Abstract
Individuals with agrammatic aphasia exhibit restricted patterns of impairment of functional morphemes, however, syntactic characterization of the impairment is controversial. Previous studies have focused on functional morphology in clauses only. This study extends the empirical domain by testing functional morphemes in English nominal phrases in aphasia and comparing patients’ impairment to their impairment of functional morphemes in English clauses. In the linguistics literature, it is assumed that clauses and nominal phrases are structurally parallel but exhibit inflectional differences. The results of the present study indicated that aphasic speakers evinced similar impairment patterns in clauses and nominal phrases. These findings are consistent with the Distributed Morphology Hypothesis (DMH), suggesting that the source of functional morphology deficits among agrammatics relates to difficulty implementing rules that convert inflectional features into morphemes. Our findings, however, are inconsistent with the Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH), which suggests that patients have difficulty building complex hierarchical structures.
Keywords: Agrammatic aphasia, Functional category, Nominal phrase, Clause
1. Introduction
It is well known that individuals with agrammatic aphasia have difficulty with functional morphemes, including both free standing function words and bound morphemes, in production as well as in comprehension and grammaticality judgment tasks in some cases (Benedet, Christiansen, & Goodglass, 1998; Berndt & Caramazza, 1980; Caramazza & Zurif, 1976; Damasio, 1992; Marshall, 1986; Parisi & Pizzamiglio, 1970). In English, these elements include tense markers (e.g. -ed), the plural marker (e.g. -s), auxiliaries (e.g. is and was), etc. This observation has attracted researchers’ attention because patterns of deficit may shed light on the linguistic and associated neural mechanisms that compute grammatical morphology (Avrutin, 2001; Friedmann, 2001, 2006; Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997; Grodzinsky, 1990, chap. 1). The research so far has revealed that a similar deficit can be seen in a wide variety of languages, including Dutch (Bastiaanse, 2008), English (Dickey, Milman, & Thompson, 2005, 2008; Lee, Milman, & Thompson, 2005, 2008), German (Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004, 2005), Hebrew (Friedmann, 2001, 2006; Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997), Japanese (Hagiwara, 1995) and other languages.
One explanation of these deficit patterns is the Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH) (Friedmann, 2001, 2002; Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997), which attributes the impairment of functional morphemes to damage to the nodes in the syntactic tree. Specifically, the TPH predicts that functional morphemes on the higher nodes of a syntactic structure are more likely to be impaired than those on lower nodes. Even though the TPH explains some deficit patterns, subsequent studies have reported many deficit patterns that the TPH fails to capture (e.g. Arabatzi & Edwards, 2002; Bastiaanse & Thompson, 2003; Burchert, Swoboda-Moll, & De Bleser, 2005; Nanousi, Masterson, Druks, & Atkinson, 2006; Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004, 2005). Various alternative explanations to the TPH have been proposed in the literature (see Thompson, Kielar, & Fix, 2012; for review). One explanation is what we term the Distributed Morphology Hypothesis (DMH), proposed in Dickey et al. (2008), Lee et al. (2008) and Thompson, Fix, and Gitelman (2002). Based on the theoretical framework of Distributed Morphology (Embick & Noyer, 2007; Halle & Marantz, 1993; Harley & Noyer, 1999), this hypothesis claims that impairment of morphemes is not constrained by the structure of the sentence itself, but rather by rules of morphology which are independent of syntactic structure.
