Table 2.
Recommendation | Reason | Effect | Evidence |
---|---|---|---|
Prior to FA implementation | |||
Rule out biological/medical events | May negate assessment and treatment |
|
Kennedy and Meyer (1996) O’Reilly (1995) |
Limit access to reinforcers outside of the FA | May abolish responding |
|
McGinnis et al. (2010) O’Reilly et al. (2009) |
Limit the number of target behaviors | Responses that contact reinforcement may abolish motivation for other responses |
|
Asmus et al. (2003) Jessel et al. (2014) Beavers and Iwata (2011) |
Prescreen for automatic reinforcement (when suspected) | May reduce ambiguity and make FA more efficient |
|
Querim et al. (2013) |
During FA implementation | |||
Use a fixed sequence | Limits potential carryover effects by providing control condition immediately following test |
|
Hammond et al. (2013) Iwata, Dorsey, et al. (1982/1994) |
Use different SDs in each FA condition | Limits potential carryover effects by signaling the current condition |
|
Conners et al. (2000) |
Allow problem behavior to subside before beginning the next session | Limits potential carryover effects |
|
McGonigle et al. (1987) |
Use low preferred toys or no toys in the attention condition | Toys may compete with attention |
|
Roscoe, Carreau, MacDonald, and Pence (2008) |
Use attention form typically used | Ensures attention is relevant |
|
Kodak, Northup, and Kelley (2007) Piazza et al. (1999) |
Conduct demand assessment to identify tasks to include in the demand condition | Ensures demands are aversive |
|
Call, Pabico, and Lomas (2009) Roscoe, Rooker, Pence, and Longworth (2009) |
Include tasks typically used | Ensures demands are aversive |
|
Asmus et al. (1999) Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, and Cataldo (1990) McCord, Iwata, Galensky, Ellingson, and Thomson (2001) McComas, Hoch, Paone, and El-Roy (2000), Smith, Iwata, Goh, and Shore (1995) |
Use descriptive assessment to determine items in tangible condition | Ensure that tangible item is similar to delivery outside of the FA |
|
Rooker et al. (2011) |
Use people as therapists who usually provide the consequence | Ensures that appropriate SDs and reinforcers are present |
|
English and Anderson (2004) McAdam, DiCesare, Murphy, and Marshall (2004) Thomason-Sassi, Iwata, and Fritz (2013) |
Post-session | |||
Graph all target behavior separately | Ensures responding is related to the relevant stimulus conditions |
|
Derby et al. (1994) |
Designate appropriate control conditions | Makes analysis easier |
|
Fischer, Iwata, and Worsdell (1997) Kahng and Iwata (1998) |
Graph responding occurring when the motivational operation is present | Ensures responding is related to the relevant motivation |
|
Roane, Lerman, Kelley, and Van Camp (1999) |
Use statistical guidelines for visual analysis | Ensures consistency in FA outcomes |
|
Hagopian et al. (1997) Roane, Fisher, Kelley, and Mevers (2013) |