Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jul 31.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Interv. 2014 Oct 16;30(1):1–35. doi: 10.1002/bin.1400

Table 2.

Summary of guidelines for best practice functional analysis (FA).

Recommendation Reason Effect Evidence
Prior to FA implementation
Rule out biological/medical events May negate assessment and treatment
  • Ensures results are not related to a biological process

Kennedy and Meyer (1996)
O’Reilly (1995)
Limit access to reinforcers outside of the FA May abolish responding
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

McGinnis et al. (2010)
O’Reilly et al. (2009)
Limit the number of target behaviors Responses that contact reinforcement may abolish motivation for other responses
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false positive

  • Limit false negative

Asmus et al. (2003)
Jessel et al. (2014)
Beavers and Iwata (2011)
Prescreen for automatic reinforcement (when suspected) May reduce ambiguity and make FA more efficient
  • Determines if behavior is maintained by automatic reinforcement

Querim et al. (2013)
During FA implementation
Use a fixed sequence Limits potential carryover effects by providing control condition immediately following test
  • Enhance discrimination

Hammond et al. (2013)
Iwata, Dorsey, et al. (1982/1994)
Use different SDs in each FA condition Limits potential carryover effects by signaling the current condition
  • Enhance discrimination

Conners et al. (2000)
Allow problem behavior to subside before beginning the next session Limits potential carryover effects
  • Enhance discrimination

McGonigle et al. (1987)
Use low preferred toys or no toys in the attention condition Toys may compete with attention
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

Roscoe, Carreau, MacDonald, and Pence (2008)
Use attention form typically used Ensures attention is relevant
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

Kodak, Northup, and Kelley (2007)
Piazza et al. (1999)
Conduct demand assessment to identify tasks to include in the demand condition Ensures demands are aversive
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

Call, Pabico, and Lomas (2009)
Roscoe, Rooker, Pence, and Longworth (2009)
Include tasks typically used Ensures demands are aversive
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

Asmus et al. (1999)
Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, and Cataldo (1990)
McCord, Iwata, Galensky, Ellingson, and Thomson (2001)
McComas, Hoch, Paone, and El-Roy (2000),
Smith, Iwata, Goh, and Shore (1995)
Use descriptive assessment to determine items in tangible condition Ensure that tangible item is similar to delivery outside of the FA
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative or positive

Rooker et al. (2011)
Use people as therapists who usually provide the consequence Ensures that appropriate SDs and reinforcers are present
  • Increase responding

  • Limit false negative

English and Anderson (2004)
McAdam, DiCesare, Murphy, and Marshall (2004)
Thomason-Sassi, Iwata, and Fritz (2013)
Post-session
Graph all target behavior separately Ensures responding is related to the relevant stimulus conditions
  • Differentiation may be easier to observe

Derby et al. (1994)
Designate appropriate control conditions Makes analysis easier
  • Differentiation may be easier to observe

Fischer, Iwata, and Worsdell (1997)
Kahng and Iwata (1998)
Graph responding occurring when the motivational operation is present Ensures responding is related to the relevant motivation
  • Differentiation may be easier to observe

Roane, Lerman, Kelley, and Van Camp (1999)
Use statistical guidelines for visual analysis Ensures consistency in FA outcomes
  • Differentiation may be easier to observe

Hagopian et al. (1997)
Roane, Fisher, Kelley, and Mevers (2013)