Abstract
The piriform cortex (PC) is richly innervated by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and serotonin (5-HT) containing axons arising from central amygdala and Raphe nucleus. CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/2CRs have been shown to interact in manner where CRFR activation subsequently potentiates the activity of 5-HT2A/2CRs. The purpose of this study was to determine how the activation of CRFR1 and/or 5-HT2Rs modulates PC activity at both the circuit and cellular level. Voltage sensitive dye imaging showed that CRF acting through CRFR1 dampened activation of the Layer II of PC and interneurons of endopiriform nucleus. Application of the selective 5-HT2A/CR agonist 2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI) following CRFR1 activation potentiated this effect. Blocking the interaction between CRFR1 and 5-HT2R with a Tat-CRFR1-CT peptide abolished this potentiation. Application of forskolin did not mimic CRFR1 activity but instead blocked it, while a protein kinase A antagonist had no effect. However, activation and antagonism of protein kinase C (PKC) either mimicked or blocked CRF modulation, respectively. DOI had no effect when applied alone indicating that the prior activation of CRFR1 receptors was critical for DOI to show significant effects similar to CRF. Patch clamp recordings showed that both CRF and DOI reduced the synaptic responsiveness of Layer II pyramidal neurons. CRF had highly variable effects on interneurons within Layer III, both increasing and decreasing their excitability, but DOI had no effect on the excitability of this group of neurons. These data show that CRF and 5-HT, acting through both CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CRs, reduce the activation of the PC. This modulation may be an important blunting mechanism of stressor behaviors mediated through the olfactory cortex.
Keywords: piriform cortex, corticotropin-releasing factor, 5-hydroxytryptamine receptors, corticotropin releasing factor 1 receptor, dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine, adapting high frequency, adapting low frequency, non-adapting very high frequency
Introduction
In the central nervous system, CRF binds with high affinity to CRF1 receptors (CRFR1) but with only moderate affinity to CRFR2 (Zorrilla et al., 2014). CRF within CNS originates from cell bodies in central amygdala as well as interneurons in the cortex (Anisman et al., 2007). CRFR1 receptor activation has been shown to have a number of differing effects on cellular behavior that varies with brain region. In prefrontal cortex it prolongs the serotonin (5-HT) induced increase in GABAergic synaptic activity (Tan et al., 2004). While in CA1 pyramidal neurons it modulates potassium currents (A and delayed rectifier types; Kratzer et al., 2013). It may also dampen excitability by inhibiting NMDA-induced currents in cultured hippocampal neurons (Sheng et al., 2008). In the amygdala CRF activates a Kv3 type potassium channels that contribute to action potential repolarization therefore slowing firing frequency (Fu and Neugebauer, 2008). Thus the effects of CRF seem to be highly diverse. This varied functionality may arise from heterogeneity of G proteins that couple to CRF receptors. For example, in above mentioned studies these actions were mediated by either protein kinase A (PKA; Fu and Neugebauer, 2008) or protein kinase C (PKC; Tan et al., 2004) signaling pathways.
A similar situation is apparent for the neurotransmitter 5-HT which arise from cells located in the Raphae nucleus (Sheldon and Aghajanian, 1990). Its activity throughout the brain is highly diverse. For example, Aghajanian and co-workers found that 5-HT is excitatory on interneurons and inhibitory on pyramidal cells of PC (Sheldon and Aghajanian, 1990, 1991; Marek and Aghajanian, 1996). However, unlike CRF, this diversity in activity of 5-HT seems to be due to the wide range of 5-HT receptor subtypes that are heterogeneously expressed throughout the brain.
The heterogeneous functions of both these neurotransmitters are thought to be important in mediating stressor or anxiety responses (Eison, 1990; Hayley et al., 2005). For example, it has been well-documented that DOI, a 5-HT2A/CR agonist produces very strong anxiety responses, and increases in extrahypothalamic CRF has been associated with anxiety disorders (Anisman et al., 2008; Magalhaes et al., 2010). CRFR1 activation can enhance 5-HT2A/CR mediated signaling via a mechanism involving the recruitment of endosomal 5-HT2A/CR to the plasma membrane (Magalhaes et al., 2010). Furthermore the same study showed that prior dosing of mice with a CRFR1 agonist enhanced DOI-induced anxiety. However, an understanding of how these neurotransmitters affect the neural circuit behavior is incomplete and more research is needed to understand the impact of their activation on local circuit behavior.
One region of the brain that highly expresses both CRFR1 and 5-HT2Rs is the PC (Pazos et al., 1985; Van et al., 2000). The PC is of interest in epilepsy because its circuitry easily supports seizures (Ekstrand et al., 2001). We have recently shown that PC excitability is under the control of feed forward disinhibitory circuit that potentiates excitatory input from the lateral olfactory tract (LOT; Birjandian et al., 2013). How CRF and/or 5-HT may modulate this circuit is not known. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to understand how CRFR1 activation and their possible interaction of 5-HT2A/CR modulate the activity of the PC.
Materials and Methods
All the procedures performed for this project were in accordance with the guidelines of Canadian council of animal care and approved by The University of Western Ontario council on animal care.
Slice Preparation and Dye Loading
Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats weighing 150–180 g were used in all experiments. They were housed individually with free access to food and water under a continuous 12 h light/dark cycle. Animals were anesthetized with ketamine–medetomidine hydrochloride combination and then perfused through heart with an ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) in which sodium ions was replaced by choline ions. The composition of this ACSF used is choline chloride, 110 mM; KCl, 2.5 mM; NaH2PO4, 1.2 mM; NaHCO3, 25 mM; CaCl2, 0.5 mM; MgCl2, 7 mM; sodium pyruvate, 2.4 mM; ascorbate, 1.3 mM; dextrose, 20 mM (McIntyre et al., 2002). The brains were perfused to flush any blood out of the vessels to prevent the iron in the blood from oxidizing and causing damage to neuronal cells. The brain was rapidly removed following perfusion and the region containing anterior PC was carefully cut into a block to facilitate slicing by a Vibratome (slices were 400 μM thick). The slices were incubated at room 37°C for 30 min and subsequently moved to room temperature (22°C) bath for 45 min. The perfusion, slicing and incubation procedures were carried out in choline-ACSF with continuous supply of carbogen (95% O2 and 5% CO2 mixture). These slices were used for both voltage sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) and patch clamp recording. For the VSDI slices were incubated in the voltage sensitive dye Di-4-ANEPPS (D-199, Invitrogen Molecular Probes, Inc., Eugene, OR, USA) for 35 min. The stock solution of the dye was dissolved in ethanol (22 mg/ml). On the day of experiment the dye incubation was prepared by mixing 60 μl of dye stock with 500 μl of fetal bovine serum (FBS), 500 μl of ACSF and 310 μl of 10% cremophore-EL solution. The concentration of dye in the final solution was 0.1 mg/ml. After incubation slices were washed for 8–10 min with ACSF and transferred to recording chamber. The temperature of the bath was maintained at 32°C during recordings and continuously supplied with carbogen bubbled ACSF having a composition of NaCl, 110 mM; KCl, 2.5 mM; NaH2PO4, 1.2 mM; NaHCO3, 25 mM; CaCl2, 2.0 mM; MgCl2, 2.0 mM; dextrose, 20 mM. The pH and osmolarity of the solutions were adjusted to 7.3–7.4 and 297–305 mOsm respectively.
A platinum/iridium electrode (Microprobes, Inc., Gaithersburg, MD, USA) with a tip diameter of 200–300 μM was used to stimulate LOT of PC. The stimulation of each slice was in the range of 160–200 μA, each square pulse was 2.0 ms in length. The electrode was connected to a stimulator (S88X dual output square pulse stimulator, Grass Technologies, Astro-Med, Inc., Brossard, QC, Canada), which controlled the pulse frequency and train duration (Birjandian et al., 2013).
Voltage Sensitive Dye Imaging
Each optical recording was about 10 s in length and consisted of two phases. The first contained recording of background activity for 2 s followed by the application of stimulus for 1 s with a train of frequencies ranging from 5 to 80 Hz. The acquisition rate was set at 5 ms/frame. The camera saturation was set around 50% for each recording. Optical signals were recorded by a CMOS camera (Micam Ultima Brain Vision, Inc., Tokyo, Japan) mounted on top of an upright microscope (Fixed stage upright microscope, BX51WI, Olympus). The light from a 100 W halogen lamp source (HLX 64625, Microlites Scientific, Corp.) passed through an excitation filter (λ = 530 ± 10 nm). A long pass emission filter (λ > 590 nm) collected the fluorescent signals. A long working distance objective was used in the experiments (XLFluor 4X N.A.0.28, Olympus). The movies were analyzed using Brain Vision Analyzer (Tokyo, Japan) software. A detailed explanation of the technique is described elsewhere (Birjandian et al., 2013).
Patch Clamp Electrophysiology
Patch clamp recordings were done as previously described in McIntyre et al. (2002) and Gavrilovici et al. (2012). Patch electrodes were pulled from borosilicate glass capillaries and filled with K+-gluconate solution having a composition (in mM) of: potassium gluconate 147, KCl 1, CaCl2 2, HEPES 10, EGTA 10, Glucose 10, MgATP 2, GTP 0.3 (300 mOsm, pH 7.3–7.4). Whole-cell patch clamp recordings were made with an Axon instruments Patch 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA, USA) from neurons in Layers II and III of anterior PC. Series resistance compensation was performed in all recordings. The initial access resistance was <20 MΩ and compensated by 50–70%. Bridge balance was set to auto to correct the voltage drop across the membrane. All experiments were performed at 32°C. Stimulation of the LOT for these recordings was done in an identical manner as that used for VSDI. Excitability of the cells was assayed by current-clamp protocols (Axon instruments; Clampex 10.3; 500 ms pulses in 50 pA increments). For recordings done in Layer III we have previously identified by the cluster analysis five differing functional phenotypes (Gavrilovici et al., 2012). These clusters were named according to the terminology described by Kroner et al. (2007) and Ascoli et al. (2008). To decide whether interneuron significantly slowed action potential generation (adapted) an interspike interval ratio (IIR) was calculated. The IIR is the ratio between the last spike interval at the end of the 500 ms current pulse divided by the first spike interval. An IIR < 1.25 were termed as non-adapting cells while neurons with IIR > 1.25 were classified as adapting cells. As well cells were classified based on their firing frequency (FF) at 2X current threshold (I2t). Cells that had an average FF > 50 Hz were classified as high frequency (HF) while those FF < 50 Hz were termed as low frequency (LF). Cells with a FF of >100 Hz were classified as very high frequency (vHF).
Tat Peptide Co-immunoprecipitation
Human embryonic kidney (HEK293) cells were maintained in Eagle’s minimal essential medium with 10% FBS. HEK293 cells were seeded onto 10 cm dishes 24 h before transfection to attain 70–80% confluency. Transient transfection was performed using a modified calcium phosphate protocol described by Ferguson and Caron (2004). Cells were transfected with 1 μg of YFP-SAP97 and 1 μg of either FLAG-5-HT2AR or HA-CRFR1. These constructs have been described previously (Dunn et al., 2013, 2014). 18 h post-transfection, cells were washed with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and re-suspended with trypsin containing 0.25% EDTA. 24 h after the removal of transfection reagents, cells were treated with Tat-Scrambled or Tat-CRFR1-CT for 2 h at the described concentrations (Tao et al., 2008). Cells were subsequently lysed in lysis buffer (50 mM Tris, pH 8.0, 150 mM NaCl, and 1% Triton X-100) containing protease inhibitors (1 mM AEBSF, 10 μg/ml leupeptin, and 5 μg/ml aprotinin) for 20 min on a rocking platform at 4°C. Samples were collected into 1.5-ml Eppendorf tubes and centrifuged at 15,000 × g for 15 min at 4°C to pellet insoluble material. A Bronsted–Lowry protein assay was performed, and 400 μg of protein from each condition was incubated for 1–2 h at 4°C with either FLAG-immunoprecipitation beads from Sigma-Aldrich or protein G-sepharose and mouse anti-HA antibody (1:50). After incubation, beads were washed three times with cold lysis buffer and incubated overnight at room temperature in 3x SDS Loading Buffer containing 2-mercaptoethanol. Samples were separated by SDS-PAGE, transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane, and immunoblotted to identify co-immunoprecipitated YFP-SAP97 (rabbit anti-GFP, 1:1000) with either FLAG-5-HT2AR or HA-CRFR1 in the presence or absence of Tat-CRFR1-CT.
Reagents
2,5-Dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine, H-89, Forskolin and BIS were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich Co., St. Louis, MO, USA. CRF, antisauvagine-30 and antalarmin were obtained from Tocris Biosciences, Ellisville, MO, USA. Tat-CRFR1-CT peptide was obtained from CanPeptide, Inc., Pointe-claire, QC, Canada. The Tat-CRFR1-CT sequence used was YGRKKRRQRR-PTRVSFHSIKQSTAV. This peptide has been shown previously to prevent the functional synergism between CRFR1 and 5-HT2Rs (Magalhaes et al., 2010). Di-4-ANEPPS stock solution was prepared in alcohol and cremophore-EL solution which can be stored at 4°C for 2 months. FBS and ACSF were added on the day of experiment. CRF stock was dissolved in HBSS and milli-Q water mixture. DOI was prepared in alcohol and Milli-Q water. Forskolin, H-89, BIS, antalarmin, antisauvagine-30, and PMA stock solutions were prepared in dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO). All the stock solutions were made 1000 times more concentrated than working concentrations.
Statistical Analysis
Comparison of all measured values was done using two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or repeated measures ANOVA as appropriate. Post hoc comparisons were carried out using a Fisher Exact Test. A Mann–Whitney test (non-parametric) was used to evaluate the action potential number data taken form pyramidal cell recordings. The significance for all tests was set at p< 0.05. All statistical evaluations were done using Statview Software.
Results
We have recently demonstrated that the activation of the PC through the stimulation of the LOT induces a novel feed forward disinhibitory loop that potentiates activation of the Layer II pyramidal cell layer (see Birjandian et al., 2013) for a complete description). Responses were followed by VSDI of the PC brain slices (sampling rate is 200 frames/second) that include all three layers along with the underlying dorsal endopiriform nucleus (DEn). When the (LOT) was stimulated with a bipolar electrode with frequencies ranging from 20 to 80 Hz for 1 s Layer II was first activated. This initial response was followed about 40–50 ms later by the activation of the dorsal endopiriform nucleus (DEn) and then (after about 200 ms) the deactivation of Layer III. An example of a response is shown in Figure 1. The first frame, before the stimulation, shows of the PC Layers I–III and endopiriform nucleus (DEn). Each subsequent frame in the picture has a time difference of 2 s from the preceding frame. The excitatory responses originating from Layer II and DEn are shown in Figures 1B,D. The inhibitory response originating from Layer III is shown in Figure 1C. The change in fluorescence in these recordings is color coded by the software so that excitation is shown by green, yellow, and red while decreased activity is shown by blue and magenta colors. Our interpretation of this sequence of events is that the initial activation of the Layer II activates inhibitory neurons in the DEn that then in turn inhibit Layer III neurons. This disinhibition of Layer III potentiates the activation of Layer II neurons (again see Birjandian et al., 2013) for complete characterization of this response). In Figures 1B–D we show the relationship between frequency of stimulation and change in the magnitude of the response in Layers II, III, and DEn respectively showing that the (de)activation occurs over the gamma frequency band of stimulation trains.
CRF Reduces the Activity of the PC
We wanted to investigate how CRF might modulate this response. This was done by comparing control input/output relationship (as shown in Figures 1B–D) to those obtained after ACSF containing 100 nM CRF was perfused over the slice. In Figure 2A, the left panel shows the control response (2 s after the stimulation) to an 80 Hz train. The middle panel shows the equivalent response after 15–20 min of 100 nM CRF application and the right most panel shows the response after CRF washout. CRF reduced the activity of pyramidal cell layer as evidenced by decreased signal (less red) in Layer II [F(1,9) = 6.58, p < 0.001, n = 9]. Similarly, the activity of neurons in DEn was also reduced which can be seen by decreased green/yellow/red scaling of the fluorescence signal. The decreased blue/violet color in Layer III represents decreased disinhibitory drive arising from the DEn neurons. In Figures 2B–D, we show the effect of CRF over the entire input/output relationships in the three areas tested. CRF reduced the activity of Layer II pyramidal cells and the cells in DEn as the curves shifted downward (Figures 2B,D). Application of CRF also reduced the inhibition of Layer III, thus shifting the curve upward. This CRF effect was reversible within 10 min (Figure 2C). We also found that the effects of CRF were most robust within γ-frequency range (40–80 Hz). There was no significant difference in fluorescence signals within θ to high β frequency range (5–20 Hz).
CRFR1 Mediate the Actions of CRF in PC
Corticotropin-releasing factor exerts its actions through two major kinds of receptor subtypes named CRFR1 and CRFR2 (Zorrilla et al., 2014). These two receptors are widely distributed in the rat central nervous system (De Souza et al., 1985; Primus et al., 1997; Van et al., 2000). Since these receptors bind to different G proteins, activate various signaling cascades and produce different effects it is essential to understand which CRFR subtype mediate the actions of CRF in this context. We employed antalarmin, a CRFR1 antagonist to see if it blocks the actions of CRF. We found that antalarmin at a concentration of 10 μM abolished the activity of CRF [100 nM; F(1,6) = 6.542, p < 0.001, n = 10]. In Figure 3A we show that there is no difference in control (left panel) and CRF and antalarmin treated slice (right panel). In Figure 3B, we show the dose/inhibition curve of antalarmin in the presence of 100 nM CRF. We found that the IC50 of antalarmin was 100 nM. We also tested antisuvagine-30, a CRFR2 antagonist to determine whether CRFR2 has any role in the actions of CRF. Antisauvagine-30 was not able to block the actions of CRF (data not shown). These data indicate that CRF reduces the excitability of the PC through the activation of CRFR1.
Induction of DOI Activity is Dependent on Previous CRF Activation of CRFR1
It has been previously reported that CRFR1 potentiates 5-HT2R mediated signaling in vitro (Magalhaes et al., 2010). Therefore, we wished to see if these receptors could alter PC activation and perhaps act in similar synergistic manner. We used the relatively selective 5-HT2A/C receptor agonist DOI. Surprisingly, DOI had no effect on the PC circuit activation (n = 5). Figure 4A shows images of the signal distribution (1 s after the end of 1 s 60 Hz stimulus) before and after the application of DOI. The quantification of these results shows that no frequency of stimulation altered circuit activity in all three layers of PC (Figures 4B–D). To test if prior CRFR1 activation could induce a DOI response we first applied CRF and then DOI. Under these conditions DOI produced similar responses to those produced by CRF. In Figure 5 we show images of responses taken before and after the application of these agonists. In Figure 5A the control response is in the left panel while in the middle panel we show the activity of CRF and in the right panel we show the effect of DOI. It is evident that DOI further reduced the effects of the activation of this circuit [F(1,9) = 9.82, p < 0.01, n = 9]. In Figure 5B we show the quantitation of the effects of CRF and DOI after CRF in all three layer of PC. These data show that 5-HT2A/CR activity is dependent on the prior activation of CRFR1. Unlike CRF, the effects of DOI were not reversible within 30 min of “washouts” that were attempted in these experiments.
The Activity of DOI is Dependent on CRF and 5-HT2 Receptor Complex Formation Mediated by a PDZ Domain-Containing Protein
It has been previously reported that the activation and subsequent desensitization of CRFR1 leads to a complex formation by CRFR1 with 5-HT2R on endosomes involved in trafficking to the cell membrane (Magalhaes et al., 2010). Therefore we wanted to determine if the DOI effect after CRFR1 stimulation may occur through the crosstalk of CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CR, which has previously been demonstrated to involve PDZ interactions (Magalhaes et al., 2010). To do this, we utilized a Tat-tagged peptide corresponding to the final 15 amino acids of the CRFR1 carboxyl terminus (Tat-CRFR1-CT) which includes a class I PDZ-binding motif similar to that found in 5-HT2A/CR. This peptide was previously demonstrated to abolish the functional crosstalk between CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CR (Magalhaes et al., 2010). Therefore, we investigated whether this peptide could sequester PDZ domain-containing proteins capable of interacting with class I PDZ-binding motifs, thereby preventing their interaction with CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CRs. Figure 6 shows that 30 μM of Tat-CRFR1-CT was sufficient to block the interaction of HA-CRFR1 with PDZ domain-containing protein YFP-SAP97 in HEK293 cells. Additionally, the interaction of FLAG-5-HT2AR with PDZ domain-containing proteins was inhibited at 30 μM of Tat-CRFR1-CT, as evidenced by a lack of interaction between FLAG-5-HT2AR and YFP-SAP97 (Figures 6A,B). Therefore, Tat-CRFR1-CT is capable of blocking the interaction of both CRFR1 and 5-HT2ARs with PDZ domain-containing proteins, thereby providing a potential mechanism for the inhibition of receptor crosstalk. Next we determined if the same peptide could prevent the activity of DOI after CRF in brain slices. As a control we also used a scrambled Tat-tagged sequence. The Tat-CRFR1-CT peptide was incubated with the slices for 40 min before the application of CRF and DOI following CRF. CRF produced effects comparable to the effects that it produced in the absence of Tat-CRFR1-CT while the DOI activity after CRF receptor activation was abolished (Figure 7). The scrambled peptide was without effect and DOI was able to further potentiate the activity of CRF. These observations are consistent with interpretation that in order for DOI to have an effect CRFR1 must interact with 5-HT2Rs through a complex involving PDZ domain-containing proteins.
The Effects of CRF are Mediated through a Protein Kinase C Dependent Signaling Cascade
We found that CRFR1 mediates the actions of CRF as antalarmin blocked the actions of CRF. Next we determined which signaling cascade is activated by CRFR1 stimulation. Although the CRFR1 is usually classified as GPCR that is coupled to Gαs (activation of adenylcyclase), there are several reports that CRFR1 signaling can activate Gαq/11 cascades as well (Elliott-Hunt et al., 2002; Blank et al., 2003; Wietfeld et al., 2004). To see if a Gαs mediated pathway may be activated by CRFR1 simulation we first used the adenylylcyclase activator forskolin (20 μM, for 15–20 min) to see if it would mimic the effects of CRF in all layers of PC. It did not produce effects like CRF (not shown). However, the subsequent application of CRF and/or DOI also had no effect and so activation of adenylcyclase occludes the effects of CRF/DOI (Figure 8A). Next we used the PKA antagonist H-89 (10 μM) to see if it could affect the CRF/DOI responses. It had no activity on either the CRF or the DOI response (Figure 8B). The responses of CRF and DOI after CRF were still significant in the presence of H-89 [F(1,5) = 6.34, p < 0.05, n = 6]. Thus antagonism of PKA activity does not affect the CRF/DOI responses. Next we applied bisindolylmaleimide-I (BIS; 100 nM), a PKC antagonist to the slices before the application of CRF and DOI following CRF. We found that BIS blocked the effects of CRF and DOI following CRF (Figure 9A; n = 6). In the next step we applied PMA alone, an activator of PKC to the see if it could mimic the effects of CRF. We observed that PMA (100 nM) produced effects like CRF. However, DOI applied after PMA failed to produce a further change in the PC activity (Figure 9B; n = 6).
Single Cell Activity Correlates to VSDI
In order to understand how CRF/DOI may affect the activity of individual cells we conducted whole-cell patch clamp recordings in the Layers II and III of PC. To examine the responses in the Layer II we stimulated LOT using 80 Hz trains which were identical to stimuli used for VSD imaging. The response to this train was reduced by CRF (Figure 10A) even though each stimulus generated an action potential during the train (not shown). Thus CRF does not seem to have any effect on the efficacy of the stimulus only the response to them. The addition of DOI after CRF further reduced the efficacy of these stimuli. In Figure 10B we show a raster plot of 18 recordings done from pyramidal cells showing that both CRF and DOI reduced the activity induced by LOT stimulation (n = 10). Like that observed in the VSDI recordings the effects of CRF were reversible after about 15–20 min of washing (Figure 10C), but we saw no reversibility within the same time frame for DOI after CRF applications. Finally, we also found that CRF and DOI reduced the excitability of pyramidal cells as equivalent current pulses were unable to generate same activity during a 500 ms pulse of current (Figure 10D). In control recordings at two times the threshold current required to induce an action potential (I2t) the average interspike frequency of the train was 44.8 ± 1.1 Hz while in CRF and DOI this was 39.0 ± 2.0 Hz (p < 0.01) and 42.9 ± 1.3 Hz respectively (p > 0.05 compared to control). Thus the average frequency of trains generated was not largely changed. However, the number of action potentials generated by this current injection was greatly reduced (Control 11 ± 1, CRF 6 ± 1, and DOI 2 ± 1 p’s < 0.001 for all comparisons). Thus the pyramidal cells generated action potentials at similar or slightly reduced frequencies but the cells accommodated to the stimulus generating fewer action potentials.
Corticotropin-releasing factor had variable and highly complex effects on the excitability of cells located in Layer III which are almost exclusively GABAergic interneurons. In contrast to pyramidal cells DOI had no effect on any recordings done in this cell layer. We have previously shown that interneurons in Layer III fire with five well-defined and differing spike patterns (Gavrilovici et al., 2012). The naming of these firing patterns were adopted from the nomenclature defined in Ascoli et al. (2008). Here we found that CRF altered the spiking patterns of some, but not all these subtypes. There were two types of outcomes on these interneurons. One response converted low frequency firing patterns to high frequency ones. While the second type of response showed a conversion from a high frequency spiking patterns to low. In some recordings there was no apparent effect of CRF. In Figure 11A we show an example of the conversion of an ALF interneuron (<50 Hz average firing frequency at two times threshold (I2t); also see methods for description of classifications) to an AHF phenotype (>50 Hz at I2t). All interneurons having this initial phenotype responded in this manner (Control: 44.0 ± 0.1; CRF: 62.3 ± 0.6 Hz, p < 0.001, n = 14). The graph in Figure 11B shows the averaged interspike frequency versus spike interval relationship for all 14 recordings. One line shows the relationship at the threshold (It) where a train of action potentials is elicited, while the other shows the relation at I2t. It is evident that CRF produced a dramatic change in the input/output responses of these cells. Similarly those cells that had the weakly ALF phenotype (wALF) all converted to an AHF pattern (Figure 11C; Control: 29.1 0 ± 0.6; CRF: 69.1 ± 0.8 Hz, p < 0.001, n = 6). The average change in the input/output for this population is shown in Figure 11D. The second type of responses were more variable. Interneurons that initially fired with an AHF phenotype sometimes converted to ALF pattern (Figures 12A,B; Control: 62.9 ± 0.9; CRF: 43.9 ± 1.0 Hz, p < 0.001, n = 6) but another cohort did not change (n = 7; not shown). NAvHF interneurons (>100 Hz) similarly had variable responses; some did not change (n = 4) while another group converted to the weakly adapting low frequency interneuron phenotype (wALF; Figures 12C,D, Control: 101.3 ± 1.0; CRF: 28.1 ± 0.9 Hz, p < 0.001, n = 5). The fifth pattern that we identified (Gavrilovici et al., 2012), strongly ALF, which occurred in less than 5% of the 205 recordings, was not seen in 38 recordings we did here and so we are unsure as to the effects of CRF on these cells.
Discussion
Disturbances in homeostasis due to stressors are thought to mitigate the onset of depression or anxiety (de Kloet et al., 2005). The physiological mechanisms following stressful stimuli involve various brain regions, such as amygdala, hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. These areas interact and integrate their responses by releasing neuromodulators that regulate stressor reactions (Mora et al., 2012). The importance of these brain regions during stress and anxiety has been extensively studied (McEwen, 2007). The PC, a part of the limbic region that is interconnected with amygdale, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex (Loscher and Ebert, 1996), has not been extensively studied to determine how it may respond to stressors. Odor perception can induce stressor responses which are highly context dependent (Kadohisa, 2013; Krusemark et al., 2013) and therefore it is reasonable to hypothesize the PC could be modulated by neurochemical processes depending on the nature of the stressor. In this context, we have shown here that CRF and 5-HT receptors, which are implicated in stress, anxiety and depression in many regions (Aghajanian and Davis, 1975; Kahn et al., 1988; Eison, 1990; Davis, 1992; Arborelius et al., 1999; Naughton et al., 2000; Muller et al., 2003; Merali et al., 2004; Risbrough and Stein, 2006) have profound effects in the PC. What is surprising is that although CRF and 5-HT2R activation is known to be anxiogenic, they dampened the overall activity of the PC. Thus deactivation of PC would seem to be the usual response to these ligands in this particular brain region. As the primary function of the PC is olfaction this may in fact be a reflexive response to dampen output to the amygdala from the PC which is presumably already being, through prefrontal or other pathways, activated by the perceived threat. Our data also indicate that serotonergic neurotransmission on 5-HT2R receptors, in the stimulation paradigm used here, when CRF is released (presumably from the central amygdala or local CRF containing neurons). Thus, it would seem that amygdala activation is required for this CRF/DOI interaction to occur.
The effects of DOI were dependent on prior CRFR1 activation requiring the formation – CRFR1 – 5-HT2R complex. However, the interaction of CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CRs appears to only occur on pyramidal cells as the interneurons of Layer III were unaffected by DOI application. As pyramidal cells express both 5-HT2A/CRs we are unsure what subtype(s) are being trafficked. Although the application of PMA mimicked CRF, activation of PKC did not induce DOI responses (like CRF). This outcome fits with the interpretation that the DOI activity is dependent on CRFR1 activation and presumably CRFR1 endocytosis. So although PKC activation dampened excitability, DOI could not add to this effect as CRFR1 activation had been bypassed.
Corticotropin-releasing factor activity seems to vary with brain region and cell type. Another study using VSDI demonstrated that CRF enhanced hippocampal excitation (von et al., 2011) and several patch clamp studies reported that CRF was able to facilitate action potential firing in hippocampus (Aldenhoff et al., 1983; Hollrigel et al., 1998; Blank et al., 2003). CRF has also been shown to depress excitatory neurotransmission in the hippocampal formation (Sheng et al., 2008), but we found no evidence of this here as LOT stimulation was unaffected by CRF. It has also been shown that in the prefrontal cortex CRF decreased the excitability of pyramidal cells by enhancing GABA release (Tan et al., 2004). Although we did not measure GABA release here, this outcome seems unlikely. The NAvHF interneurons whose excitability was reduced densely innervates Layer II pyramidal cells and therefore a reduction GABA release would be predicted. The reason for these highly variable outcomes throughout the brain is unclear. However, these outcomes cannot be explained by CRF interacting with differing CRFRs. It is more likely that divergent actions of CRF are, at least, in part due to divergent signaling pathways as other studies have shown that CRFR1 can activate either Gαs or Gαq/11 signaling. Elliot-Hunt and co-workers reported that CRF activated PKA and MAPK signaling pathways in hippocampus (Elliott-Hunt et al., 2002). In another study, Sheng et al. (2008) observed, in cultured hippocampal neurons, inhibition of PKC abolished CRF regulation of NMDA currents. (Tan et al., 2004) showed that forskolin did not mimic the actions of CRF but PKC activation did; observations that are identical to ours. Thus it seems that CRF signally is promiscuous varying from cell to cell and region to region. Interestingly we found that activation of adenylcyclase by forskolin- occluded the effects of CRF. The mechanism by which this occurs is unclear but may involve heterologous desensitization of the CRFR1 itself as a consequence of forskolin mediated activation of PKA. As we have found no evidence CRF signals through differing cascades here (although it cannot be ruled out), it seems more likely that the CRFR1 activation is linked to divergent effector cascades that alter neuron function.
The most profound effect of the CRFR1/5-HT2A/CR interaction is the dampening of the Layer II excitability. These likely accounts for the reduced activation the circuit with the highly variable effects of CRF on interneurons in Layer III modulating the overall response. Although, it would also seem reasonable to conclude that the reduced excitability of the majority of interneurons would also enhance the dampening of circuit activation. Nevertheless, the fact that the activity of CRF in Layer III varied changing from one functionally distinct interneuron subtype to another argues for more subtle modulatory roles for CRF within this circuit. Given that an important function of interneurons is to control network synchrony this suggests that CRF in Layer III modulates the firing patterns of the PC and may also be important for governing the impact of circuit activation. This variability in CRF effects has been observed before; when administered intracerebroventricularly it produced both inhibitory and excitatory effects on dorsal raphae nucleus neuron firing rate. Both effects were mediated through CRFR1 as antalarmin attenuated CRF’s activity (Kirby et al., 2000). So in sum our data suggest that the most salient outcome of CRF/DOI activity is to reduce the Layer II activity which then attenuates the disinhibition.
5-HTR activation also has been shown elsewhere to produce heterogeneous effects in many brain regions. Here, we show DOI reduced PC pyramidal cell activity but had no effect on the interneurons in Layer III. By contrast activation of 5-HT1CR (subsequently renamed 5-HT2CR) receptors in PC produced increased pyramidal cell activity (Sheldon and Aghajanian, 1990). In same study activation of 5-HT2Rs in PC increased the occurrence of spontaneous inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSP’s) on pyramidal cells. This was shown to be due to increased firing of interneurons located in the border between Layers II and III. Initially which subtype of 5-HT2R was activated was not known as the 5-HT was used as the agonist and a non-selective 5-HT2R antagonist was used to block this effect. Later it was shown that this effect was mediated by the 5-HT2AR and not 5-HT2CR (Marek and Aghajanian, 1994). We found that activation of the 5-HT2A/CRs on pyramidal cells decreased excitability although this required previous CRFR1 activation. This argues that the effects are mediated by 5-HT2A/CRs, consistent with the findings of Magalhaes et al. (2010). We also found that DOI had no effect on interneuron spiking patterns. These results are surprising as 5-HT2A/CRs are expressed in this layer and one might expect activation to alter the activity of cells in this layer. Therefore it would seem that the increased interneuron activity observed by Aghajanian and coworkers is not due to changes in their intrinsic excitability and likely represents increased excitatory drive onto these interneurons or alteration in the neurotransmitter release mechanisms.
In summary, this study shows how neuronal activity through PC layers is altered by the activation of CRFR1 and 5-HT2A/CRs. To emphasize it is surprising that two neurotransmitters that heighten anxiety would reduce excitability in a circuit which has strong connections to hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex. This may represent a behavioral feedback mechanism to blunt odor induced stressor responses. What is also striking is the highly complex and varied nature of the CRFR1 effects on the differing cell populations. The use of the selective Tat-CRFR1-CT peptide indicates these effects of DOI are mediated through an interaction between CRFR1/5-HT2A/CRs with one or multiple PDZ domain-containing proteins (like SAP97), but it doesnot rule out other similar interactions may occur with other receptor subtypes. Finally, it seems CRFR1 may signal through differing G protein signaling cascades and so it may be possible under differing conditions, epilepsy, chronic stress or perhaps head injury, that these pathways may be plastic and modulation of this network and others may be dynamic.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research operating grant and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council discovery grant to MP. CN is supported by an Ontario Graduate Student Scholarship (OGS). SF is a Career Investigator of the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Ontario and holds a Tier I Canada Research Chair in Molecular Neurobiology. HD holds a Jonathan and Joshua Memorial Scholarship in Mental Health Research. We would like to thank Dr. Vijairaj Raj for his support in plotting raster plots in MATLAB.
Abbreviations
- AHF
adapting high frequency
- ALF
adapting low frequency
- CRF
corticotrophin-releasing factor
- CRFR1
corticotrophin-releasing factor receptor 1
- DOI
dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine
- 5-HT2A/CR
5-hydroxytrytamine 2A/C receptor
- NavHF
non-adapting very high frequency
- PC
piriform cortex.
References
- Aghajanian G. K., Davis M. (1975). A method of direct chemical brain stimulation in behavioral studies using microiontophoresis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 3 127–131. 10.1016/0091-3057(75)90091-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aldenhoff J. B., Gruol D. L., Rivier J., Vale W., Siggins G. R. (1983). Corticotropin releasing factor decreases postburst hyperpolarizations and excites hippocampal neurons. Science 221 875–877. 10.1126/science.6603658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anisman H., Merali Z., Hayley S. (2008). Neurotransmitter, peptide and cytokine processes in relation to depressive disorder: comorbidity between depression and neurodegenerative disorders. Prog. Neurobiol. 85 1–74. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anisman H., Prakash P., Merali Z., Poulter M. O. (2007). Corticotropin releasing hormone receptor alterations elicited by acute and chronic unpredictable stressor challenges in stressor-susceptible and resilient strains of mice. Behav. Brain Res. 181 180–190. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arborelius L., Owens M. J., Plotsky P. M., Nemeroff C. B. (1999). The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in depression and anxiety disorders. J. Endocrinol. 160 1–12. 10.1677/joe.0.1600001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ascoli G. A., Alonso-Nanclares L., Anderson S. A., Barrionuevo G., Benavides-Piccione R., Burkhalter A., et al. (2008). Petilla terminology: nomenclature of features of GABAergic interneurons of the cerebral cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 557–568. 10.1038/nrn2402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birjandian Z., Narla C., Poulter M. O. (2013). Gain control of gamma frequency activation by a novel feed forward disinhibitory loop: implications for normal and epileptic neural activity. Front. Neural Circuits 7:183 10.3389/fncir.2013.00183 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blank T., Nijholt I., Grammatopoulos D. K., Randeva H. S., Hillhouse E. W., Spiess J. (2003). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors couple to multiple G-proteins to activate diverse intracellular signaling pathways in mouse hippocampus: role in neuronal excitability and associative learning. J. Neurosci. 23 700–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15 353–375. 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Kloet E. R., Joels M., Holsboer F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6 463–475. 10.1038/nrn1683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Souza E. B., Insel T. R., Perrin M. H., Rivier J., Vale W. W., kuhar M. J. (1985). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors are widely distributed within the rat central nervous system: an autoradiographic study. J. Neurosci. 5 3189–3203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn H. A., Walther C., Godin C. M., Hall R. A., Ferguson S. S. (2013). Role of SAP97 protein in the regulation of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 endocytosis and extracellular signal-regulated kinase 1/2 signaling. J. Biol. Chem. 288 15023–15034. 10.1074/jbc.M113.473660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn H. A., Walther C., Yuan G. Y., Caetano F. A., Godin C. M., Ferguson S. S. (2014). Role of SAP97 in the regulation of 5-HT2AR endocytosis and signaling. Mol. Pharmacol. 86 275–283. 10.1124/mol.114.093476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eison M. S. (1990). Serotonin: a common neurobiologic substrate in anxiety and depression. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 10 26S–30S. 10.1097/00004714-199006001-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekstrand J. J., Domroese M. E., Johnson D. M., Feig S. L., Knodel S. M., Behan M., et al. (2001). A new subdivision of anterior piriform cortex and associated deep nucleus with novel features of interest for olfaction and epilepsy. J. Comp. Neurol. 434 289–307. 10.1002/cne.1178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott-Hunt C. R., Kazlauskaite J., Wilde G. J., Grammatopoulos D. K., Hillhouse E. W. (2002). Potential signalling pathways underlying corticotrophin-releasing hormone-mediated neuroprotection from excitotoxicity in rat hippocampus. J. Neurochem. 80 416–425. 10.1046/j.0022-3042.2001.00712.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson S. S., Caron M. G. (2004). Green fluorescent protein-tagged beta-arrestin translocation as a measure of G protein-coupled receptor activation. Methods Mol. Biol. 237 121–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu Y., Neugebauer V. (2008). Differential mechanisms of CRF1 and CRF2 receptor functions in the amygdala in pain-related synaptic facilitation and behavior. J. Neurosci. 28 3861–3876. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0227-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gavrilovici C., Pollock E., Everest M., Poulter M. O. (2012). The loss of interneuron functional diversity in the piriform cortex after induction of experimental epilepsy. Neurobiol. Dis. 48 317–328. 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayley S., Poulter M. O., Merali Z., Anisman H. (2005). The pathogenesis of clinical depression: stressor- and cytokine-induced alterations of neuroplasticity. Neuroscience 135 659–678. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.03.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollrigel G. S., Chen K., Baram T. Z., Soltesz I. (1998). The pro-convulsant actions of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the hippocampus of infant rats. Neuroscience 84 71–79. 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00499-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadohisa M. (2013). Effects of odor on emotion, with implications. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:66 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn R. S., van Praag H. M., Wetzler S., Asnis G. M., Barr G. (1988). Serotonin and anxiety revisited. Biol. Psychiatry 23 189–208. 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90091-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby L. G., Rice K. C., Valentino R. J. (2000). Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on neuronal activity in the serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuropsychopharmacology 22 148–162. 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00093-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kratzer S., Mattusch C., Metzger M. W., Dedic N., Noll-Hussong M., Kafitz K. W., et al. (2013). Activation of CRH receptor type 1 expressed on glutamatergic neurons increases excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons by the modulation of voltage-gated ion channels. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 7:91 10.3389/fncel.2013.00091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroner S., Krimer L. S., Lewis D. A., Barrionuevo G. (2007). Dopamine increases inhibition in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex through cell type-specific modulation of interneurons. Cereb. Cortex 17 1020–1032. 10.1093/cercor/bhl012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krusemark E. A., Novak L. R., Gitelman D. R., Li W. (2013). When the sense of smell meets emotion: anxiety-state-dependent olfactory processing and neural circuitry adaptation. J. Neurosci. 33 15324–15332. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loscher W., Ebert U. (1996). The role of the piriform cortex in kindling. Prog. Neurobiol. 50 427–481. 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00036-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magalhaes A. C., Holmes K. D., Dale L. B., Comps-Agrar L., Lee D., Yadav P. N., et al. (2010). CRF receptor 1 regulates anxiety behavior via sensitization of 5-HT2 receptor signaling. Nat. Neurosci. 13 622–629. 10.1038/nn.2529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek G. J., Aghajanian G. K. (1994). Excitation of interneurons in piriform cortex by 5-hydroxytryptamine: blockade by MDL 100,907 a highly selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonist. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 259 137–141. 10.1016/0014-2999(94)90502-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek G. J., Aghajanian G. K. (1996). LSD and the phenethylamine hallucinogen DOI are potent partial agonists at 5-HT2A receptors on interneurons in rat piriform cortex. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 278 1373–1382. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87 873–904. 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntyre D. C., Hutcheon B., Schwabe K., Poulter M. O. (2002). Divergent GABA(A) receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in genetically seizure-prone and seizure-resistant rats. J. Neurosci. 22 9922–9931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merali Z., Khan S., Michaud D. S., Shippy S. A., Anisman H. (2004). Does amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) mediate anxiety-like behaviors? Dissociation of anxiogenic effects and CRH release. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20 229–239. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03468.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mora F., Segovia G., Del A. A., de B. M., Garrido P. (2012). Stress, neurotransmitters, corticosterone and body-brain integration. Brain Res. 1476 71–85. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller M. B., Zimmermann S., Sillaber I., Hagemeyer T. P., Deussing J. M., Timpl P., et al. (2003). Limbic corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1 mediates anxiety-related behavior and hormonal adaptation to stress. Nat. Neurosci. 6 1100–1107. 10.1038/nn1123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naughton M., Mulrooney J. B., Leonard B. E. (2000). A review of the role of serotonin receptors in psychiatric disorders. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 15 397–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pazos A., Cortes R., Palacios J. M. (1985). Quantitative autoradiographic mapping of serotonin receptors in the rat brain. II. Serotonin-2 receptors. Brain Res. 346 231–249. 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90857-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Primus R. J., Yevich E., Baltazar C., Gallager D. W. (1997). Autoradiographic localization of CRF1 and CRF2 binding sites in adult rat brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 17 308–316. 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00071-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risbrough V. B., Stein M. B. (2006). Role of corticotropin releasing factor in anxiety disorders: a translational research perspective. Horm. Behav. 50 550–561. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon P. W., Aghajanian G. K. (1990). Serotonin (5-HT) induces IPSPs in pyramidal layer cells of rat piriform cortex: evidence for the involvement of a 5-HT2-activated interneuron. Brain Res. 506 62–69. 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91199-Q [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon P. W., Aghajanian G. K. (1991). Excitatory responses to serotonin (5-HT) in neurons of the rat piriform cortex: evidence for mediation by 5-HT1C receptors in pyramidal cells and 5-HT2 receptors in interneurons. Synapse 9 208–218. 10.1002/syn.890090307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheng H., Zhang Y., Sun J., Gao L., Ma B., Lu J., et al. (2008). Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) depresses n-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-mediated current in cultured rat hippocampal neurons via CRH receptor type 1. Endocrinology 149 1389–1398. 10.1210/en.2007-1378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan H., Zhong P., Yan Z. (2004). Corticotropin-releasing factor and acute stress prolongs serotonergic regulation of GABA transmission in prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci. 24 5000–5008. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0143-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tao F., Su Q., Johns R. A. (2008). Cell-permeable peptide Tat-PSD-95 PDZ2 inhibits chronic inflammatory pain behaviors in mice. Mol. Ther. 16 1776–1782. 10.1038/mt.2008.192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van P. K., Viau V., Bittencourt J. C., Chan R. K., Li H. Y., Arias C., et al. (2000). Distribution of mRNAs encoding CRF receptors in brain and pituitary of rat and mouse. J. Comp. Neurol. 428 191–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von W. G., Avrabos C., Stepan J., Wurst W., Deussing J. M., Holsboer F., et al. (2011). Voltage-sensitive dye imaging demonstrates an enhancing effect of corticotropin-releasing hormone on neuronal activity propagation through the hippocampal formation. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45 256–261. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wietfeld D., Heinrich N., Furkert J., Fechner K., Beyermann M., Bienert M., et al. (2004). Regulation of the coupling to different G proteins of rat corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 1 in human embryonic kidney 293 cells. J. Biol. Chem. 279 38386–38394. 10.1074/jbc.M405335200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zorrilla E. P., Logrip M. L., Koob G. F. (2014). Corticotropin releasing factor: a key role in the neurobiology of addiction. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35:234–244. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]