Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Nutr Rev. 2014 Oct;72(0 1):34–47. doi: 10.1111/nure.12151

The Cumulative Neurobehavioral and Physiological Effects of Chronic Caffeine Intake: Individual Differences and Implications for the Use of Caffeinated Energy Products

Andrea M Spaeth 1, Namni Goel 2, David F Dinges 3
PMCID: PMC4404626  NIHMSID: NIHMS613709  PMID: 25293542

Abstract

The use of caffeine-containing energy products (CCEP) has increased worldwide in recent years and research shows that CCEP can improve cognitive and physical performance. All of the top-selling energy drinks contain caffeine, which is likely to be the primary psychoactive ingredient in CCEP. Presumably, individuals consume CCEP to counteract feelings of ‘low-energy’ in situations causing tiredness, fatigue, and/or reduced alertness. This review discusses the scientific evidence for sleep loss, circadian phase, sleep inertia and the time-on-task effect as causes of ‘low energy’ and summarizes research assessing the efficacy of caffeine to counteract decreased alertness and increased fatigue in such situations. The results of a placebo-controlled experiment on healthy adults undergoing three nights of total sleep deprivation (with or without 2 hour naps every 12 hours) are presented to illustrate the physiological and neurobehavioral effects of sustained low-dose caffeine. Individual differences, including genetic factors, in the response to caffeine and to sleep loss are discussed. We conclude with future directions for research on this important and evolving topic.


The use of caffeine-containing energy products (CCEP) including caffeine-containing energy drinks (CCED) has increased worldwide in recent years. According to Symphony IRI data compiled by Bloomberg, US CCED sales increased 6.7% to $9.7 billion from 2011-2012 to 2012-2013. In one study of college students, 51% of respondents consumed at least one CCED per month1 and data obtained from active service members showed that 44.8% consumed at least one CCED daily.2 Although the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) does not define the term ‘energy drink’, products popularly designated, advertised or consumed as energy drinks contain stimulants and are marketed to provide the consumer with behavioral ‘energy’, improved alertness, and improved cognitive and/or physical ability.3 Although these purported effects can encompass different neurobehavioral domains,4 recent research has shown that the ingestion of CCEP can improve attention, reaction speed, information processing, memory, mood and aerobic performance.5-14

All of the top-selling energy drinks contain caffeine (approximately 30-50 mg per 100 ml, with a typical serving size being 240 ml).3,15 In this review we focus on caffeine, as there is evidence that caffeine is the active ingredient responsible for the cognitive and physical effects of CCEP.3,16-18 Other articles in this issue review the potential contribution of other ingredients to the effects of CCEP. Caffeine is a methylxanthine naturally found in the seeds and leaves of certain plants, which acts as a central nervous system stimulant in humans.19 Following ingestion, caffeine is rapidly absorbed from the gastrointestinal tract into the bloodstream and passes all biological membranes, including the blood-brain barrier.20 Caffeine exerts its effects by antagonizing adenosine (primarily A1 and A2a) receptors which are widely expressed throughout the body (including in the cardiovascular, respiratory, renal and gastrointestinal systems)21 and in the brain (including areas believed to be involved in the regulation of sleep-wake).22,23 Adenosine inhibits the release of various neurotransmitters (i.e., acetylcholine, noradrenaline, and dopamine); therefore, caffeine increases the release of these neurotransmitters by inhibiting adenosine.19,24 This leads to peripheral vasoconstriction, increased blood pressure, and thermogenesis, as well as greater renal excretion of sodium and water and gastric secretion of acid and pepsin.24,25

Presumably, individuals consume energy products to counteract feelings of ‘low-energy’ in situations causing decreased alertness and increased fatigue. This review discusses the scientific evidence for sleep loss, circadian phase, sleep inertia and the time-on-task effect as causes of ‘low-energy’ and summarizes research assessing the efficacy of caffeine to counteract decreased alertness and increased fatigue in such situations. Individual differences in the vulnerability to sleep loss and to caffeine, including genetic factors, are also presented. We conclude with future directions for research on this important and evolving topic.

Causes of Low Energy

In healthy humans, alertness is primarily regulated by two processes: homeostatic regulation and circadian timing.26 The homeostatic sleep-dependent process has two components, the first (process S) balances sleep propensity by tracking recent sleep history.27 As hours of wakefulness increase, homeostatic drive increases the propensity for sleep. The second component of the homeostatic process (process U) monitors sleep-wake on a longer time scale; process U builds up over several days of prolonged wakefulness (when sleep debt is accruing).28 The status of process U codetermines the rate by which process S increases sleep propensity and decreases alertness.28 The endogenous circadian process (process C) tracks changes in light exposure (as well as other zeitgebers or ‘time-givers’) and entrains sleep propensity to the light-dark cycle. When sleep propensity is increased (during the night) waking performance is degraded.29

Sleep Loss

For most healthy adults, physiological sleep duration ranges between 7.0 and 8.5 hours; however, habitual sleep duration in adults is determined by a variety of factors and shows considerable variance within and between individuals.30 Data from a national survey revealed that approximately 28.3% of adults in the U.S. report sleeping less than 7 hours per night31 and two epidemiological studies found that the prevalence of being a self-reported ‘short sleeper’ (≤6 hours/day) has increased significantly in recent decades.32,33 Compensated work time and travel time (which includes commuting to work) are the primary determinants of sleep duration34 with longer work hours associated with shorter sleep duration.35-37 Studies suggest that habitual short sleepers do not require less sleep than other adults; rather, these individuals gradually accrue sleep debt over time.38-40 Laboratory studies examining the effects of chronic sleep restriction showed that individuals sleeping less than 7 hours per night for consecutive nights exhibited increased sleep propensity41-45 and decreased alertness42-48 with performance becoming progressively worse as sleep debt accumulated across days.

In addition to chronic sleep restriction, some individuals undergo prolonged periods of wakefulness without sleep (‘pulling all-nighters’; acute total sleep deprivation) in order to meet deadlines for school or as part of work duties. Although population-based prevalence rates for this type of sleep loss are unavailable, certain individuals, including college students,49 medical residents,50 military personnel, truck drivers51 and shift workers52 experience total sleep deprivation routinely. Acute total sleep deprivation also increased sleep propensity and decreased alertness in laboratory studies.46,53-55

The increased propensity for sleep and decreased alertness due to sleep loss from chronic sleep restriction or acute total sleep deprivation can lead to ‘microsleeps’ (very brief sleep episodes that intrude into wakefulness despite an individual's best effort to stay awake),56,57 and to wake-state instability. Wake-state instability refers to moment-to-moment shifts in the neurobiological systems mediating the motivated desire to sustain waking alertness and those mediating the involuntary homeostatic drive to fall asleep.55,58 This interaction between wake and sleep drives results in unpredictable behavior, including increased variability in cognitive performance on tests requiring vigilant attention such as the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT): sleep deprivation results in periods of accurate responding interrupted by errors of omission (i.e., lapses of attention that manifest as long response times) comingled with errors of commission (i.e., premature responses reflecting compensatory effort).55,59 Although individuals may not realize they are experiencing ‘microsleeps’ and performance decrements, over time such behavioral instability can progress into sleep attacks (involuntary sleep bouts lasting 30 seconds or more, from which individuals cannot spontaneously awaken from without additional stimulation).59-62

Circadian Phase

A circadian rhythm is an endogenous 24-hour oscillatory variation in physiology and behavior. Environmental light is transmitted from the retina to suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN, known as the central pacemaker) in the anterior hypothalamus and then to the pineal gland via a multisynaptic pathway; the pineal gland secretes melatonin, a hormone that regulates various biological functions, including sleep-wake cycles.63-67 In humans, melatonin levels peak during the dark cycle (which normally coincides with a period of inactivity and sleepiness) and decrease during the light cycle (which normally coincides with a period of activity and wakefulness).68,69 Thus, the alternation of light and darkness directly entrains an organism's circadian rhythms. At night, when melatonin levels are high, sleep propensity is increased and alertness is degraded.70 Sleep propensity/decreased alertness peaks during the early hours of the morning (02:00h-07:00h) and peaks (to a lesser degree) again in the mid-afternoon (13:00h-16:00h, known as the ‘post-lunch dip’).59,71,72 Conversely, sleep propensity is reduced and performance is improved during a 3-hour period of time prior to the onset of melatonin secretion (approximately 18:00h-21:00h) known as the ‘wake maintenance zone’.73

A progressive change associated with the time spent awake is typically superimposed on the circadian rhythms of neurobehavioral performance.74 When total sleep deprivation is extended for several days, the detrimental effects on alertness and performance increase, and although the circadian process can mitigate some of the effects of sleep loss during times of the circadian peak in alertness, it is overlaid on a continuing change reflecting increasing homeostatic pressure for sleep.

Sleep Inertia

Sleep inertia describes the confusion, disorientation, tendency to fall back asleep and decrease in alertness that occurs immediately after waking from sleep.75-79 When comparing cognitive performance immediately upon awakening after an 8-hour night of sleep to cognitive performance after one night of total sleep deprivation, Wertz and colleagues found that impairment from sleep inertia was more severe than impairment from sleep deprivation.79 Although it is believed that sleep inertia always occurs upon awakening, the magnitude and duration of its effect on performance depends on many factors including prior sleep duration, recent sleep history, timing of sleep and wake, and the sleep stage immediately prior to awakening.80-82 The effects of sleep inertia on alertness and sleep propensity are more pronounced when the sleep episode is preceded by a history of sleep deprivation83 or during the circadian nadir,83,84 and when the sleeper is aroused from slow-wave sleep (SWS).85,86

Time-on-Task Effect

The time-on-task effect reflects the decrease in performance over time during a cognitively-demanding task.87 Originally, researchers thought this phenomenon reflected boredom or decreased motivation;87,88 however, now it is believed that the cumulative increase in effort required to perform a task for an extended period of time depletes limited cognitive resources.89-91 As the duration of the task increases, there is a progressive increase in errors and reaction times as well as greater variability in performance;92-95 thus, the longer an individual is required to perform a task, the more performance is negatively affected. The time-on-task effect is amplified during extended wakefulness due to sleep loss as well as during the biological night due to circadian rhythmicity.96-100 Taking a break to rest (with or without sleep) or performing a different task replenishes cognitive resources and restores performance.101

Effects of Caffeine on Neurobehavioral Performance and Physiological Measures

Several experimental studies of healthy adults have investigated the efficacy of acute caffeine administration in counteracting the decline in neurobehavioral performance during extended wakefulness or at certain times of day using well-controlled double-blind designs. When performance was assessed during a 29-64 hour period without sleep and compared to the performance of a placebo control group, caffeine administration increased sleep latency, improved the ability to stay awake, prevented slowing of reaction times, and reduced the number of lapses in attention.102-111 The efficacy of caffeine in producing these effects is dose-dependent106,108,112 with higher doses (200 and 600 mg) producing better performance. However, more side effects (e.g., feelings of jitteriness, abdominal pain and nausea) have been reported after the ingestion of higher doses of caffeine (600 mg) compared to lower doses.111 Caffeine administered in the early morning,113-117 afternoon,118,119 or overnight120-122 to counteract circadian-related performance decrements also increased alertness and improved neurobehavioral functioning compared to placebo treatment.

In order to assess the cumulative effects of caffeine administration during extended wakefulness on various aspects of cognitive performance, sleep and sleep propensity, two studies administered a low dose of caffeine at a high frequency (0.3 mg/kg per hour). Wyatt and colleagues (2004)123 used a forced desynchrony protocol to examine the sleep homeostatic and circadian effects of caffeine on performance separately. Subjects (16 healthy men) were placed on a 42.85 hour ‘day’ which included a 28.57 hour wake period followed by a 14.28 hour sleep period for 14 ‘days’ (i.e. 25 24-hour days) and were randomized to receive either placebo or caffeine. Subjects in the caffeine group exhibited fewer unintentional sleep onsets and better performance on cognitive throughput and attention tasks during the scheduled periods of extended wakefulness (28.57 hours) and during circadian troughs in alertness.123

In a double-blind study, Dinges and colleagues124,125 also administered a sustained low-dose of caffeine (0.3 mg/kg per hour) and used a multiple-night total sleep deprivation paradigm in order to compare the effects of caffeine with or without periodic naps (NAP, 2 hour naps every 12 hours) on a range of cognitive performance outcomes. Subjects (58 healthy men) experienced three baseline nights of sleep followed by 88 hours of extended wakefulness (i.e., 3 nights of total sleep deprivation [TSD]) and 1-2 nights of recovery sleep (Figure 1). Subjects were randomized to one of four conditions: TSD with placebo, TSD with caffeine, NAP with placebo and NAP with caffeine. Caffeine or placebo was administered every hour via a pill with administration beginning during hour 22 (and continuing through hour 88) of TSD. Subjects were informed that each pill could be either placebo or caffeine (although in reality, all pills in a given condition were the same—i.e., either always placebo or always caffeine). Similar to findings in the Wyatt et al study,123 plasma caffeine concentrations rose in an exponential saturating manner during drug administration125; however, there were marked individual differences in response to caffeine administration (unpublished findings, Figure 2). The maximum concentration (CMax) of plasma caffeine ranged from 2.0–9.4 mg/l among subjects, despite the fact that they remained in a controlled environment, were under the constant supervision of laboratory and hospital staff, were relatively sedentary during the study and consumed an isocaloric, caffeine-free diet. Possible mechanisms for these individual differences in response to caffeine are discussed below. Caffeine administration did not affect heart rate (data not shown), but increased core temperature (measured rectally; Figure 3) and plasma noradrenaline levels (Figure 4) compared to placebo administration (unpublished findings), consistent with the reported physiological effects of caffeine.24,25

Figure 1. Overview of Chronic Caffeine Study Protocol.

Figure 1

Fifty-eight healthy male subjects (mean age, 29 years) with a history of moderate caffeine intake were admitted to the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania. Subjects did not use any caffeine, alcohol, tobacco or other medications in the two weeks prior to or during the experiment. The laboratory experiment began with one adaptation day and two baseline (BL) days with bedtimes from 23:30h-07:30h. Subjects then underwent 88 hours of extended wakefulness (i.e. three nights of total sleep deprivation; TSD). If subjects were randomized to the NAP condition, they were provided with seven 2-hour nap opportunities scheduled every 12 hours from 14:45h-16:45h and from 02:45h-04:45h. Starting 22 hours into the 88-hour period of extended wakefulness, subjects received a pill containing either 0.3 mg/kg caffeine or placebo every hour (except during naps). After the 88-hour period of extended wakefulness, subjects remained in the laboratory for 1-2 nights of recovery sleep.

Figure 2. Plasma Caffeine Levels among Subjects Administered Caffeine during Extended Wakefulness.

Figure 2

Subjects were administered placebo or caffeine (0.3 mg/kg) every hour from hours 22-88 of extended wakefulness (i.e. they received 66 pills). For example, a 75 kg subject would have received 22.5 mg of caffeine per pill which results in a total cumulative dose of 1485 mg caffeine (540 mg caffeine per day). Blood samples were taken via an indwelling intravenous catheter for assessment of plasma concentrations of caffeine at 90 minute intervals. Caffeine levels were assessed using standardized radioimmunoassay techniques. Caffeine levels rose steadily within 3.25 hours of the first administration. There were marked individual differences in plasma caffeine levels (indicated by SD and the CMax range: 2.0-9.4 mg/l). Following the end of administration, there was a steady decline in plasma caffeine levels. Data shown as Mean ± SD for n=25 subjects receiving caffeine (unpublished findings).

Figure 3. Core Temperature among Subjects Administered Caffeine or Placebo during 88 hours of Extended Wakefulness.

Figure 3

Core temperature was monitored continuously from Baseline Day 3 until the end of the 88 hour period of extended wakefulness (total sleep deprivation). Core temperature was sampled at 2-minute intervals using a Steri-Probe 491B rectal thermistor (YSI, Yellow Springs, OH) connected to an Actillume ambulatory recording system (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., NY). Core temperature was significantly higher in the caffeine group relative to the placebo group 4-22 hours after the first caffeine administration (p< 0.0001). As expected, both groups demonstrated a significant circadian rhythm in core temperature. Data presented as raw means (unpublished findings).

Figure 4. Plasma Noradrenaline Levels among Subjects Administered Caffeine or Placebo during 88 hours of Extended Wakefulness.

Figure 4

Plasma noradrenaline levels were assessed in blood samples taken every 90 minutes beginning on Baseline Day 3 (BL 3), through the 88 hours of extended wakefulness (total sleep deprivation; TSD1-3) and ending during the first recovery day (REC). Although sleep loss did not significantly affect noradrenaline levels, sustained low-dose caffeine administration significantly increased plasma noradrenaline levels compared to placebo administration (p=0.016). There were notable individual differences in plasma noradrenaline levels in response to caffeine (the standard deviation on TSD Day 3 was much larger among subjects in the caffeine group (SD=389.1) compared to subjects in the placebo group (SD=161.7). Data shown as Mean ± SEM for n=17 subjects in the TSD Condition (unpublished findings).

It is well established that neurobehavioral performance on tests of attention, cognitive throughput and memory worsens following sleep deprivation. Consistent with other studies,126 we found that sustained low-dose caffeine ingestion significantly attenuated decrements in reaction time and number of lapses in attention on the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT);127 however, this effect became less potent as the hours of extended wakefulness increased (unpublished findings, Figures 5A and 5B). Caffeine did not, however, significantly improve cognitive throughput as measured by the Digit Symbol Substitution Test128 or working memory as measured by the Probed Memory Recall Task129 (unpublished findings, Figures 5C and 5D). Notably, a 2-hour nap every 12 hours during the 88-hour sleep deprivation phase of the experiment, in combination with double-blind caffeine administration, improved attention, cognitive throughput and memory. Moreover, this effect lasted throughout the sleep deprivation period (unpublished findings, Figure 5).