Several prior studies have examined functional morphology deficits in aphasia, interpreting the findings in light of these two hypotheses (i.e. the TPH and the DMH). However, these studies have primarily focused on functional morpheme in clauses (e.g. Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997; Lee et al., 2008; Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004, 2005). Furthermore, these studies have resulted in inconsistent findings, leaving the underlying source of functional category impairments unclear. In order to further examine these two hypotheses, this study extends the empirical domain by testing English nominal phrases. In the theoretical linguistics literature (Abney, 1987; Bernstein, 1993; Brame, 1981, 1982; Horrocks & Stavrou, 1987; Stowell, 1991; Szabolcsi, 1983, 1994), it has been revealed that nominal phrases exhibit a hierarchical structure parallel to clauses. On the other hand, a major distinction between the two is, as far as English is concerned, that morphological inflection patterns differ between nominal phrases and clauses. For example, the functional morphemes in the top node of the nominal structure (i.e. demonstratives) exhibit inflectional alternations (the singular demonstrative this vs. the plural demonstrative these) (Leu, 2008). The similarities in the hierarchical structure and the differences in the functional morphology between clauses and nominal phrases thus can potentially provide a good testing ground for the two hypotheses because these two hypotheses predict patterns of impairment of functional morphemes in nominal phrases which will be different from those in clauses. As an attempt to bring new evidence to distinguish the TPH and the DMH, this study aims to investigate agrammatic speakers’ impairment of functional morphemes inside nominal phrases (demonstratives and numerals), in comparison to functional morphemes in clauses (complementizers and auxiliary verbs).
2. Theoretical background
This section presents an introduction to the theoretical background assumed in this study. Details about the two hypotheses to be tested will be introduced first, followed by the parallelisms and differences between clauses and nominal phrases. The section ends with a summary of the predictions of impairment patterns on clauses and nominal phrases made by the two hypotheses.
2.1. Two hypotheses of functional category deficits
One hypothesis is the Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH), which relates the deficits of functional morphemes to the hierarchical structure of sentences (Friedmann, 1998, 2001, 2002, 2006; Friedmann & Grodzinsky, 1997, 2000; Grodzinsky, 2000a, 2000b; also see Hagiwara, 1995). Essentially, the TPH is built on the following model of grammar (Chomsky, 1981, chap. 1):
- (1)
According to this model, the derivation of syntactic structures goes through four levels of representation. The derivation starts at D-Structure, where logical-thematic theta roles match the grammatical functions of sentences. Afterwards, S-Structure is derived from D-Structure via application of transformations. Subsequently, the syntactic derivation splits, obtaining the phonological representation at PF and obtaining the semantic representation at LF.
Based on this model of grammar, an example of the TPH adopted by Friedmann and Grodzinsky (1997) assumes the following hierarchical structure for a clause in order to give a systematic account of aphasic patients’ impaired syntactic abilities:
Within this hierarchical structure, function words and bound functional morphemes may be represented in different functional projections. For example, CP hosts both complementizers (e.g. that) and wh words. Complementizers are in the head position of CP and wh words are in the specifier position of CP. Other functional projections include IP, which is responsible for the tense inflection of the verb and AgrP, which is responsible for the agreement in person, gender, and number between the subject and the verb. Finite verbs move from V, their base-generated position within the VP, to Agr and then to T in order to check (or collect) their inflection (Chomsky, 1992).
Specifically, the TPH makes the following predictions. First, the higher a syntactic projection is in the structure in (2), the more likely it is that the projection (including whatever occurs in this projection) will be impaired. Moreover, once a projection is impaired, any node above it will also be impaired. For example, CP is more likely to be impaired than IP, which is more likely to be impaired than AgrP; and if IP is impaired, CP will also be impaired.1 These predictions were confirmed by the results reported in Friedmann and Grodzinsky (1997), which tested a female Hebrew-speaking agrammatic patient on speech production tasks. They found that the patient was able to produce verbal, adjectival and nominal agreement but had difficulty with verbal tense inflections, copulas, and word order alternations between negation and the copula. For example, in sentence completion tasks, the patient was able to choose a correct verb form inflected for agreement but not able to choose a correct verb form inflected for tense. Additionally, her difficulty with CP-related elements (i.e. wh words, complementizers and embedded sentence structures) was noted. In a word, the patient had no difficulty with AgrP-related properties, but had difficulty with TP-related properties and those above TP (i.e. CP-related phenomena). Furthermore, the TPH predicts that breakdown at any node along the tree structure in (2) will disrupt both morphological and syntactic operations associated with that projection (Thompson, Kielar, et al., 2012). Impairment of CP, for instance, will lead to impairment of whatever occurs in CP (e.g. complementizers generated at C or wh words moved to Spec CP).
Although the predictions of the TPH have been supported by subsequent studies (Benedet et al., 1998; Friedmann, 2002; Gavarró & Martínez-Ferreiro, 2007; Hagiwara, 1995; Kolk, 2000), more recent research has produced results that are not predicted by the TPH. For example, some studies (Dickey et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2005, 2008; Thompson et al., 2002) have shown that patients’ accuracy for verb inflections was significantly lower than that for complementizers in English and other studies (e.g. Burchert et al., 2005) did not find a significant difference between tense and agreement for the tested patients.
In particular, Thompson and colleagues (Dickey et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2005, 2008; Thompson et al., 2002) found that aphasic individuals presented no difficulty with CP-related elements but had difficulty with tense inflections. For example, in both production and grammaticality judgment tasks, agrammatic participants had significantly higher accuracy for complementizers than for tense inflections. In addition, their tense inflection errors were dominated by substitutions of incorrect morphemes rather than omissions of morphemes, indicating their ability to project verb inflection and to implement inflectional rules in their grammar. These studies went on to propose an alternative account based on the theory of Distributed Morphology (DM) (Embick & Noyer, 2007; Halle & Marantz, 1993; Harley & Noyer, 1999). We refer to this hypothesis as the Distributed Morphology Hypothesis (DMH), which adopts the following model of grammar (Halle & Marantz, 1993: 114):
- (3)
In contrast to the model of grammar in (1), Distributed Morphology assumes that there is an interface called Morphological Structure (MS) located between S-Structure and PF. The basic idea of Distributed Morphology is that the machinery of what has traditionally been called morphology is computed among the several components of the grammar. Specifically, DM assumes that the entries that make up the Vocabulary of a language are each composed of two distinct sets of features: morphosyntactic/semantic and phonological. Moreover, at the syntactic levels of LF, D-Structure and S-Structure, terminal nodes in the syntactic structure possess morphosyntactic/semantic features but lack phonological features. It is at MS that terminal nodes obtain their phonological features through the mechanism of Vocabulary Insertion (VI), which converts morphosyntactic/semantic features into phonological features. An example to illustrate this process is the formation of talked: during VI, the suffix –ed is inserted to replace the feature [+PAST] possessed by talk and after the process of VI, the combination of talk with –ed is interpreted at PF. The DMH claims that even if aphasic individuals have intact hierarchical syntactic structure, impairment still results if they have flawed feature-to-morpheme mapping in MS. Therefore, the DMH predicts that aphasic individuals show impairment on those functional morphemes that involve inflectional alternations.
2.2. The structure of nominal phrases in relation to the structure of clauses
These two hypotheses (the TPH and the DMH) attempt to explain agrammatic patients’ impairment of functional projections by referring to different theoretical linguistic models, thus generating different predictions regarding deficit patterns. To date, these predictions have been tested exclusively on functional projections in clauses. In theoretical syntax, close parallelisms have been observed between clauses and nominal phrases in terms of their hierarchical functional projections (Abney, 1987; Bernstein, 1993; Brame, 1981, 1982; Horrocks & Stavrou, 1987; Stowell, 1991; Szabolcsi, 1983, 1994). This makes nominal phrases appropriate for testing the TPH, because this hypothesis reduces aphasic deficits to difficulty with building complex hierarchical structures. Meanwhile, inflections among functional morphemes in English nominal phrases are different from those in English clauses. This is appropriate for testing the DMH, as this hypothesis predicts that within the same node, only those functional morphemes that involve inflection will be impaired.
2.2.1. Parallelisms between clauses and nominal phrases in English
It has been established in generative syntax that the structure of clauses is analyzed as consisting of functional projections on top of the Verb Phrase, as illustrated in (2) (Chomsky, 1986, chap. 1; Pollock, 1989). Since the 1980s, numerous studies have proposed that nominal structure should also consist of functional projections on top of Noun Phrase in order to capture systematic parallelisms between clauses and nominal phrases (see Bernstein, 2003; for review). Specifically, Abney (1987) proposed that in nominal structure, a functional projection called the Determiner Phrase (DP) hosts determiner elements (for example, the in English) (Bernstein, 2003). In addition, subsequent research indicates that a Number Phrase intervenes between DP and NP (Lobeck, 1995, chap. 2; Ritter, 1991).
For the sake of highlighting the parallelism between clauses and nominal phrases, we assume clauses and nominal phrases have the following structures, respectively:
The structural parallelisms between clauses (4) and nominal phrases (5) are motivated by parallel properties between clauses and nominal phrases, as summarized in Table 1 and illustrated by the examples that follow.
Table 1.
Parallelisms | Examples |
---|---|
External distributions | (6a) and (6b) |
(6c) and (6d) | |
Taking the agent and the theme | (7) |
Binding relations | (8) and (9) |
Control relations | (10) and (11) |
Selectional relations | (12), (13), (14) and (15) |
Head movements | (16) and (17) |
Phrase movements | (18) |
Ellipsis patterns | (19) |
First, nominal phrases and clauses are similar in terms of the external distribution. For example, both nominal phrases and clauses can occur as subjects ((6a) and (6b)) or direct objects ((6c) and (6d)) (Abney, 1987; Lees, 1960):
-
(6)
- The prisoner’s murder of a policeman surprised me.
- That the prisoner murdered a policeman surprised me.
- I learned the prisoner’s murder of a policeman.
- I learned that the prisoner murdered a policeman.
Second, nominal phrases have a similar internal structure to clauses in that both can take arguments such as an agent and a theme (Abney, 1987; Lees, 1960).
-
(7)
- Rome’s destruction of the city
- Rome destroyed the city.
Third, nominal phrases and clauses are similar with respect to binding and control relations (Abney, 1987). In (8), John and himself can refer to the same individual, but the co–reference relation between John and him is not possible in (9). The contrast applies identically in both clauses and nominal phrases.
-
(8)
- Johni portrayed himselfi.
- Johni’s portrayal of himselfi
-
(9)
- *Johni portrayed himi.
- *Johni’s portrayal of himi
Moreover, the empty (i.e. unpronounced) pronoun (PRO) in an adjunct can refer to the subject John (10) but not the object Bill (11). The contrast, again, obtains in both clauses and nominal phrases.
-
(10)
- Johni criticized Bill after PROi talking.
- Johni’s criticism of Bill after PROi talking
-
(11)
- *John criticized Billi after PROi talking.
- *John’s criticism of Billi after PROi talking
This indicates that both clauses and nominal phrases establish a domain for binding and control relations and that there is an asymmetric relation between the subject and the object in both clauses and nominal phrases.
Another parallelism is that within both nominal phrases and clauses, there is a selectional relation between the heads of two projections. In sentences, the complementizer that selects an embedded clause that contains a tensed verb (12) and the complementizer/preposition for selects an infinitival complement (13).
-
(12)
- I think [cp that [ip they will go home]].
- *I think [cp that [ip them to go home]].
-
(13)
- It is strange [cp for [ip them to go home]].
- *It is strange [cp for [ip they will go home]].
In nominal phrases, a singular demonstrative selects a singular numeral (14) and a plural demonstrative selects a plural numeral (15):
-
(14)
- [dp this [nump one [np table]]]
- *[dp this [nump two [np tables]]]
-
(15)
- [dp these [nump two [np tables]]]
- *[dp these [nump one [np table]]]
Moreover, similar movement patterns in clauses and nominal phrases also indicate that there must be a structural position in both clauses and nominal phrases to host the moved elements (Ihsane, 2008, chap. 1). The first kind of movement is head movement. One of the well documented examples is exhibited by the difference in word order within clauses in English and French: certain adverbs precede verbs in English but follow verbs in French:
-
(16)
- My friends all love Mary.
-
Mes amis aiment tous Marie.my friends love all Mary‘My friends all love Mary.’
Pollock (1989) suggests that the underlying word order in both languages is that adverbs precede verbs. The deviation of French from this underlying word order is due to movement of the verb to a higher position in the hierarchical structure of the clause. A similar movement is also observed in the nominal phrases of the two languages: for example, adjectives precede nouns in English (17a) but adjectives follow nouns in French (17b).
(17) a. the red flower b. le fleuri rouge ti the flower red ‘the red flower’
One analysis advanced in many studies (e.g. Cinque, 1995) is that in French, the noun may move leftward, bypassing the adjective, in the same way that verbs move across adverbs in the same language.
Additionally, wh phrases in both clauses and nominal phrases may move to their left periphery, which indicates a structural position on the left periphery for moved phrases (Alexiadou, Haegeman, & Stavrou, 2007, Part II, chap. 1).
-
(18)
- [cp [ip I like this book the best]].
- [cp Which book [c′do [ip you like the best]]]?
- This is [dp [d′a [ap very important] decision]].
- [dp [ap How important] [d′a decision]] is this?
In (18b), which book moves to the specifier position of CP and in (18d), the phrase how important moves to the specifier position of DP.
Finally, ellipsis patterns reveal the presence of functional projections in the structures of both clauses and nominal phrases.
-
(19)
- Mary likes swimming but John [ip doesn’t [vp like swimming]].
- My sister’s two boys are wild but [dp John’s [nump two [np boys]]] are really quite well-behaved.
In (19a), the verb phrase in the second conjunct, like swimming, is deleted and according to Lobeck (1995, chap. 2), the existence of a functional category (IP) is crucial for licensing the ellipsis of the verb phrase. Similarly, the ellipsis of the noun phrase in the second conjunct of (19b), boys, must be licensed by a functional category, which is NumP in this case.
2.2.2. Differences between clauses and nominal phrases
Despite the structural parallelisms between clauses and nominal phrases in English illustrated above, there are some differences in terms of functional morphemes between clauses and nominal phrases. The first difference concerns those words that fill the top nodes of nominal phrases and clauses, i.e. DP and CP, respectively. English demonstratives, which are associated with DP, exhibit a difference between a plural form (such as these) and a singular form (such as this), depending on whether the following Number Phrase indicates a singular number or a plural number (Leu, 2008). But in clauses, complementizers, like that, if and whether, do not exhibit inflectional alternations. The second difference concerns the words that fill the intermediate projections in nominal phrases and clauses, i.e. NumP and TP, respectively. Within nominal phrases, numerals, which are located at Number Phrase, do not inflect for number ((20a) and (20b)). Within clauses, however, auxiliaries, which fill the node of Tense Phrase, may inflect for tense ((21a) and (21b)).
-
(20)
- one apple
- two(*s) apples
-
(21)
- The men have finished the work.
- The men had finished the work.
2.3. The predictions
So far, we have illustrated the structural parallelisms and differences in inflectional properties between clauses and nominal phrases in English. The two hypotheses (the TPH and the DMH) predict different impairment patterns on nominal phrases, as compared to the impairment patterns in clauses. The main focus of this paper is to test these predictions in order to tease apart the TPH and the DMH. Referring to the clausal structure in (4) and the nominal structure in (5), let us spell out the impairment patterns in clauses and nominal phrases predicted by these two hypotheses.
The TPH claims that the higher a functional projection is along the hierarchical syntactic structure, the more likely it is to be impaired, and that the impairment of a node implies impairment of all the nodes above it. Moreover, the TPH claims that impairment of any projection will lead to the impairment of whatever is in that projection. Specifically, the TPH generates the following predictions: complementizers, which occur in CP, such as that, if and whether, are more likely to be impaired than auxiliaries (such as have and had), which occur in TP. In the nominal domain, demonstratives, which occur in DP (such as this and these), are more likely to be impaired than numerals, which occur in NumP (such as one and two).
In contrast, the DMH claims that aphasic patients have faulty implementation of feature-to-morpheme mapping rules, which leads to the prediction that aphasic patients will encounter difficulty whenever they have to implement a morphological rule. As far as clauses are concerned, because complementizers in English (like that, if and whether) do not undergo morphological inflection, it is predicted that aphasic patients will perform normally on these morphemes. On the other hand, as had is the past tense inflected form of have, it is predicted that aphasic patients will be impaired on had. Regarding the nominal phrase, if we assume that the plural demonstrative (these) is the inflected form of the singular demonstratives (this) (Leu, 2008), it is predicted that aphasic patients will exhibit impaired performance on these. But the singular demonstrative this and numerals are very likely to be preserved because they do not undergo morphological inflection.
In order to test these predictions, we designed four experiments to test aphasic participants’ performance on four functional categories in clauses and nominal phrases: complementizers, auxiliary verbs, demonstratives and numerals. Moreover, these experiments included two kinds of tasks: sentence completion and grammaticality judgment. We tested patients on these two modalities as there is a debate regarding whether deficits in aphasic patients are the same across modalities (Burchert et al., 2005; Grodzinsky, 1984, 2000a, 2000b; Grodzinsky & Finkel, 1998; Linebarger, Schwartz, & Saffran, 1983; Nanousi et al., 2006). For example, it is reported that patients exhibited similar patterns of impairment in both sentence completion and grammaticality judgment tasks (Varlokosta et al., 2006;Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004) and it is reported in Faroqi-Shah and Dickey (2009) that patients had difficulty with tense inflections in both grammaticality judgment and production (narration and elicited picture description). By including two types of tasks, we hope to address the question of whether grammatical morphology impairment is due to a deficit of the central grammatical representation or due to impaired access to the central representation via a particular modality (Dickey et al., 2008). In the former case, we would expect the same impairment pattern in both sentence completion and grammaticality judgment. In the latter case, we may find different deficit patterns in the two tasks.
3. Sentence completion experiments
3.1. Experiment 1: sentence completion testing complementizers and tense inflections in clauses
This experiment examined patients’ use of complementizers and tense inflections in a task requiring participants to select from a response set a missing word in sentences presented. The TPH predicts that response accuracy (expressed as the percentage of correct responses) will be significantly higher for complementizers than for auxiliary verbs. Conversely, the DMH predicts that response accuracy for complementizers and have will be close to ceiling and that response accuracy for had will be below chance.
3.1.1. Participants
A total of twenty individuals, ten agrammatic aphasic and ten healthy control participants, were included in the experiment. All participants were native monolingual speakers of American English. The aphasic individuals, recruited from the subject pool of the Aphasia and Neurolinguistic Research Laboratory at Northwestern University, presented with a single left hemisphere stroke, with the exception of one patient (DSG), who presented with aphasia secondary to a single right hemisphere stroke; all were between 2 and 15 years post-onset of stroke at the time of the study (Mean age = 55.1; range = 48–75). MR scans (T1 images) were obtained for five participants (see Fig. 1), with lesions involving inferior frontal and surrounding regions, extending to temporoparietal regions and including subcortical white matter. For health reasons, the remaining patients were unable to undergo MR scanning, however, previous medical history revealed that all experienced ischemic middle cerebral artery events. All patients had normal visual and hearing acuity. All except one patient (MD) were premorbidly right-handed. These patients were mildly or moderately impaired according to their scores on the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) (Kertesz, 1982), with WAB Aphasia Quotients ranging from 60.8 to 87.4. Demographic information and WAB scores for the aphasic participants are included in Table 2. Reading scores derived from administration of various tests, presented in Table 3, also showed that all patients could read at least single words.5 The control participants had no prior history of speech-language, learning or neurological disorders (Mean age = 50.2; range = 50–71).6
Table 2.
Demographic information | WAB | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Gender | Handedness | Years of post-onset |
Information content |
Fluency | Comprehension | Repetition | Naming | Aphasia quotient |
|
DSG | 58 | M | R | 6 | 10 | 5 | 10 | 10 | 9.8 | 89.6 |
EAS | 75 | F | R | 3 | 9 | 5 | 9.8 | 8.8 | 8.9 | 83 |
EJ | 41 | M | R | 3 | 10 | 9 | 8.9 | 9 | 9.7 | 93.2 |
JN | 55 | F | R | 7 | 10 | 10 | 10 | 9.8 | 9.2 | 98 |
JP | 51 | F | R | 4 | 8 | 5 | 7.45 | 8.4 | 8.3 | 74.3 |
JY | 48 | M | R | 5 | 9 | 9 | 9.85 | 9.2 | 9.8 | 93.7 |
KC | 35 | M | R | 4 | 9 | 5 | 7.8 | 9.4 | 7.6 | 77.6 |
MD | 64 | M | L | 22 | 9 | 4 | 8.45 | 5.1 | 9.3 | 71.7 |
MK | 67 | M | R | 13 | 8 | 4 | 9.4 | 9 | 8.3 | 77.4 |
OC | 57 | M | R | 6 | 10 | 5 | 7.9 | 7.6 | 8.5 | 78 |
All patients except MD was premorbidly right-handed.
Table 3.
Participant | WAB-R reading commands score (20) |
PALPA subtest 34 | NAVI pre-test reading |
---|---|---|---|
DSG | 19 | 100.00% | 100% |
EAS | NA | NA | 92% |
EJ | NA | NA | 70% |
JN | NA | NA | 83% |
JP | 20 | 98.00% | 100% |
JY | NA | NA | NA |
KC | 19 | 83.33% | 58% |
MD | 17 | 65.60% | 83% |
MK | NA | NA | NA |
OC | 17 | NA | 75% |
PALPA = Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia. Subtest 34 tests ‘Lexical Morphology and Reading’; NAVI Pre-Test Reading examined participants’ ability to read single nouns and verbs as well as active subject-verb-object sentences; NA = not administered.
The aphasic participants were diagnosed with agrammatic aphasia based on production patterns observed in their narration of the Cinderella story as well as scores derived from administration of tests of grammatical morphology production (i.e., the Northwestern Assessment of Verb Inflection (NAVI), Lee & Thompson, 2009, experimental version) and production and comprehension of verbs and sentences (i.e., the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS), Thompson, 2011). Analysis of the narrative data, using a method developed by Thompson et al. (1995) (also see Thomspon, Cho, et al., 2012), indicated that (compared to cognitively healthy controls from Thomspon, Cho, et al., 2012) the aphasic participants evinced reduced mean length of utterance, ranging from 4.8 to 10.39 words (M = 7.5), decreased words per minute, ranging from 26.55 to 85.67 (M = 60.16), and a lowered proportion of grammatically correct sentences, ranging from 22% to 82% (M=65%) (see Table 4). On the NAVI (Table 5), patients showed greater difficulty with finite compared to nonfinite forms, and on the NAVS (Table 6), patients showed spared verb comprehension, but poorer production of three-argument compared to one- and two-argument verbs as well as difficulty producing verb arguments in sentence contexts as tested by the Argument Structure Production Test (ASPT) of the NAVS. In addition, aphasic participants showed better comprehension and production of canonical than noncanonical sentences (see Table 6).
Table 4.
Participant | MLU – Words | WPM | %Grammatically correct sentences |
---|---|---|---|
DSG | 8.07 | 58.19 | 82% |
EAS | 5.91 | 56.68 | 55% |
EJ | 10.08 | 90 | 70% |
JN | 8.78 | 85.67 | 71% |
JP | 6.15 | 26.55 | 74% |
JY | 7.28 | 36.15 | 81% |
KC | 6.06 | 33.06 | 79% |
MD | 4.8 | 73.48 | 22% |
MK | NA | NA | NA |
OC | 10.39 | 81.65 | 48% |
Aphasic mean (SD) | 7.5 (1.96) | 60.16 (24.08) | 65% (0.2) |
Normal mean (SD) | 11.11 (0.56) | 133.22 (5.22) | 93.02% (1.21) |
MLU = Mean length of utterance; WPM = words per minute; vmi = verbal morphology inflection. NA = not administered. The normal mean is from cognitively healthy speakers (n = 13) from Thomspon, