Bellebaum, C., Jokisch, D., Gizewski, E., Forsting, M., & Daum, I. (2012). The neural coding of expected and unexpected monetary performance outcomes: Dissociations between active and observational learning. Behavioural Brain Research, 227(1), 241–251. |
Participants had to learn by observing the performance and outcomes of another subject. For each trial, they observed another subject choose between two stimuli and receive monetary feedback, such as monetary reward (20 cents) or non-reward (neither reward nor punishment). |
Money |
Observational feedback learning task: Reward outcome > Non-reward outcome |
Personal correspondence |
Braams, B. R., Güroğlu, B., de Water, E., Meuwese, R., Koolschijn, P. C., Peper, J. S., & Crone, E. A. (2013). Reward- related neural responses are dependent on the beneficiary. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nst077. |
Participants could win money for their best friend in a gambling task. |
Money |
Friend gain > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Burke, C. J., Tobler, P. N., Baddeley, M., & Schultz, W. (2010). Neural mechanisms of observational learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(32), 14431–14436. |
Participants could learn by observing the performance and outcomes of another subject. During the gain sessions, they observed another subject choose between two stimuli and receive a reward (10 points) or non-reward (0 points). |
Points |
Full observational learning during gain session: 10-point gain > 0-point gain |
Personal correspondence |
Chester, D. S., Powell, C. A., Smith, R. H., Joseph, J. E., Kedia, G., Combs, D. J., & DeWall, C. N. (2013). Justice for the average Joe: The role of envy and the mentalizing network in the deservingness of others’ misfortunes. Social Neuroscience, 8(6), 640–649. |
Participants observed that non-enviable targets had been accepted into a prestigious student program (i.e., good fortune). |
Acceptance into student program |
Good fortune for low envy targets > Baseline |
Personal correspondence |
Hamilton, J. P., Chen, M. C., Waugh, C. E., Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2014). Distinctive and common neural underpinnings of major depression, social anxiety, and their comorbidity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsu084. |
Participants passively listened to positive affective statements (praise) directed at another person. |
Positive social feedback |
Healthy controls only: Other positive > Baseline |
Personal correspondence |
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., Knoepfle, D. T., & Rangel, A. (2010). Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 583–590. doi: 30/2/583 [pii] 0.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-09.2010 |
Participants made donations to different charities. In forced donation trials subjects were instructed how much they had to donate in that trial ($0 – $100 in $5 increments) and had to move a slider to the mandated amount (i.e. forced response). |
Money |
For all amounts except $0: Forced response > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Miyakawa, A., Knight, R. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2008). The influence of personality on neural mechanisms of observational fear and reward learning. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 2709–2724. |
Participants learned object-emotion associations by observing whether a woman reacted with a happy or neutral expression to a neutral object. |
Object |
Learn happy > Learn neutral |
Table 6 |
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron, 58(2), 284–294. |
Participants saw blocks of all positive words showing what others thought of another person (i.e. high social reputation) or saw no feedback about the other person (i.e. no social reputation). |
Positive social feedback |
Other high social reputation > Other no social reputation |
Personal correspondence |
Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage, 34(4), 1744–1753. |
Participants saw videos of people drinking pleasant and neutral liquids. |
Juice |
Pleasant > Neutral |
Personal correspondence |
Jung, D., Sul, S., & Kim, H. (2013). Dissociable neural processes underlying risky decisions for self versus other. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7. |
Participants were asked to perform a gambling task on behalf of another person (decision-for-other condition). These decisions sometimes resulted in a person winning either 10 points or 90 points. Points were converted to money. |
Points/Money |
Other Win > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Kätsyri, J., Hari, R., Ravaja, N., & Nummenmaa, L. (2013). Just watching the game ain’t enough: striatal fMRI reward responses to successes and failures in a video game during active and vicarious playing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. |
Participants watched a pre-recorded gameplay video (vicarious playing) and observed another player’s successes (wins) and failures (losses). |
Video game wins |
Vicarious play: Win > Loss |
Personal correspondence |
Korn, C. W., Prehn, K., Park, S. Q., Walter, H., & Heekeren, H. R. (2012). Positively biased processing of self-relevant social feedback. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16832–16844. |
Participants observed others receive desirable feedback about their personality traits. |
Positive social feedback |
Other desirable feedback > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Meffert, H., Gazzola, V., den Boer, J. A., Bartels, A. A., & Keysers, C. (2013). Reduced spontaneous but relatively normal deliberate vicarious representations in psychopathy. Brain, 136(8), 2550–2562. |
Participants watched videos of others’ hands receiving loving or neutral touch. |
Loving touch |
Healthy controls only: Observe loving touch > Observe neutral touch |
Personal correspondence |
Meshi, D., Morawetz, C., & Heekeren, H. R. (2013). Nucleus accumbens response to gains in reputation for the self relative to gains for others predicts social media use. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.
|
Participants observed others receive positive feedback about their personality traits or no feedback. |
Positive social feedback |
Other high positive feedback > Other no feedback |
Personal correspondence |
Molenberghs, P., Bosworth, R., Nott, Z., Louis, W. R., Smith, J. R., Amiot, C. E., Decety, J. (2014). The influence of group membership and individual differences in psychopathy and perspective taking on neural responses when punishing and rewarding others. Human Brain Mapping.
|
Participants gave monetary rewards or nothing (neutral) to in-group members during a trivia game. |
Money |
Reward in-group > Neutral in-group |
Personal correspondence |
Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2014). The neural components of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 39–47. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss088 |
Participants were asked to empathize with photos of others’ happy events (e.g., getting engaged) and to view others’ neutral events (e.g., ironing). |
Positive emotional events |
Happy empathize > Neutral |
Table 1 |
Morrison, I., Björnsdotter, M., & Olausson, H. (2011). Vicarious responses to social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing speeds. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(26), 9554–9562. Morrison |
Participants observed brush strokes on another person’s arm at two different speeds: 3 cm/s (pleasant) and 30 cm/s (neutral). |
Pleasant touch |
Study 1: Seen 3 > Seen 30 |
Table 1 |
Morrison, I., Björnsdotter, M., & Olausson, H. (2011). Vicarious responses to social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing speeds. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(26), 9554–9562. |
Participants observed brush strokes on another person’s arm at two different speeds: 3 cm/s (pleasant) and 30 cm/s (neutral). |
Pleasant touch |
Study 2: Seen 3 > Seen 30 |
Table 2 |
Perry, D., Hendler, T., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2012). Can we share the joy of others? Empathic neural responses to distress vs joy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(8), 909–916. |
Participants read sentences depicting everyday positive emotional events occurring to a fictional character. |
Positive emotional events |
Other positive > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). An integrative model of the neural systems supporting the comprehension of observed emotional behavior. Neuroimage, 59(3), 3050–3059. doi: S1053-8119(11)01166-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.005 |
Participants watched video clips of others’ experiencing positive emotions and were asked to imagine why they felt that way. They also did a shape matching task which served as a neutral condition. |
Positive emotional events |
Positive Why task > Shape matching |
Personal correspondence |
Telzer, E. H., Masten, C. L., Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2010). Gaining while giving: An fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance among White and Latino youth. Social Neuroscience, 5(5–6), 508–518.” |
Participants choose whether to accept or reject a payment option that affected their own and their family’s endowment. One type of payment included a noncostly-donation to the family (e.g., YOU –$0.00 FAM +$3.00). |
Money |
Noncostly donation > Fixation |
Personal correspondence |
Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Galván, A. (2013). Ventral striatum activation to prosocial rewards predicts longitudinal declines in adolescent risk taking. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 45–52. |
Paritcipants choose whether to accept or reject a payment option that affected their own and their family’s endowment. One type of payment included a noncostly-donation to the family (e.g., YOU –$0.00 FAM +$3.00). For the control condition, YOU and FAM were presented without a financial gain or loss. |
Money |
Noncostly donation > Control |
Personal correspondence |
Tricomi, E., Rangel, A., Camerer, C. F., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Neural evidence for inequality-averse social preferences. Nature, 463(7284), 1089–1091. |
Inequality was created by recruiting pairs of subjects and giving one of them a large monetary endowment (i.e. high-pay player). This high-pay player then evaluated monetary transfers from the experimenter to the other participant (i.e. low-pay player). |
Money |
High-pay player: Payments to low-pay player > Control |
Personal correspondence |
Varnum, M. E., Shi, Z., Chen, A., Qiu, J., & Han, S. (2014). When “Your” reward is the same as “My” reward: Self-construal priming shifts neural responses to own vs. friends’ rewards. Neuro Image, 87, 164–169. |
Experimenters manipulated participants’ self-construal (independent vs. interdependent). Participants then played a game in which they could win money for a friend during a gambling game. |
Money |
Interdependent prime for main task: Friend win > Neutral |
Personal correspondence |
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My Insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40(3), 655–664. |
Participants watch videos of other people smelling pleasant odors. |
Pleasant odors |
Observation of pleasure > Neutral |
Personal correspondence |