Figure 5. Neurobehavioral Performance across 88 hours of Extended Wakefulness.

Figure 5

Subjects were randomized to one of four conditions during 88 hours of extended wakefulness: No nap opportunities with placebo treatment (TSD/Placebo), No nap opportunities with caffeine treatment (TSD/Caffeine), 2-hour nap opportunities every 12 hours with placebo treatment (NAP/Placebo) and 2-hour nap opportunities every 12 hours with caffeine treatment (NAP/Caffeine). Standardized measures of performance were collected every 2 hours using a 30-minute computerized assessment battery. This test battery included the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT)127, the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)128 and the Probed Recall Memory Task (PRM)129. The PVT is a simple reaction time test of behavioral alertness that is free of a learning curve and is highly sensitive to sleep deprivation. During the DSST, subjects are presented with nine different symbols which each correspond to a number (1-9) and must type in the correct number when each symbol that appears on the screen. In the PRM task subjects are presented with a list of six word pairs for 30 seconds and then after taking another test for 10 minutes, subjects are given one word from each set of pairs and have 2.5 minutes to fill in the complementary word. Sleep deprivation led to significantly slower reaction times (A) and an increase in the number of lapses in attention (B) on the PVT, a decreased number of correct responses on the DSST, reflecting slowed cognitive throughput (C) and a decrease in the number of words correctly recalled on the PRM, indicating impaired memory (D). Caffeine administration improved reaction time and reduced attentional lapses during the first 22 hours of administration (hours 24-46 of extended wakefulness; p< 0.05) but had no significant effect on DSST or PRM performance. Naps in combination with caffeine improved performance on all three tasks across the entire period of extended wakefulness. Data presented as raw means (unpublished findings).

Additionally, in this experiment124,125 caffeine affected the number of times subjects experienced sleep attacks (i.e., fell asleep for 30 seconds without spontaneous recovery while performing the PVT). Although caffeine reduced the total number of sleep attacks, a 2-hour nap opportunity every 12 hours, with or without caffeine ingestion, significantly reduced the number of sleep attacks and the percent of subjects experiencing a sleep attack, and extended the number of waking hours until the first sleep attack (unpublished findings, Table 1).

Table 1.

Effect of placebo versus caffeine treatment on sleep attacks* while taking the Psychomotor Vigilance Test during either 88 hours of total sleep deprivation (TSD) or 88 hours of extended wakefulness with 2-hour nap opportunities every 12 hours (NAP)**

Condition Total Number of Sleep Attacks Subjects with > 1 Sleep Attack (% of subjects in that condition) Wake Time (hours) to First Sleep Attack
TSD/Placebo 173 8 (57%) 23
TSD/Caffeine 59 5 (42%) 23
NAP/Placebo 3 2 (15%) 72
NAP/Caffeine 3 2 (13%) 50
*

falling asleep for 30 seconds without spontaneous recovery

**

(unpublished findings)

Interestingly, in addition to the aforementioned effects, caffeine attenuated the typically observed decreases in cognitive performance upon awakening from nap sleep (i.e., sleep inertia). Van Dongen and colleagues (2001)125 measured PVT performance immediately before and after five 2-hour nap opportunities among subjects receiving either placebo or caffeine during 88 hours of extended wakefulness. In the placebo condition, the number of lapses in attention was higher and response speed was slower during the test bout immediately following the nap compared to performance on the two test bouts prior to the nap. In the caffeine condition, the number of lapses and response speed during the test bout immediately following the nap were slightly improved and not significantly different from performance during the two test bouts prior to the nap;125 thus, caffeine eliminated the effect of sleep inertia on alertness (Figure 6). Consistent with this finding, Newman et al (2013)130 found that caffeine administration (using caffeinated gum) attenuated the effect of sleep inertia on performance when subjects are aroused after 1 and 6 hours of overnight sleep, and Hayashi et al (2003)131 found that caffeine administration (using 100 ml of coffee) attenuated the effect of sleep inertia on performance following an afternoon nap.

Figure 6. Caffeine and Sleep Inertia.

Figure 6

Performance lapses (> 500 ms reaction time; total per test bout) on the PVT are shown. Dotted lines indicate the placebo condition; solid lines indicate the caffeine condition. The data are presented as collapsed over the consecutive 12-hour segments around the last five naps of the experiment. Thus, the abscissa is collapsed over AM and PM times of day; naps (gray bar) took place from 14:45h-16:45h and from 02:45- 04:45h. Sleep inertia (increases in the number of PVT lapses) was consistently observed immediately after each nap in the placebo condition, but not in the caffeine condition. Data presented as Mean ± SEM for n=28 subjects in the NAP condition. (Figure reprinted with permission from Van Dongen et al., 2001, Sleep).

Van Dongen and colleagues also assessed the effect of caffeine administration on sleep physiology during nap sleep.125 Subjects in the NAP condition were provided with an opportunity to nap for 2 hours from 14:45h-16:45h and from 02:45h-04:45h during the 88-hour period of extended wakefulness; only naps (n=5) that occurred after pill administration began were used for analyses (Figure 1). During nap sleep, sleep latency was significantly longer (by 9.7 minutes) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep duration was reduced (by 5.2 minutes) in the caffeine condition compared to the placebo condition,125 consistent with a large amount of data on the inhibitory effects of caffeine on sleep onset and depth. Non-REM (NREM) sleep duration was reduced in only the first nap following pill administration and NREM slow-wave energy was reduced in only the first and second naps following pill administration in the caffeine condition compared to the placebo condition.125 Thus, as hours of wakefulness increased across protocol days, the increased pressure for sleep superseded caffeine's disruptive effects on sleep.

When assessing alertness across the work day, high levels of caffeine consumption produced greater alertness, an attenuated slowing of reaction time, fewer cognitive failures, and a reduced risk for work-related accidents.132 Laboratory studies have also shown that caffeine administration improved performance on long-duration tests of attention,95,133,134 reduced impairment (e.g., lane shifting) during 1-2 hours of continuous driving in a driving simulator,135,136 and attenuated fatigue induced by the time-on-task effect.7,99,113

Effects of Caffeine on Circadian Rhythms

While much is known about the effects of caffeine on sleep and neurobehavioral performance deficits due to sleep loss, far less is known about caffeine's effects on circadian rhythms, with the vast majority of data obtained from nonhumans. Caffeine has been reported to phase shift the melatonin rhythm in chick pineal cells in a phase-dependent manner similar to the effects of light,137 and to phase shift the temperature rhythm of rats.138 In hamsters, caffeine attenuated phase shifts induced by wheel running.139 Mechanistically, in rodents, caffeine may act directly on the SCN to alter circadian phase.140-142 This finding would be consistent with reports that the adenosine A1 receptor modulates light-induced phase shifts, protein expression in the SCN, and SCN field potential amplitude following optic nerve stimulation.143-145 Caffeine also lengthened the circadian period in both the fungus Neurospora146 and in mouse cell lines and cultured liver explants,141 where caffeine lengthened the circadian period of clock gene expression. In intact mice, under ad libitum feeding conditions,147 caffeine modified clock gene expression in the liver and jejunum.

By contrast, in humans, few data on the circadian effects of caffeine are available. In a forced desynchrony protocol, caffeine did not significantly affect circadian period compared to placebo.123 However, in a modified constant routine protocol, caffeine decreased nighttime melatonin levels, attenuated the normal decrease in body temperature, and phase delayed the temperature rhythm during 45.5 hours of extended wakefulness.148 It is unknown, however, whether these effects last beyond the day of measurement to subsequent days. Appropriately timed doses of caffeine, given in the evening, may be able to phase delay the endogenous circadian pacemaker in humans, similar to light. However, further research is warranted, including the development of phase-response curves using different physiological doses of caffeine. Such information would be valuable given caffeine's extensive use in situations of circadian misalignment including night shift work and jetlag.

Individual Differences in Response to Sleep Deprivation and Genetic Contributors

Individual differences in response to caffeine have long been recognized (see next section), and more recently, stable individual differences in response to sleep deprivation have also been confirmed, posing the question of whether individual differences in the two response domains share a common biological basis. We demonstrated experimentally that differential cognitive vulnerability to sleep deprivation is stable and trait-like, strongly suggesting a genetic component.149,150 In experiments involving repeated exposure to sleep deprivation in the same subjects, the intraclass correlation (ICC) coefficient, which expresses the proportion of variance that is explained by systematic inter-individual variability, revealed that trait-like responses accounted for 58-68% of the overall variance in PVT performance lapses of attention.150,151 Thus, healthy adults who had high lapse rates during sleep deprivation after one exposure had high lapse rates during a second exposure, and those with low lapse rates were similarly consistent.150,151 Because high ICCs were evident when subjects were exposed to 36h of total sleep deprivation repeatedly under markedly different state conditions (6h versus 12h sleep time per night for 7 days before the total sleep deprivation protocol), marked differences in neurobehavioral vulnerability to sleep deprivation can be characterized as phenotypic (trait-like, rather than state-specific).150,152

A recent study by Kuna et al. (2012)153 using monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs confirmed the trait-like feature of neurobehavioral vulnerability to sleep loss. The authors found that the ICC for PVT lapses over 38h of total sleep deprivation in MZ twin pairs was 56.2% whereas it was only 14.5% for DZ twins, showing that behavioral impairment produced by sleep deprivation is a highly heritable trait (a heritability, or the proportion on variance in a trait due to genes, of .83). Rupp et al. (2012)154 found in a small sample of healthy adults that those who had greater neurobehavioral deficits in response to acute total sleep deprivation also had greater deficits in response to chronic sleep restriction, indicating that the type of sleep deprivation did not alter one's neurobehavioral vulnerability to sleep loss, though more extensive studies are needed to confirm and extend this preliminary finding.

The discovery that more than 50% of the variance in performance deficits due to sleep loss is trait-like resulted in a search for markers to predict more- versus less-vulnerable individuals. Thus far, these phenotypic differences in neurobehavioral responses to sleep deprivation have not been predicted by demographic factors (e.g., age, sex, IQ), baseline cognitive performance, aspects of habitual sleep timing, circadian phase preference, or any other investigated factor.150,155 The apparent stability of these responses over time, as well as their high heritability, suggest an underlying genetic component. To this end, a number of studies have investigated the effects of genetic polymorphisms on individual differences in neurobehavioral vulnerability to sleep loss. Polymorphisms studied have included those in the circadian gene PERIOD3 (PER3),47,156-160 the human leukocyte antigen (HLA) gene DQB1*0602,161 the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) gene,162,163 the Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) gene,164 and various adenosine genes.160,165-168

Individual Differences in Response to Caffeine and Genetic Contributors

It has long been established that there are marked individual differences in sensitivity to caffeine.169 Twin studies have indicated substantial heritability in caffeine consumption, heavy caffeine use, caffeine intoxication, caffeine tolerance, and caffeine withdrawal, with heritabilities ranging from 0.35 to 0.77 for these measures.170-172 Similarly, Luciano et al. (2007)173 found substantial heritability (0.40) in caffeine's effects on sleep disturbance. In recent years, the genetic basis of individual differences in response to caffeine has been investigated; findings indicate that both pharmacodynamic (drug-receptor) and pharmacokinetic (metabolic) polymorphisms are associated with such differences.174

Variations in caffeine metabolism, accomplished mainly in the liver by the CYP1A2 enzyme, underlie some of the individual differences in response to caffeine. A polymorphism in the CYP1A2 gene that decreases enzyme inducibility results in slower metabolism.175 Indeed, slower caffeine metabolism is associated with a higher prevalence of this polymorphism in Asian and African populations (vs. Caucasians), and in women compared to men.175

Of interest, some of the same polymorphisms associated with individual differences in response to sleep loss (reviewed above) also appear to play a role in individual differences in response to caffeine. Two examples include polymorphisms found in the Adenosine A2A receptor (ADORA2A) gene and in the Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene. A polymorphism in the ADORA2A gene has been associated with caffeine consumption176, caffeine-induced anxiety177-179 and self-identified caffeine-sensitive individuals.166 This variation also contributes to individual differences in the effects of caffeine on subjective and objective sleep-related outcomes.166,180

Caffeine stimulates adrenomedullary secretion of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) enzyme.181 A Val158Met polymorphism181 in the COMT gene was associated with higher risk of acute myocardial infarction in middle-aged males who were heavy caffeine users (coffee drinkers) and had the lower, but not the higher activity COMT allele. We found that higher COMT activity subjects showed differentially smaller SWS increases and smaller reductions in stage 2 sleep during chronic sleep restriction, more stage 1 sleep across nights, and shorter baseline REM sleep latency—all indicative of a lower sleep homeostatic drive.163 By contrast, lower COMT activity subjects showed significantly less stage 1 sleep and a longer REM sleep latency at baseline and during chronic sleep restriction—indicative of a greater sleep homeostatic drive.166 Future studies should examine individual differences in caffeine response in the high and low COMT activity genotypes undergoing sleep loss.

Conclusion

The increasing prevalence of adults experiencing total sleep deprivation or chronic sleep restriction due to work, lifestyle, or biological factors has undoubtedly contributed to a growing market for CCEP that can increase alertness and mitigate declines in performance, especially for those individuals who are vulnerable to sleep loss. All of the top-selling energy drinks contain the central nervous stimulant caffeine, which can (within limits and to varying degrees) be an effective countermeasure for sleepiness and fatigue related to sleep loss, circadian rhythmicity, sleep inertia and time-on-task. The effects of other ingredients in CCED (e.g. taurine, glucose and B-vitamins) on cognitive and physical performance are reviewed elsewhere in this issue, but further research is required in this area.3

The U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has classified caffeine as ‘generally regarded as safe’ so its use as an additive is exempt from the Federal Food, Drug, and Cosmetic Act tolerance requirements.15 However, caffeine may be unsafe and its effects difficult to predict at higher doses, especially for individuals with certain genetic polymorphisms or with specific medical conditions (e.g., heart conditions, hypertension, diabetes, gastrointestinal issues). Recently, the FDA has expressed concerns over possible adverse effects related to cumulative caffeine intake since caffeine is added to an increasing number of products.182 As discussed in this review, we found that chronic administration of a low dose (0.3mg/kg per hour) of caffeine during the final 66 hours of 88 hours of sleep deprivation (with and without naps) led to increased core temperature and elevated plasma noradrenaline. Interestingly, when subjects were asked whether the pill they were administered in the previous hour contained caffeine or placebo, accurate perceptions were not significantly better than chance in both placebo and caffeine groups.125 In addition to being unable to detect small frequent doses of ingested caffeine, individuals often do not realize how many foods/drinks contain caffeine and underestimate how much caffeine they consume.183 Future studies should thoroughly investigate the physiological effects of cumulative caffeine intake in large, diverse populations.

It is important for caffeine consumers to understand that caffeine at any dose is not a chemical substitute for adequate healthy sleep. As the experimental results summarized in this review indicate, when the pressure for sleep is high, caffeine has little effect on preventing performance deficits and sleep attacks that can pose serious risks, especially in safety-sensitive areas (e.g., motor vehicle operation). Dinges and colleagues77,184 as well as others83,185 have shown that naps are effective countermeasures that prevent performance decline in situations of increased sleep propensity and decreased alertness. Although naps are effective and lack the physiological side effects of caffeine, one unwanted side effect is subsequent sleep inertia, which can be reduced by caffeine. Thus, brief naps in combination with properly-dosed and well-timed energy products containing caffeine may provide the most benefit.

Effective and safe use of caffeine to promote alertness requires an appreciation of one's responses to the stimulant (e.g., acute and chronic effects). After ingestion, peak blood concentrations of caffeine are reached within one hour and the half-life of caffeine is about 5-6 hours;186,187 however, as described above, there are significant individual differences in the response to caffeine, with half-life ranges varying between 2.5-10 hours (this range is even larger among individuals who are pregnant, are taking antidepressants, or have liver disease).188 If ingested in the evening, at night or prior to daytime naps, caffeine can disrupt sleep189-194 and perhaps phase delay circadian rhythms and thereby contribute to sleep loss and subsequent decreases in alertness. Finally, many adults develop tolerance to caffeine, so limiting its use may enhance its efficacy.195-198 If caffeine is to be used effectively to improve performance, it is wise to avoid consuming it when nonessential (i.e., as a readily available food/drink), and instead ingest it as a countermeasure only when needed.

Due to the 24-hour nature of modern society, situations leading to ‘low-energy’ will invariably arise and there is a need for effective countermeasures to maintain performance, especially where decrements in performance pose a high-risk for human safety. For healthy adults, CCED may be an effective and generally safe option when used strategically and especially in combination with naps.

Future Research Directions

As summarized in this review, a significant amount of research has characterized the effects of caffeine on neurobehavioral performance during situations of ‘low-energy’; however, the effects of caffeine on energy balance are less well understood. In addition to causing a decrease in alertness and an increase in fatigue, sleep loss and circadian disruption (due to habitual short sleep durations and/or shift work) have also been associated with weight gain and greater risk for obesity.199-201 We have recently shown that sleep restriction in the laboratory leads to weight gain and increased caloric intake particularly during late-night hours.202 It remains unclear how CCEP affect weight gain, metabolism and appetite under normal or ‘low-energy’ conditions. Caffeine has been associated with increased energy expenditure and lipolysis,203 greater satiety,204,205 and reduced weight gain206,207 suggesting its possible effectiveness for weight management. Unfortunately, caffeine is often ingested as part of sugar-sweetened beverages—all of the top-selling CCED contain sugar (although some brands offer sugar-free options) — which may actually promote weight gain and obesity.208 Future studies are critically needed to examine the efficacy of caffeine for attenuating the energy balance effects of sleep loss and circadian misalignment, particularly in light of our nation's growing obesity epidemic.

Acknowledgments

The time and effort required to write the review were supported by NIH grant R01 NR004281 (D.F. Dinges), NIH grant F31 AG044102 (A. M. Spaeth); Department of the Navy, Office of Naval Research Award No. N00014-11-1-0361 (N. Goel); and the National Space Biomedical Research Institute through NASA award NCC 9-58 (D.F. Dinges). David F. Dinges, Ph.D., is a compensated member of the Mars Science Advisory Council. He has not performed research for Mars or any of its affiliates on energy drinks. He is also compensated as Editor in Chief of the scientific journal SLEEP. The authors acknowledge Janet Mullington, Ph.D., Hans Van Dongen, Ph.D., Martin Szuba, M.D., Naomi L. Rogers, Ph.D., Greg Maislin, M.A., M.S., Shiv Kapoor, Ph.D., Melissa Mallis, Ph.D., Scott M. Doran, Ph.D., and Nicholas Price, who contributed to data collection and analyses of some of the experimental results presented in this review. The research data presented in the review were acquired through support from the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (grant F49620-95-1-0388), and by the National Institutes of Health grant (RR00040) to the General Clinical Research Center of the Hospital of Pennsylvania.

Footnotes

Andrea Spaeth and Namni Goel, Ph.D.have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Contributor Information

Andrea M Spaeth, Department of Psychology, School of Arts and Sciences, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104.

Namni Goel, Division of Sleep and Chronobiology, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104.

David F Dinges, Division of Sleep and Chronobiology, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104.

References

  • 1.Malinauskas BM, Aeby VG, Overton RF, Carpenter-Aeby T, Barber-Heidal KA. Survey of energy drink consumption patterns among college students. Nutr J. 2007;6:35. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-6-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Toblin RL, Clarke-Walper K, Kok BC, Sipos ML, Thomas JL. Energy drink consumption and its association with sleep problems among U.S. service members on a combat deployment - Afghanistan, 2010. Center for Disease Control and Prevention Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2012 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.McLellan TM, Lieberman HR. Do energy drinks contain active components other than caffeine? Nutr Rev. 2012;70:730–744. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2012.00525.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dinges DF. Stress, fatigue, and behavioral energy. Nutr Rev. 2001;59:S30–S32. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2001.tb01892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Seidl R, Peyrl A, Nicham R, Hauser E. A taurine and caffeine-containing drink stimulates cognitive performance and well-being. Amino Acids. 2000;19:635–642. doi: 10.1007/s007260070013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Alford C, Cox H, Wescott R. The effects of red bull energy drink on human performance and mood. Amino Acids. 2001;21:139–150. doi: 10.1007/s007260170021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. A glucose-caffeine ‘energy drink’ ameliorates subjective and performance deficits during prolonged cognitive demand. Appetite. 2004;42:331–333. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. Cognitive and physiological effects of an “energy drink”: an evaluation of the whole drink and of glucose, caffeine and herbal flavouring fractions. Psychopharmacology. 2004;176:320–330. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1935-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Smit HJ, Cotton JR, Hughes SC, Rogers PJ. Mood and cognitive performance effects of “energy” drink constituents: caffeine, glucose and carbonation. Nutr Neurosci. 2004;7:127–139. doi: 10.1080/10284150400003041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rao A, Hu H, Nobre AC. The effects of combined caffeine and glucose drinks on attention in the human brain. Nutr Neurosci. 2005;8:141–153. doi: 10.1080/10284150500096994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Childs E, de Wit H. Enhanced mood and psychomotor performance by a caffeine-containing energy capsule in fatigued individuals. Exp Clin Psychophar. 2008;16:13–21. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Howard MA, Marczinski CA. Acute effects of a glucose energy drink on behavioral control. Exp Clin Psychophar. 2010;18:553–561. doi: 10.1037/a0021740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sunram-Lea SI, Owen-Lynch J, Robinson SJ, Jones E, Hu H. The effect of energy drinks on cortisol levels, cognition and mood during a fire-fighting exercise. Psychopharmacology. 2012;219:83–97. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2379-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wesnes KA, Barrett ML, Udani JK. An evaluation of the cognitive and mood effects of an energy shot over a 6h period in volunteers: a randomized, double-blind, placebo controlled, cross-over study. Appetite. 2013;67:105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Heckman MA, Weil J, Gonzalez de Mejia E. Caffeine (1, 3, 7-trimethylxanthine) in foods: a comprehensive review on consumption, functionality, safety, and regulatory matters. J Food Sci. 2010;75:R77–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2010.01561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Giles GE, Mahoney CR, Brunye TT, Gardony AL, Taylor HA, Kanarek RB. Differential cognitive effects of energy drink ingredients: caffeine, taurine, and glucose. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2012;102:569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Peacock A, Martin FH, Carr A. Energy drink ingredients: contribution of caffeine and taurine to performance outcomes. Appetite. 2013;64:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pettitt RW, Niemeyer JD, Sexton PJ, Lipetzky A, Murray SR. Do the noncaffeine ingredients of energy drinks affect metabolic responses to heavy exercise? J Strength Cond Res. 2013;27:1994–1999. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3182736e31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nehlig A, Daval JL, Debry G. Caffeine and the central nervous system: mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychostimulant effects. Brain Res Rev. 1992;17:139–170. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(92)90012-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kamimori GH, Karyekar CS, Otterstetter R, et al. The rate of absorption and relative bioavailability of caffeine administered in chewing gum versus capsules to normal healthy volunteers. Int J Pharm. 2002;234:159–167. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5173(01)00958-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ribeiro JA, Sebastiao AM. Caffeine and adenosine. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20:S3–15. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Scharf MT, Naidoo N, Zimmerman JE, Pack AI. The energy hypothesis of sleep revisited. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;86:264–280. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Porkka-Heiskanen T, Kalinchuk AV. Adenosine, energy metabolism and sleep homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:123–135. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Benowitz NL. Clinical pharmacology of caffeine. Annu Rev Med. 1990;41:277–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev.me.41.020190.001425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Astrup A, Toubro S, Cannon S, Hein P, Breum L, Madsen J. Caffeine: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study of its thermogenic, metabolic, and cardiovascular effects in healthy volunteers. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;51:759–767. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/51.5.759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Borbely AA. Processes underlying sleep regulation. Horm Res. 1998;49:114–117. doi: 10.1159/000023156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Daan S, Beersma DGM, Borbely AA. Timing of human sleep - recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am J Physiol. 1984;246:R161–R178. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.2.R161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McCauley P, Kalachev LV, Smith AD, Belenky G, Dinges DF, Van Dongen HPA. A new mathematical model for the homeostatic effects of sleep loss on neurobehavioral performance. J Theor Biol. 2009;256:227–239. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cajochen C, Khalsa SB, Wyatt JK, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am J PhysiolReg Integ Comp Physiol. 1999;277:R640–R649. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.3.r640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Van Dongen HP, Vitellaro KM, Dinges DF. Individual differences in adult human sleep and wakefulness: leitmotif for a research agenda. Sleep. 2005;28:479–496. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.4.479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Krueger PM, Friedman EM. Sleep duration in the United States: A cross-sectional population-based study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169:1052–1063. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Knutson KL, Van Cauter E, Rathouz PJ, DeLeire T, Lauderdale DS. Trends in the prevalence of short sleepers in the USA: 1975-2006. Sleep. 2010;33:37–45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Luckhaupt SE, Tak S, Calvert GM. The prevalence of short sleep duration by industry and occupation in the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep. 2010;33:149–159. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.2.149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM, et al. American time use survey: sleep time and its relationship to waking activities. Sleep. 2007;30:1085–1095. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hale L. Who has time to sleep? J Public Health Med. 2005;27:205–211. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdi004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Virtanen M, Ferrie JE, Gimeno D, et al. Long working hours and sleep disturbances: the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Sleep. 2009;32:737–745. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.6.737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nakashima M, Morikawa Y, Sakurai M, et al. Association between long working hours and sleep problems in white-collar workers. J Sleep Res. 2010;20:110–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00852.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Aeschbach D, Cajochen C, Landolt H, Borbély AA. Homeostatic sleep regulation in habitual short sleepers and long sleepers. Am J Physiol Reg Integ Comp Physiol. 1996;270:R41–R53. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.270.1.R41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Klerman EB, Dijk DJ. Interindividual variation in sleep duration and its association with sleep debt in young adults. Sleep. 2005;28:1253–1259. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.10.1253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bradshaw DA, Yanagi MA, Pak ES, Peery TS, Ruff GA. Nightly sleep duration in the 2-week period preceding multiple sleep latency testing. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:613–619. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Cumulative effects of sleep restriction on daytime sleepiness. Psychophysiology. 1981;18:107–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02921.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997;20:267–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:1–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Bliese PD, Balkin TJ. Banking sleep: realization of benefits during subsequent sleep restriction and recovery. Sleep. 2009;32:311–321. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Banks S, Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep. 2010;33:1013–1026. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–126. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, Dinges DF. PER3 polymorphism predicts cumulative sleep homeostatic but not neurobehavioral changes to chronic partial sleep deprivation. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wu H, Stone WS, Hsi X, et al. Effects of different sleep restriction protocols on sleep architecture and daytime vigilance in healthy men. Physiol Res. 2010;59:821–829. doi: 10.33549/physiolres.931895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Thacher PV. University students and “the all nighter”: correlates and patterns of students' engagement in a single night of total sleep deprivation. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6:16–31. doi: 10.1080/15402000701796114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Baldwin DC, Daugherty SR. Sleep deprivation and fatigue in residency training: results of a national survey of first- and second-year residents. Sleep. 2004;27:217–223. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Philip P, Taillard J, Leger D, et al. Work and rest sleep schedules of 227 European truck drivers. Sleep Med. 2002;3:507–511. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(02)00138-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wright KP, Jr, Bogan RK, Wyatt JK. Shift work and the assessment and management of shift work disorder (SWD) Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17:41–54. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Rosenthal L, Roehrs TA, Rosen A, Roth T. Level of sleepiness and total sleep time following various time in bed conditions. Sleep. 1993;16:226–232. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.3.226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Harma M, Suvanto S, Popkin S, Pulli K, Mulder M, Hirvonen K. A dose-response study of total sleep time and the ability to maintain wakefulness. J Sleep Res. 1998;7:167–174. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00115.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Doran SM, Van Dongen HPA, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: Evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol. 2001;139:253–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Akerstedt T, Torsvall L, Gillberg M. Sleepiness in shiftwork - a review with emphasis on continuous monitoring of EEG and EOG. Chronobiol Int. 1987;4:129–140. doi: 10.3109/07420528709078519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Torsvall L, Akerstedt T. Sleepiness on the job: continuously measured EEG changes in train drivers. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1987;66:502–511. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90096-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature. 2005;437:1257–1263. doi: 10.1038/nature04284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kleitman N. Sleep and Wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago; 1963. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dinges DF, Kribbs NB. Performing while sleepy: effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. In: Monk TH, editor. Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance. Chister: Wiley; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Harrison Y, Horne JA. Occurrence of ‘microsleeps’ during daytime sleep onset in normal subjects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;98:411–416. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95612-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sadun AA, Schaechter JD, Smith LEH. A retinohypothalamic pathway in man - light mediation of circadian-rhythms. Brain Res. 1984;302:371–377. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90252-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ralph MR, Foster RG, Davis FC, Menaker M. Transplanted suprachiasmatic nucleus determines circadian period. Science. 1990;247:975–978. doi: 10.1126/science.2305266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Watts AG. The efferent projections of the suprachiasmatic nucleus: anatomical insights into the control of circadian rhythms. In: Klein DC, Moore RY, Reppert SM, editors. Suprachiasmatic nucleus: The mind's clock. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. pp. 77–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Czeisler CA. The effect of light on the human circadian pacemaker. Ciba Found Symp. 1995;183:254–290. doi: 10.1002/9780470514597.ch14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lucas RJ, Stirland JA, Darrow JM, Menaker M, Loudon ASI. Free running circadian rhythms of melatonin, luteinizing hormone, and cortisol in Syrian hamsters bearing the circadian tau mutation. Endocrinology. 1999;140:758–764. doi: 10.1210/endo.140.2.6538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Aschoff J, Wever R. The circadian system of man. In: Aschoff J, editor. Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Vol. 4. New York: Plenum Press; 1981. pp. 311–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Shanahan TL, Czeisler CA. Light exposure induces equivalent phase-shifts of the endogenous circadian-rhythms of circulating plasma melatonin and core body-temperature in men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1991;73:227–235. doi: 10.1210/jcem-73-2-227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science. 1999;284:2177–2181. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5423.2177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lack LC, Lushington K. The rhythms of human sleep propensity and core body temperature. J Sleep Res. 1996;5:1–11. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1996.00005.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Monk TH. The post-lunch dip in performance. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24:e15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Shekleton JA, Rajaratnam SM, Gooley JJ, Van Reen E, Czeisler CA, Lockley SW. Improved neurobehavioral performance during the wake maintenance zone. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9:353–362. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.2588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Goel N, Basner M, Rao H, Dinges DF. Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and human performance. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci. 2013;119:155–190. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00007-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Langdon DE, Hartman B. Technical documentary report. 62-17. USAF School of Aerospace Medicine; 1961. Performance upon sudden awakening; p. 8p. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lubin A, Hord DJ, Tracy ML, Johnson LC. Effects of exercise, bedrest and napping on performance decrement during 40 hours. Psychophysiology. 1976;13:334–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb03086.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Dinges DF, Orne EC, Evans FJ, Orne MT. Performance after naps in sleep-conducive and alerting environments. In: Johnson LC, Tepas DI, Colquhoun WP, Colligan MJ, editors. Biological rhythms, sleep and shift work. New York: Spectrum; 1981. pp. 539–552. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Jewett ME, Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Khalsa SB, Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Time course of sleep inertia dissipation in human performance and alertness. J Sleep Res. 1999;8:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1999.00128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wertz AT, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP. Effects of sleep inertia on cognition. JAMA. 2006;295:163–164. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Tassi P, Muzet A. Sleep inertia. Sleep Med Rev. 2000 Aug;4:341–353. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2000.0098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Lovato N, Lack L. The effects of napping on cognitive functioning. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:155–166. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00009-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Matchock RL. Circadian and sleep episode duration influences on cognitive performance following the process of awakening. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2010;93:129–151. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(10)93006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lavie P, Weler B. Timing of naps: effects on post-nap sleepiness levels. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1989;72:218–224. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90246-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Dinges DF, Orne MT, Orne EC. Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations. Behav Res Meth Inst Comp. 1985;17:37–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Webb WB, Agnew H. Reaction time and serial response efficiency on arousal from sleep. Percept Mot Skills. 1964;18:783–784. doi: 10.2466/pms.1964.18.3.783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Feltin M, Broughton R. Differential effects of arousal from slow wave versus REM sleep. Psychophysiology. 1968;5:231. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Mackworth JF. Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. Psychol Rev. 1968;75:308–322. doi: 10.1037/h0025896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Frankmann JP, Adams JA. Theories of vigilance. Psychol Bull. 1962;59:257–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Grier RA, Warm JS, Dember WN, Matthews G, Galinsky TL, Parasuraman R. The vigilance decrement reflects limitations in effortful attention, not mindlessness. Hum Factors. 2003;45:349–359. doi: 10.1518/hfes.45.3.349.27253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Smit AS, Eling PA, Coenen AM. Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychol. 2004;115:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Warm JS, Parasuraman R, Matthews G. Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Hum Factors. 2008;50:433–441. doi: 10.1518/001872008X312152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Lorist MM, Boksem MA, Ridderinkhof KR. Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;24:199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Lim J, Wu WC, Wang JJ, Detre JA, Dinges DF, Rao HY. Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: an ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. Neuroimage. 2010;49:3426–3435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Faber LG, Maurits NM, Lorist MM. Mental fatigue affects visual selective attention. PLoS One. 2012;7:e48073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Foxe JJ, Morie KP, Laud PJ, Rowson MJ, de Bruin EA, Kelly SP. Assessing the effects of caffeine and theanine on the maintenance of vigilance during a sustained attention task. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:2320–2327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Wilkinson RT. Effects of up to 60 hours sleep-deprivation on different types of work. Ergonomics. 1964;7:175–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Angus RG, Heslegrave RJ. Effects of sleep loss on sustained cognitive performance during a command and control simulation. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput. 1985;17:55–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Steyvers FJ, Gaillard AW. The effects of sleep deprivation and incentives on human performance. Psychol Res. 1993;55:64–70. doi: 10.1007/BF00419894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Wesensten NJ, Belenky G, Thorne DR, Kautz MA, Balkin TJ. Modafinil vs. caffeine: effects on fatigue during sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:520–525. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Valdez P, Ramirez C, Garcia A, Talamantes J, Cortez J. Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained attention. Chronobiol Int. 2010;27:393–416. doi: 10.3109/07420521003765861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Bergum BO, Lehr DJ. Vigilance performance as a function of interpolated rest. J Appl Psychol. 1962;46:425–427. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Loke WH. Effects of caffeine on mood and memory. Physiol Behav. 1988;44:367–372. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90039-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Kelly TL, Mitler MM, Bonnet MH. Sleep latency measures of caffeine effects during sleep deprivation. Electroen Clin Neuro. 1997;102:397–400. doi: 10.1016/s0921-884x(97)96135-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Wright KP, Badia P, Myers BL, Plenzler SC. Combination of bright light and caffeine as a countermeasure for impaired alertness and performance during extended sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 1997;6:26–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Beaumont M, Batejat D, Pierard C, et al. Slow release caffeine and prolonged (64-h) continuous wakefulness: effects on vigilance and cognitive performance. J Sleep Res. 2001;10:265–276. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00266.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Lieberman HR, Tharion WJ, Shukitt-Hale B, Speckman KL, Tulley R. Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during US Navy SEAL training. Psychopharmacology. 2002;164:250–261. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1217-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Wesensten NJ, Belenky G, Kautz MA, Thorne DR, Reichardt RM, Balkin TJ. Maintaining alertness and performance during sleep deprivation: modafinil versus caffeine. Psychopharmacology. 2002;159:238–247. doi: 10.1007/s002130100916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Kamimori GH, Johnson D, Thorne D, Belenky G. Multiple caffeine doses maintain vigilance during early morning operations. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76:1046–1050. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.McLellan TM, Kamimori GH, Bell DG, Smith IF, Johnson D, Belenky G. Caffeine maintains vigilance and marksmanship in simulated urban operations with sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76:39–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Wesensten NJ, Killgore WDS, Balkin TJ. Performance and alertness effects of caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:255–266. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00468.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Killgore WDS, Rupp TL, Grugle NL, Reichardt RM, Lipizzi EL, Balkin TJ. Effects of dextroamphetamine, caffeine and modafinil on psychomotor vigilance test performance after 44 h of continuous wakefulness. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:309–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00654.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Rosenthal L, Roehrs T, Zwyghuizendoorenbos A, Plath D, Roth T. Alerting effects of caffeine after normal and restricted sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1991;4:103–108. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Smith AP, Clark R, Gallagher J. Breakfast cereal and caffeinated coffee: effects on working memory, attention, mood, and cardiovascular function. Physiol Behav. 1999;67:9–17. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Reyner LA, Horne JA. Early morning driver sleepiness: effectiveness of 200 mg caffeine. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:251–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Maridakis V, Herring MP, O'Connor PJ. Sensitivity to change in cognitive performance and mood measures of energy and fatigue in response to differing doses of caffeine or breakfast. Int J Neuro. 2009;119:975–994. doi: 10.1080/00207450802333995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Adan A, Serra-Grabulosa JM. Effects of caffeine and glucose, alone and combined, on cognitive performance. Hum Psychopharm Clin. 2010;25:310–317. doi: 10.1002/hup.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Souissi M, Abedelmalek S, Chtourou H, Atheymen R, Hakim A, Sahnoun Z. Effects of morning caffeine' ingestion on mood states, simple reaction time, and short-term maximal performance on elite judoists. Asian J Sports Med. 2012;3:161–168. doi: 10.5812/asjsm.34607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Smith AP, Rusted JM, Eatonwilliams P, Savory M, Leathwood P. Effects of caffeine given before and after lunch on sustained attention. Neuropsychobiology. 1990;23:160–163. doi: 10.1159/000119444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Smith AP, Rusted JM, Savory M, Eaton-Williams P, Hall SR. The effects of caffeine, impulsivity and time of day on performance, mood and cardiovascular function. J Psychopharmacol. 1991;5:120–128. doi: 10.1177/026988119100500205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Borland RG, Rogers AS, Nicholson AN, Pascoe PA, Spencer MB. Performance overnight in shiftworkers operating a day-night schedule. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1986;57:241–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Muehlbach MJ, Walsh JK. The effects of caffeine on simulated night-shift work and subsequent daytime sleep. Sleep. 1995;18:22–29. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Jay SM, Petrilli RM, Ferguson SA, Dawson D, Lamond N. The suitability of a caffeinated energy drink for night-shift workers. Physiol Beh. 2006;87:925–931. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Wyatt JK, Cajochen C, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. Low-dose repeated caffeine administration for circadian-phase-dependent performance degradation during extended wakefulness. Sleep. 2004;27:374–381. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.3.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Shearer WT, Reuben JM, Mullington JM, et al. Soluble TNF-alpha receptor 1 and IL-6 plasma levels in humans subjected to the sleep deprivation model of spaceflight. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2001;107:165–170. doi: 10.1067/mai.2001.112270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Van Dongen HP, Price NJ, Mullington JM, Szuba MP, Kapoor SC, Dinges DF. Caffeine eliminates psychomotor vigilance deficits from sleep inertia. Sleep. 2001;24:813–819. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.7.813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Einother SJ, Giesbrecht T. Caffeine as an attention enhancer: reviewing existing assumptions. Psychopharmacology. 2013;225:251–274. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2917-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Dinges DF, Powell JW. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput. 1985;17:652–655. [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corp; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Dinges DF, Kribbs NB, Bates BL, Carlin MC. A very brief probed-recall memory task: Sensitivity to sleep loss. Sleep Res (APSS Abstract) 1993;22:330. [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Wesensten NJ, Picchioni D, Balkin TJ. Caffeine gum minimizes sleep inertia. Percept Mot Skills. 2013;116:280–293. doi: 10.2466/29.22.25.PMS.116.1.280-293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Hayashi M, Masuda A, Hori T. The alerting effects of caffeine, bright light and face washing after a short daytime nap. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:2268–2278. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00255-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Smith AP. Caffeine at work. Hum Psychopharm Clin. 2005;20:441–445. doi: 10.1002/hup.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. The use of prophylactic naps and caffeine to maintain performance during a continuous operation. Ergonomics. 1994;37:1009–1020. doi: 10.1080/00140139408963714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Impact of naps and caffeine on extended nocturnal performance. Physiol Behav. 1994;56:103–109. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90266-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Horne JA, Reyner LA. Counteracting driver sleepiness: effects of napping, caffeine, and placebo. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:306–309. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00428.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Reyner LA, Horne JA. Efficacy of a ‘functional energy drink’ in counteracting driver sleepiness. Physiol Behav. 2002;75:331–335. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00669-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Zatz M, Heath JR. Calcium and photoentrainment in chick pineal cells revisited: effects of caffeine, thapsigargin, EGTA, and light on the melatonin rhythm. J Neurochem. 1995;65:1332–1341. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.65031332.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Pelissier AL, Gantenbein M, Bruguerolle B. Caffeine-induced modifications of heart rate, temperature, and motor activity circadian rhythms in rats. Physiol Behav. 1999;67:81–88. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00038-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Antle MC, Steen NM, Mistlberger RE. Adenosine and caffeine modulate circadian rhythms in the Syrian hamster. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2901–2905. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200109170-00029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Ding JM, Buchanan GF, Tischkau SA, et al. A neuronal ryanodine receptor mediates light-induced phase delays of the circadian clock. Nature. 1998;394:381–384. doi: 10.1038/28639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Oike H, Kobori M, Suzuki T, Ishida N. Caffeine lengthens circadian rhythms in mice. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2011;410:654–658. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2011.06.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Diaz-Munoz M, Dent MA, Granados-Fuentes D, et al. Circadian modulation of the ryanodine receptor type 2 in the SCN of rodents. Neuroreport. 1999;10:481–486. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199902250-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Watanabe A, Moriya T, Nisikawa Y, et al. Adenosine A1-receptor agonist attenuates the light-induced phase shifts and fos expression in vivo and optic nerve stimulation-evoked field potentials in the suprachiasmatic nucleus in vitro. Brain Res. 1996;740:329–336. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(96)00881-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Elliott KJ, Todd Weber E, Rea MA. Adenosine A1 receptors regulate the response of the hamster circadian clock to light. Eur J Pharmacol. 2001;414:45–53. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(01)00786-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Sigworth LA, Rea MA. Adenosine A1 receptors regulate the response of the mouse circadian clock to light. Brain Res. 2003;960:246–251. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03896-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Feldman JF. Circadian periodicity in a neurospora: alteration by inhibitors of cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase. Science. 1975;190:789–790. doi: 10.1126/science.173018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Sherman H, Gutman R, Chapnik N, Meylan J, le Coutre J, Froy O. Caffeine alters circadian rhythms and expression of disease and metabolic markers. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 2011;43:829–838. doi: 10.1016/j.biocel.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Wright KP, Badia P, Myers BL, Plenzler SC, Hakel M. Caffeine and light effects on nighttime melatonin and temperature levels in sleep-deprived humans. Brain Res. 1997;747:78–84. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(96)01268-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Goel N. Genetics of sleep timing, duration and homeostasis in humans. Sleep Med Clin. 2011;6:171–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2011.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Van Dongen HPA, Baynard MD, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27:423–433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Dealing with inter-individual differences in the temporal dynamics of fatigue and performance: importance and techniques. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75(3 Suppl):A147–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Goel N, Dinges DF. Predicting risk in space: genetic markers for differential vulnerability to sleep restriction. Acta Astronaut. 2012;77:207–213. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2012.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Kuna ST, Maislin G, Pack FM, et al. Heritability of performance deficit accumulation during acute sleep deprivation in twins. Sleep. 2012;35:1223–1233. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Balkin TJ. Trait-like vulnerability to total and partial sleep loss. Sleep. 2012;35:1163–1172. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Goel N, Dinges DF. Behavioral and genetic markers of sleepiness. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7:S19–21. doi: 10.5664/JCSM.1348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Groeger JA, Viola AU, Lo JC, von Schantz M, Archer SN, Dijk DJ. Early morning executive functioning during sleep deprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism. Sleep. 2008;31:1159–1167. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Viola AU, Archer SN, James LM, et al. PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr Biol. 2007;17:613–618. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Vandewalle G, Archer SN, Wuillaume C, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7948–7956. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0229-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Lo JC, Groeger JA, Santhi N, et al. Effects of partial and acute total sleep deprivation on performance across cognitive domains, individuals and circadian phase. PLoS One. 2012;7:e45987. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Newman R, Balkin TJ. PER3 and ADORA2A polymorphisms impact neurobehavioral performance during sleep restriction. J Sleep Res. 2013;22:160–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, Dinges DF. DQB1*0602 predicts interindividual differences in physiologic sleep, sleepiness, and fatigue. Neurology. 2010;75:1509–1519. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f9615d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Bodenmann S, Xu S, Luhmann UF, et al. Pharmacogenetics of modafinil after sleep loss: catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype modulates waking functions but not recovery sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2009;85:296–304. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2008.222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Goel N, Banks S, Lin L, Mignot E, Dinges DF. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism associates with individual differences in sleep physiologic responses to chronic sleep loss. PLoS One. 2011;6:e29283. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Bachmann V, Klein C, Bodenmann S, et al. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism modulates sleep intensity: EEG frequency- and state-specificity. Sleep. 2012;35:335–344. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Retey JV, Adam M, Honegger E, et al. A functional genetic variation of adenosine deaminase affects the duration and intensity of deep sleep in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:15676–15681. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505414102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Retey JV, Adam M, Khatami R, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2007;81:692–698. doi: 10.1038/sj.clpt.6100102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Bachmann V, Klaus F, Bodenmann S, et al. Functional ADA polymorphism increases sleep depth and reduces vigilant attention in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:962–970. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Bodenmann S, Hohoff C, Freitag C, et al. Polymorphisms of ADORA2A modulate psychomotor vigilance and the effects of caffeine on neurobehavioural performance and sleep EEG after sleep deprivation. Br J Pharmacol. 2012;165:1904–1913. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2011.01689.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Hollingworth H. The influence of caffeine alkaloid on the quality and amount of sleep. Am J Psychol. 1912;23:89–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Hettema JM, Corey LA, Kendler KS. A multivariate genetic analysis of the use of tobacco, alcohol, and caffeine in a population based sample of male and female twins. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;57:69–78. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00053-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Caffeine intake, tolerance, and withdrawal in women: a population-based twin study. Am J Psychiat. 1999;156:223–228. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.2.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Luciano M, Kirk KM, Heath AC, Martin NG. The genetics of tea and coffee drinking and preference for source of caffeine in a large community sample of Australian twins. Addiction. 2005;100:1510–1517. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01223.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Luciano M, Zhu G, Kirk KM, et al. “No thanks, it keeps me awake”: the genetics of coffee-attributed sleep disturbance. Sleep. 2007;30:1378–1386. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.10.1378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Yang A, Palmer AA, de Wit H. Genetics of caffeine consumption and responses to caffeine. Psychopharmacology. 2010;211:245–257. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1900-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Gunes A, Dahl ML. Variation in CYP1A2 activity and its clinical implications: influence of environmental factors and genetic polymorphisms. Pharmacogenomics. 2008;9:625–637. doi: 10.2217/14622416.9.5.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Cornelis MC, El-Sohemy A, Campos H. Genetic polymorphism of the adenosine A2A receptor is associated with habitual caffeine consumption. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:240–244. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.1.240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Alsene K, Deckert J, Sand P, de Wit H. Association between A2a receptor gene polymorphisms and caffeine-induced anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1694–1702. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Childs E, Hohoff C, Deckert J, Xu K, Badner J, de Wit H. Association between ADORA2A and DRD2 polymorphisms and caffeine-induced anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2791–2800. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Rogers PJ, Hohoff C, Heatherley SV, et al. Association of the anxiogenic and alerting effects of caffeine with ADORA2A and ADORA1 polymorphisms and habitual level of caffeine consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1973–1983. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Byrne EM, Johnson J, McRae AF, et al. A genome-wide association study of caffeine-related sleep disturbance: confirmation of a role for a common variant in the adenosine receptor. Sleep. 2012;35:967–975. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Happonen P, Voutilainen S, Tuomainen TP, Salonen JT. Catechol-o-methyltransferase gene polymorphism modifies the effect of coffee intake on incidence of acute coronary events. PLoS One. 2006;1:e117. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Taylor MR. FDA to Investigate Added Caffeine. Food and Drug Administration Consumer Health Information. 2013 [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Snel J, Lorist MM. Effects of caffeine on sleep and cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2011;190:105–117. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00006-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Dinges DF, Orne MT, Whitehouse WG, Orne EC. Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. Sleep. 1987;10:313–329. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Ficca G, Axelsson J, Mollicone DJ, Muto V, Vitiello MV. Naps, cognition and performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:249–258. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Statland BE, Demas TJ. Serum caffeine half-lives - healthy-subjects vs patients having alcoholic hepatic-disease. Am J Clin Pathol. 1980;73:390–393. doi: 10.1093/ajcp/73.3.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Hammami MM, Al-Gaai EA, Alvi S, Hammami MB. Interaction between drug and placebo effects: a cross-over balanced placebo design trial. Trials. 2010;11:1–10. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-11-110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Magkos F, Kavouras SA. Caffeine use in sports, pharmacokinetics in man, and cellular mechanisms of action. Crit Rev Food Sci. 2005;45:535–562. doi: 10.1080/1040-830491379245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Hindmarch I, Rigney U, Stanley N, Quinlan P, Rycroft J, Lane J. A naturalistic investigation of the effects of day-long consumption of tea, coffee and water on alertness, sleep onset and sleep quality. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149:203–216. doi: 10.1007/s002130000383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Roehrs T, Roth T. Caffeine: sleep and daytime sleepiness. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12:153–162. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e1005–1010. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Carrier J, Paquet J, Fernandez-Bolanos M, et al. Effects of caffeine on daytime recovery sleep: A double challenge to the sleep-wake cycle in aging. Sleep Med. 2009;10:1016–1024. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Paterson LM, Nutt DJ, Ivarsson M, Hutson PH, Wilson SJ. Effects on sleep stages and microarchitecture of caffeine and its combination with zolpidem or trazodone in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharm. 2009;23:487–494. doi: 10.1177/0269881109104852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Lodato F, Araujo J, Barros H, et al. Caffeine intake reduces sleep duration in adolescents. Nutr Res. 2013;33:726–732. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Judelson DA, Armstrong LE, Sokmen B, Roti MW, Casa DJ, Kellogg MD. Effect of chronic caffeine intake on choice reaction time, mood, and visual vigilance. Physiol Behav. 2005;85:629–634. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Addicott MA, Laurienti PJ. A comparison of the effects of caffeine following abstinence and normal caffeine use. Psychopharmacology. 2009;207:423–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1668-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Kennedy DO, Haskell CF. Cerebral blood flow and behavioural effects of caffeine in habitual and non-habitual consumers of caffeine: a near infrared spectroscopy study. Biol Psychol. 2011;86:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Rogers PJ, Heatherley SV, Mullings EL, Smith JE. Faster but not smarter: effects of caffeine and caffeine withdrawal on alertness and performance. Psychopharmacology. 2013;226:229–240. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2889-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Lucassen EA, Rother KI, Cizza G. Interacting epidemics? Sleep curtailment, insulin resistance, and obesity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1264:110–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06655.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Antunes LC, Levandovski R, Dantas G, Caumo W, Hidalgo MP. Obesity and shift work: chronobiological aspects. Nutr Res Rev. 2010;23:155–168. doi: 10.1017/S0954422410000016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Allison KC, Goel N, Ahima RS. Delayed timing of eating: impact on weight and metabolism. Curr Obes Rep. 2013 doi: 10.1007/s13679-013-0084-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Spaeth AM, Dinges DF, Goel N. Effects of experimental sleep restriction on weight gain, caloric intake, and meal timing in healthy adults. Sleep. 2013;36:981–990. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Greenberg JA, Boozer CN, Geliebter A. Coffee, diabetes, and weight control. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84:682–693. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/84.4.682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Kovacs EM, Lejeune MP, Nijs I, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Effects of green tea on weight maintenance after body-weight loss. Br J Nutr. 2004;91:431–437. doi: 10.1079/BJN20041061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Lejeune MP, Kovacs EM. Body weight loss and weight maintenance in relation to habitual caffeine intake and green tea supplementation. Obes Res. 2005;13:1195–1204. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Greenberg JA, Axen KV, Schnoll R, Boozer CN. Coffee, tea and diabetes: the role of weight loss and caffeine. Int J Obes. 2005;29:1121–1129. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Lopez-Garcia E, van Dam RM, Rajpathak S, Willett WC, Manson JE, Hu FB. Changes in caffeine intake and long-term weight change in men and women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:674–680. doi: 10.1093/ajcn.83.3.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Bray GA, Popkin BM. Calorie-sweetened beverages and fructose: what have we learned 10 years later. Pediatric Obesity. 2013;8:242–248. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2013.00171.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES