Skip to main content
Iranian Journal of Public Health logoLink to Iranian Journal of Public Health
. 2015 May;44(5):646–653.

Mother’s Happiness with Cognitive - Executive Functions and Facial Emotional Recognition in School Children with Down Syndrome

Maryam MALMIR 1,*, Maryam SEIFENARAGHI 1, Dariush D FARHUD 2,3, GAli AFROOZ 4, Mohammad KHANAHMADI 5
PMCID: PMC4537621  PMID: 26284205

Abstract

Background:

According to the mother’s key roles in bringing up emotional and cognitive abilities of mentally retarded children and respect to positive psychology in recent decades, this research is administered to assess the relation between mother’s happiness level with cognitive- executive functions (i.e. attention, working memory, inhibition and planning) and facial emotional recognition ability as two factors in learning and adjustment skills in mentally retarded children with Down syndrome.

Methods:

This study was an applied research and data were analyzed by Pearson correlation procedure. Population is included all school children with Down syndrome (9–12 yr) that come from Tehran, Iran. Overall, 30 children were selected as an in access sample. After selection and agreement of parents, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) was performed to determine the student’s IQ, and then mothers were invited to fill out the Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI). Cognitive-executive functions were evaluated by tests as followed: Continues Performance Test (CPT), N-Back, Stroop test (day and night version) and Tower of London. Ekman emotion facial expression test was also accomplished for assessing facial emotional recognition in children with Down syndrome, individually.

Results:

Mother’s happiness level had a positive relation with cognitive-executive functions (attention, working memory, inhibition and planning) and facial emotional recognition in her children with Down syndrome, significantly.

Conclusion:

Parents’ happiness (especially mothers) is a powerful predictor for cognitive and emotional abilities of their children.

Keywords: Happiness, Cognitive-Executive function, Facial emotional recognition, Down syndrome

Introduction

Happiness as a concept was attended by most of scientists and philosophers during the time. Most of philosophers and historians believe that, at the first, happiness was focused on good chance, but now, happiness defined as an emotion that is under control of human and persuaded as a goal. Centuries before Christ, the word eudemonia was used for well-being and it is translated in English as happiness. Historians stated that, firstly, this word was used in poems of Hesiode. However, there was a deterministic perspective about happiness in ancient Greece. Socrates believed that happiness is a content that is briefly in hands of people (1). He defined happiness as being and behaving well and living in its shadow.

Many terms are used as a synonym of happiness up to now: life satisfaction, quality of life, well-being, positive emotions, interesting and so on (2). Today, the word subjective well-being is the common term for happiness (3).

From the mid of twenty century, scientific researchers have been performed about the role of happiness in social sciences (46). Most of studies were related to happiness and its predictors. Happiness is correlated with factors like cognitive abilities, personality, behaviors, biology and emotions (711). Happiness has a positive and reciprocal relationship with cognitive abilities (1214).

Human being bringing in a complex system up and several aspects of environment influence on him/her. An effective factor is the family. Family and each member of family have a reciprocal interaction. Therefore, it can be said that parents have influence on children and vice versa. So, several studies accomplished in this area (15, 16). In recent decades, studies are directed to exceptional children family. By using these researches, our knowledge about the family of children with disabilities is promoted (17). Although primary studies were focused on negative effects of parents’ limitations, recent studies include positive effects of parents’ behaviors (18).

Recent research models are investigating both strengths and distresses in the family of children with mental retardation. This studies respect to effects of parents on children and effects of child on parents (18, 19). It is clear that family structure has an important impact on forming child’s skills (20, 21). Applying educational programs for parents leads to improvement in children abilities (22). An important factor that should be investigated in child-parent interaction is the effect of parents’ emotions on cognitive and affective abilities of their children. Parents’ emotions like happiness can play an important role in child’s development, because parents’ emotions affect both child’s performance and their relation with family members (23). Therefore, such effects should be studied in children with mental retardation.

Among mentally retarded children, Down syndrome has a special place: First, the prevalence of this syndrome is high. Second, requirements of this group are not respected. Third, with existing facilities and learning methods, the ability of children with Down syndrome can be improved. Down syndrome is a chromosomal disorder that influence on emotional, cognitive and physical abilities. The important factor for training to this group is the realization of factors that affected emotional and cognitive abilities (24).

For example among cognitive abilities, cognitive-executive functions play an important role in learning process. Cognitive-executive functions are under control of prefrontal of brain and it is responsible for planning, control and managing behavior (25). Important indicators of cognitive-executive functions are as follow: attention, working memory, inhibition planning (26). Children with Down syndrome have problem in attention, working memory, inhibition and planning. Improvement in this area should be respected (2730).

In addition, among emotional abilities, interpreting the facial emotions is a necessary skill that capable people to direct their social environment (31). Emotions recognition from the face is the first sign for understanding feelings and making empathy relations. Facial expressions recognition is common in all people in the world and it is not related to special culture. Normal children (after 4 yr) are able to recognize emotions like happy, fear, anger, and sadness (32). People with mental retardation like Down syndrome have deficits in facial emotional recognition (31, 33).

In one hand, the family is an effective system in the life of human and in another hand; happiness influences emotional and cognitive abilities of individuals. In the scope of exceptional children, the relation between parent’s happiness and emotional-cognitive abilities of children with mental retardation is on top. Among emotional abilities, cognitive-executive functions are the most effective factors that affect learning and social adaptation of mentally retarded children. Down syndrome is attended as an important and wide group among all groups with mental retardation.

In this study, we discuss the relation between maternal happiness level with cognitive-executive function and facial emotional recognition in children with Down syndrome.

Materials and Methods

This study is an applicable research and data are analyzed by correlation procedure. Population is included all school children with Down syndrome (9–12 yrs) that come from Tehran. According to their IQ (50–70), they are placed in experimental group. Among all, 30 students with mean age 10.5 yrs selected as an in access sample from Resalat, Touhid and Pirouzi schools. After selection and agreement of parents, the WISC-R is performed to determine the student’s IQ. The mean IQ score of the sample was 65. The sample includes 9 female and 21 male students. The mean age of mothers was 45 yrs.

It should be noted that children with epilepsy were missed from experiment because using computerized tests can trigger the onset in epileptic children.

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R)

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-revised (WISC-R), is a general test of intelligence. The WISC-R consist of 12 subtests that it is divided into two parts, verbal and non-verbal. Validity and reliability of the test are assessed in several studies. Reliability of the test (in Iran) through test- retest is 0.79 to 0.96.

Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI) (34)

The OHI comprises 29 items, each involving the selection of one of four points (Likert scale) that are different for each item. The highest score in this scale is 87, which shows the highest point of happiness. Normal and mean range score in this scale is 40 to 42.Reliability of the OHI is 0.91 and internal correlation of items is about 0.04 to 0.67. In addition, reliability of the test in Iran by test-retest is 0.79.

Continues Performance Test (CPT) (35)

CPT is a neuropsychological test. This test is computerized and it will be used to evaluate attention disorder. In this research, the Persian form of CPT is used. This test has two forms: numbers and pictures. The picture form of the test for mentally retarded children is used. The test included 150 stimulus, 30 stimulus are goal that the subject has to answer to it with pressing the button. Reliability of the test in Iran is 0.59. In current study, for assessing the validity of test, experts’ rate is used. In addition, reliability of the test assessed in a group (N=15) with Down syndrome (0.68).

N-Backtest (36)

N-Back is a task for assessing working memory as a subscale of cognitive-executive function. In current study, 1-Back was used (comparing current stimulus with recent stimulus). The whole time of performing test is 3 minutes. This test has a significant correlation with other tests that assessing working memory. Reliability of the test assessed in several studies and it is reported about 0.79, 0.81 and 0.84. Authors used experts’ rate for validity and assessed reliability on a group of children (N=15) with Down syndrome (0.76).

Stroop Task – Day/Night Version (28)

This task assesses the ability to inhibit a predominant response. In this task, participants were asked to say ‘night’ when they saw a picture of the sun drawn on a white card, and ‘day’ when they saw a picture of moon and stars drawn on a black card. After being given a brief practice trial, participants were then shown a sequence of 16 cards arranged in a set order. The minimum score was 0 and the maximum was 16, for experimental condition. Reliability is reported 0.86 and validity of the test is based on experts’ rate. In addition, reliability (test-retest) of the test is assessed by authors on a group of children (n=15) with Down syndrome and it is obtained 0.78.

Tower of London Test (37)

This test was used for planning abilities of patients with frontal lobe disorder. In current study, computerized version of test is used. Tower of London Test includes two sets. Each set has three bases with three colored balls (green, blue and red). The subject has to change second set with the first one. The whole time for performing the test is 3 minutes. The correlation between results of this test and Proteus test is reported 0.41. Reliability of the test is reported 0.79. In addition, reliability (test-retest) of the test was assessed by authors on a group of children (n=15) with Down syndrome and it is obtained 0.81.

Emotion Facial expression Test (38)

This test is common and reliable in emotion facial recognition. In the current study, from 110 pictures of original test, 36 pictures of 3 males and 3 females were selected. Pictures included 6 basic emotions (happy, anger, sad, hate, fear and surprise). Reliability of the test assessed in Iran and it is obtained 0.71. Validity of the test is based on experts’ rate. In addition, reliability of the test assessed by authors through test- retest, on a group of children (n=15) with Down syndrome and it is obtained 0.79.

In this study, after selection of sample and agreement of family to take part in research, schoolchildren’ IQ is assessed by WISC-R. Then, mothers were asked to fill the Oxford happiness inventory. For assessing abilities of schoolchildren with Down syndrome the following tests is used: CPT, N-Back, Stroop test (day/night), London tower test and Emotion Facial expression test. These tests performed individually.

For data analysis, the descriptive and interfering statistics was used. First, data were categorized in tables and figures then by using Pearson correlation coefficient, the relation between variables was assessed.

Results

Obtained data were categorized in order and by using descriptive mathematics, mean and standard deviation were assessed for each variable. Findings are presented in Table 1.

Table 1:

Mean and standard deviation of subjects for each variable

Variables SD
Children with DS CPT 123.6 20.22
N-Back 55.06 19.52
Stroop 5.43 2.82
Tower of London 10.7 5.8
Emotion Facial Expression Test 19.93 4.18
Mothers of Children with DS OHI 39.9 10.03

For assessing the relation between mothers happiness level as an independent variable, with 5 dependent variable in research (attention, working memory, inhibition, planning and facial emotional recognition), the Pearson correlation coefficient was used. Findings are showed in Table 2.

Table 2:

Correlation coefficient between mothers’ scores in OHI and their children (with Down syndrome) scores in cognitive-executive functions and facial emotional recognition

Variables R R2 Adjusted R2 Std. error of the estimate Change statistics
R2 T df sig
CPT 0.636 0.404 0.383 15.8 0.404 4.35 28 0.05
N-Back 0.425 0.181 0.151 17.9 0.181 2.48 28 0.05
Stroop 0.528 0.279 0.254 2.4 0.279 3.29 28 0.05
Tower of London 0.691 0.477 0.458 4.2 0.477 5.05 28 0.05
Emotion Facial Expression Test 0.622 0.387 0.366 3.3 0.387 4.2 28 0.05

In Table 2, the correlation analysis between mothers’ scores in OHI and their children (with Down syndrome) abilities in cognitive-executive functions and facial emotional recognition is indicated. The mathematical model used for analyzing research hypothesis – relation between happiness and each variable- is the t distribution. In comparing with critical value of t (df. =28, α=0.05, t=2.04), Obtained t value for each variable is high, then zero hypothesis is rejected and research hypothesis is accepted. Moreover, as observed correlation coefficient between mothers’ happiness with each variables is significantly higher than table critical value (α=0.05, p=0.36), zero hypothesis is rejected and it is concluded that there is a positive and significant relation between mothers’ happiness with their children abilities (attention, working memory, inhibition, planning and facial emotional recognition).

Discussion

Results show that happiness of mothers of children with Down syndrome has a positive and significant relation with cognitive – executive functions and facial emotional recognition ability in their children.

According to the authors search, there is no match study with the title search of current research. However, there are similar studies that used as supportive literature. These supportive findings are divided into two categories:

  • - In the scope of classical psychology; studies that evaluating the relationship between mothers’ negative mood (opposite of happiness), with cognitive-executive functions and facial emotional recognition of their children.

  • - In the scope of positive psychology; studies that evaluating the relationship between happiness and its synonyms in mothers, with cognitive-executive functions and facial emotional recognition of their children.

Mothers’ happiness & Cognitive-Executive functions of their children:

A group of studies indicate that emotional and mental characteristics of mothers are the important factors that have influence on cognitive-emotional abilities of their children.

Depression (opposite form of happiness) as an emotional factor has a negative and significant impact on cognitive-executive function abilities of children. It means that mothers’ negative emotion is a predictor of decreasing the abilities of children (39).

A researcher investigated the effects of mothers’ depression on family communications. Findings show that mothers’ depression has a negative impact on children. One of the negative impacts is the attention deficit in children (40). Children of mothers with depression are more likely with attention deficit (41). Forty one percent of children with attention deficit, affective disorder and anxiety have a depressed mother (42).

Mothers’ depression can influence on brain and its functions like memory (43, 44). Its biological reason is increasing of production of stress hormone during pregnancy that leads to problems for brain growth (45). Vulnerability of infants with developmental disabilities is more than others (46). Brain activities of children with depressed mother will be changed. Children with depressed mother have more problems in the activity of frontal and pre-frontal than children with non-depressed mother (47).

In another study, the effect of mothers’ depression on cognitive-executive functions of children was evaluated. They conclude that mothers’ depression has a negative and significant effect on inhibition ability of children (44).

In a classical view, the effect of mothers’ depression during pregnancy is investigated and they figured the negative and significant effect of mothers’ depression on planning ability of children (39, 48).

A group of research in the scope of positive psychology investigates the relationship between happiness and life-satisfaction in mothers of children with attention deficit. Findings figure a positive and significant relation between subjective-wellbeing in mothers and attention ability in their children. Parents of children with attention deficit (generally), and their mothers (specially) express a lower life satisfaction and lower happiness in comparing with normal population (49).

Family environment is an important factor that affects children’s ability. When parents are not preparing to take new responsibilities, then stress and negative emotions cover their interactions. Continuous stress in family leads to common problems for children. A problem that affects children is the deficit attention. Moreover, life satisfaction and happiness, in this family, are in low level (50).

Mother’s positive mood likely leads to positive interaction with children and cognitive-emotion will be affected by this atmosphere, so improvement in abilities of children will be seen (51).

In the scope of positive psychology, it is focused on the relation of happiness and cognitive abilities. Several studies indicate the effect of mothers’ happiness on cognitive abilities of children (51, 52). Findings indicate that mothers’ happiness leads to improvement in cognitive abilities of children (23). Increasing of life satisfaction in parents leads to increasing the ability of inhibition (53). Subjective well-being in mothers leads to the optimal planning function in their children (49).

Mothers’ happiness & facial emotional recognition of their children:

Primary experiences of children have a significant effect on their facial emotional recognition ability (54).

In a classical view, the effect of parents’ depression on regulation of emotion is investigated (55). If mothers are affected by depression, it is more likely that their child has a problem in emotional recognition (56). Mothers’ severe depression is a powerful predictor for emotional problems in their children (57, 58).

Effects of mothers’ depression on children behaviors are summarized as follow:

  • - Low adjustment

  • - Low awareness toward environment

  • - Decrease of interaction behaviors

  • - Finally, decrease of emotional recognition and empathy (42).

Conclusion

Mothers’ happiness level as an important factor is presented in cognitive abilities of children, especially mentally retarded children. Mentally retarded children with happier mothers show significant performance on cognitive executive functions (memory, attention, inhibition and planning) and facial emotional recognition abilities.

Ethical consideration

Parents’ agreements received before starting the research. In addition, special codes assigned instead the name of schoolchildren. Ethical issues (Including plagiarism, informed consent, misconduct, data fabrication and/or falsification, double publication and/or submission, redundancy, etc.) have been completely observed by the authors.

Acknowledgment

We wish to express our appreciation to the managers and staffs of schools (Tohid, Resalat, and Pirouzi) and parents of school children for their helpful assistance in accomplishing the research. The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests.

References

  • 1. Oishi S, Graham J, Kesebir S, Galinha IC. (2013). Concept of happiness across time and cultures. Personality and Soc Psychol Bulletin, 39: 559–577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Ojanen M. (2008). Can We Raise the Level of Happiness? Helsinki Collegiums for Advanced Studies, 41 – 58
  • 3. Malmir M, Khanahmadi M, Farhoud DD. (2013). Happiness and its predictors factors (in press). [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Diener ED, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. (1999). Subjective well-being, three decades of progress. Psychol Bulletin, 125(2): 276–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Frey BS, Stutzer A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? J of Econ Literature, 40(2): 402–435 [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Schimmack U, Schupp J, Wagner GG. (2007). The influence of environment & personality on the affective & cognitive component of subjective wellbeing. Soc Indicators Res, 89(1): 41–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Veenhovean R. (2006). How do we assess how happy we are? tenets, implications & tenability of three theories. Paper present at conference on new direction in the study happiness, United States & international perspectives. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Galinha IC, Ribeiro JLP. (2011). Cognitive, affective & contextual predictors of SWB. Int J Wellbeing, 2(1): 34–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Veenhovean R, Choi Y. (2012). Does Intelligence Boost Happiness? Smartness of all pays more that being smarter than others. Int J Happiness Develop, 1: 5–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Malmir M, Khanahmadi M, Farhud DD. (2013). Relation between personality factors and happiness (in press). [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Malmir M, Khanahmadi M, Farhud DD. (2013). Relation between cognitive factors and happiness (in press). [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Malmir M, Khanahmadi M, Farhud DD. (2013). Relation between behavioral factors and happiness (in press). [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Farhud DD, Malmir M, Khanahmadi M. (2013). Happiness and health, the biological factors. Iran J Public Health, 43( 11): 1468–1478. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Sasanpour M, Khodabakhshi M, Nooryan Kh. (2012). The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence, Happiness and Mental Health in Students of Medical Sciences of Isfahan University. Int J Collaborative Res on Internal Med & Public Health, 4: 1614–1620. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Falci CD. (1997). The effects of family & family process on the psychological wellbeing of children . From the children point of view, the faculty of Virjinia polytechnic Institute.
  • 16. Logan DE, Scharff L. (2005). Relationships between family & parents characteristics in children with recurrent pain syndrome: an investigation of moderating effects on the path way from pain to disability. J Pediater Psycol, 30( 8): 698–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Crnic K, Arbona APY, Baker B, Blacher J. (2009). Mothers & Fathers Together: Contrasts in parenting across preschool to early school age in children with developmental delays. Int Rev Res Ment Retard, 37: 3–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Blacher J, Baker BL. (2007). Positive impact of intellectual disability on families. Am J Ment Retard, 112: 330–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Baker BL, Blacher J, Kopp CB, Kraemer B. (1997). Parenting children with mental retardation. Int Rev Res Ment Retard, 20: 1–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Hill MS, Yeung WJJ, Duncan GJ. (2001). Childhood Family Structureand Young Adult Behaviors. J Population Econ, 14( 2): 271–299. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Dahl G, Lochner L. (2012). The Impact of Family Income on Child Achievement: Evidence from the Earned Income Tax Credit. Am Econ Rev, 102( 5): 1927–1956. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Seifnaraghi M, Naderi E. (2010). Psychol and Edu of MR Children. 7th ed Tehran, SAMT press. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Nikolau D. (2012). Happy mother’s successful children: effects of maternal life satisfaction on child’s outcomes [these PhD] The Ohio state university; Ohaio: p. 95–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Afrooz GA. (2004). Introduction to the: psychology & rehabilitation of Down’s Syndrome children. 4th ed Tehran, University of Tehran press. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Luria AR. (1966). Higher cortical function in man. New York, Basic Books; p. 144–230 [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Best JA, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. (2011). Relation between Executive function and academic achievement from age 5–17 in large representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21: 327–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Raw J, Lawender A, Turk V. (2006). Cognitive Executive Function in Down syndrome. Br J Clin Psychol, 45: 5–17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory: a functional dissociation in persons with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 38: 240–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Danielsson H, Henry L, Messer D, Rönnberg J. (2012). Strengths and weaknesses in executive functioning in children with intellectual disability. J Res Develop Disabil, 33( 2): 600–607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Lanfranchi S, Jerman O, Dal Pont E, Alberti A, Vianello R. (2010). Executive Function in adolescents with Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res, 54: 308–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Zaja RH, Rojahn J. (2008). Facial emotion recognition in intellectual disability. Curr Opin Psychiatry, 21 (5): 441–444 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Rump KM, Giovannell JL, Minshew NJ, Strauss MS. (2009). The developmental of emotion recognition individuals with autism. Child Development, 80: 1434–1447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Kasari C, Freeman SFN, Hughes MA. (2001). Emotion recognition by children with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard, 106( 1): 59–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Hills P, Argile M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: a compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33: 1073–1082. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Azadfalah P, Rasolzade K, Soltanifar F, Mashhadi A. (2013). Continues Performance Test [computerized software]. Tehran: Sina Cognitive & behavioral Science Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Azadfalah P, FathiAshtiani A, Nazarboland F. (2013). N-Back Test [computerized software]. Tehran: Sina Cognitive & behavioral Science Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Azadfalah P, Rasolzade K, Soltanifar F, Mashhadi A. (2013). Tower of London Test [computerized software]. Tehran: Sina Cognitive & behavioral Science Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Ekman P, Friesen W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Hughes C, Roman G, Hart MJ, Ensor R. (2013). Does maternal depression predict young children’s executive function? - A 4-year longitudinal study. J Child Psycol psychiatry, 54( 2): 169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Burke L. (2003). The impact of maternal depression on familial relationships. Int Rev Psychiatr, 15: 243–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Wiedmann M, Garfield C. (2007). Maternal depression and child development: strategies for primary care providers. Family practice education network, Illinois: pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Orvaschel H, Walsh-Allis G, Ye WJ. (1988). Psychopathology in children of parents with recurrent depression. J Abnormal Child Psychol, 16( 1): 17–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Shonkoff JP, Duncan GJ, Yoshikawa H, Guyer B, Magnuson K, Philips D, et al. (2009). Maternal depression can undermine the development of young children. Centre on the developing child, Harvard University. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Reising MM. (2013). The effects of chronic stress of executive function in children of depressed parents [these PhD], Nashville, Tennessee. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Diego MA, Field T, Hernandez RM, Schanberg S, Kuhn C, Gonzalez – Quintera VH. (2009). Prenatal depression restricts fetal growth. Early Human Develop, 85: 65–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Golden O, McDaniel M, Loprest P, Stanczyk A. (2013). Disconnected Mothers and the Well-Being of Children: A Research Report. The Urban Institute; pp. 7–11 [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Dawson G, Sharon BA, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Self J, Yamada E, Lara Embry L. (2003). Pre-school Outcomes of Children of Depressed Mothers: Role of Maternal Behavior, Contextual Risk, and Children’s Brain Activity. Child Develop, 74( 4): 1158–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Sohr – Preston SL, Scaramella L. (2006). Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev, 9( 1): 65–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Sun J. (2003). Early indicators of executive functions and attention in preterm and full term infant [these PhD] Queensland University, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Chutko LS, Anisimova TI, Surushkina YU, Aitbekov KA. (2011). Family problems and family therapy in attention deficit disorder. Neuroscience Behavioral Physiol, 41( 4): 404–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Jouriles EN, Murphy CM, O’Leary KD. (1989). Effects of maternal mood on mother – child interaction patterns. J Abnormal Child Psychol, 17( 5): 513–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Fahey T, Keilthy P, Polek E. (2012). Family Relationships and Family Well-Being: A Study of the Families of Nine Year-Olds in Ireland [these PhD] University College Dublin, Dublin: pp. 50–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Bumrind D. (1980). Effects of authoritative parenting on child behavior. National Institute of Ment Health, 5: 884–907. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Schmidt AT, Orsten KD, Hanten GR, Li X, Levin HS. (2012). Family environment influences emotion recognition following paediatric traumatic brain Injury. Brain Inj, 24(13–14): 1550–1560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Feng X, Shaw DS, Kovacs M, Lane T, O’Rourke FE, Alarcon JH. (2008). Emotion Regulation in Preschoolers: The Roles of Behavioral Inhibition, Maternal Affective Behavior, and Maternal Depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatr, 49( 2): 132–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Silk JS, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Lane TL, Kovacs M. (2006). Maternal Depression and Child Internalizing: The Moderating Role of Child Emotion Regulation. J Clin Child Adolescent Psychol, 35( 1): 116–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Perry MA, Fantuzzo J. (2010). A Multivariate Investigation of Maternal Risks and Their Relationship to Low-Income, Preschool Children’s Competencies. Appl Develop Sci, 14( 1): 1–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Berger EM, Spieb CK. (2011). Maternal life satisfaction and child outcomes: are they related? J Econ Psychol, 32(1): 142–158. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Iranian Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of Tehran University of Medical Sciences

RESOURCES