Ream R, Burgess W. Acoustic determination of activity and flipper stroke rate in foraging northern fur seal females. Endangered Species Research. 2007;4(1–2):147–55. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Loughlin TR, Bengtson JL, Merrick RL. Characteristics of feeding trips of female northern fur seals. Can J Zool. 1987;65(8):2079–84. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gentry RL, Kooyman GL, Goebel ME. Feeding and diving behavior of northern fur seals In: Gentry RL, Kooyman GL, editors. Fur seals: Maternal strategies on land and at sea. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press; 1986. p. 61–78. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Springer AM, Ream RR, Iverson SJ. Seasonal foraging strategies and consequences for northern fur seals at colonies with opposite population trends—Year 2 North Pacific Research Board Final Report Project 514, 79 p, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Nordstrom CA, Battaile BC, Cotté C, Trites AW. Foraging habitats of lactating northern fur seals are structured by thermocline depths and submesoscale fronts in the eastern Bering Sea. Deep Sea Research Part II: Topical Studies in Oceanography. 2013;88–89:78–96. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Sterling JT, Ream RR. At-sea behavior of juvenile male northern fur seals (Callorhinus ursinus). Can J Zool. 2004;82(10):1621–37. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Shepard ELC, Wilson RP, Quintana F, Laich AG, Liebsch N, Albareda DA, et al. Identification of animal movement patterns using tri-axial accelerometry. Endangered Species Research. 2008;10:47–60. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Nathan R, Spiegel O, Fortmann-Roe S, Harel R, Wikelski M, Getz WM. Using tri-axial acceleration data to identify behavioral modes of free-ranging animals: general concepts and tools illustrated for griffon vultures. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2012;215(6):986–96. 10.1242/jeb.058602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Sakamoto KQ, Sato K, Ishizuka M, Watanuki Y, Takahashi A, Daunt F, et al. Can ethograms be automatically generated using body acceleration data from free-ranging birds? PLoS ONE. 2009;4(4):e5379 10.1371/journal.pone.0005379 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Shamoun-Baranes J, Bom R, van Loon EE, Ens BJ, Oosterbeek K, Bouten W. From Sensor Data to Animal Behaviour: An Oystercatcher Example. PloS ONE. 2012;7(5):e37997 10.1371/journal.pone.0037997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Wilson RP, Shepard E, Liebsch N. Prying into the intimate details of animal lives: use of a daily diary on animals. Endangered Species Research. 2008;4(1–2):123–37. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Fossette S, Gleiss AC, Myers AE, Garner S, Liebsch N, Whitney NM, et al. Behaviour and buoyancy regulation in the deepest-diving reptile: the leatherback turtle. J Exp Biol. 2010;213(23):4074–83. 10.1242/jeb.048207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Halsey LG, Portugal SJ, Smith JA, Murn CP, Wilson RP. Recording raptor behavior on the wing via accelerometry. J Field Ornithol. 2009;80(2):171–7. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Whitney NM, Pratt HL Jr., Pratt TC, Carrier JC. Identifying shark mating behaviour using three-dimensional acceleration loggers. Endangered Species Research. 2010;10:71–82. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Viviant M, Trites AW, Rosen DAS, Monestiez P, Guinet C. Prey capture attempts can be detected in Steller sea lions and other marine predators using accelerometers. Polar Biol. 2010;33(5):713–9. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Naito Y. How can we observe the underwater feeding behavior of endotherms? Polar Science. 2007;1(2–4):101–11. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Skinner JP, Norberg SE, Andrews RD. Head striking during fish capture attempts by Steller sea lions and the potential for using head surge acceleration to predict feeding behavior. Endangered Species Research. 2009;10:61–9. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Gleiss AC, Wilson RP, Shepard ELC. Making overall dynamic body acceleration work: on the theory of acceleration as a proxy for energy expenditure. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2011;2(1):23–33. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Gleiss AC, Gruber SH, Wilson RP. Multi-channel data-logging: towards determination of behaviour and metabolic rate in free-swimming sharks. Tagging and tracking of marine animals with electronic devices. 2009;9:211–28. [Google Scholar]
- 25. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria: URL http://www.R-project.org/. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Zeppelin TK, Orr AJ. Stable isotope and scat analyses indicate diet and habitat partitioning in northern fur seals Callorhinus ursinus across the eastern Pacific. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2010;409:241–53. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Kuhn CE, Baker JD, Towell RG, Ream RR. Evidence of localized resource depletion following a natural colonization event by a large marine predator. J Anim Ecol. 2014;83:1169–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Paredes R, Orben RA, Suryan RM, Irons DB, Roby DD, Harding AM, et al. Foraging responses of black-legged kittiwakes to prolonged food-shortages around colonies on the Bering Sea Shelf. PloS one. 2014;9(3):e92520 10.1371/journal.pone.0092520 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Boogaart KGvd, Tolosana-Delgado R. Analyzing Compositional Data with R. Gentleman R, Bornik K, Parmigiani G, editors. New York: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Aitchison J. The statistical analysis of compositional data: Springer; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Jonsen ID, Flemming JM, Myers RA. Robust state-space modeling of animal movement data. Ecology. 2005;86(11):2874–80. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Breed GA, Jonsen ID, Myers RA, Bowen WD, Leonard ML. Sex-specific, seasonal foraging tactics of adult grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) revealed by state-space analysis. Ecology. 2009;90(11):3209–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Dowd M, Joy R. Estimating behavioral parameters in animal movement models using a state-augmented particle filter. Ecology. 2011;92(3):568–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.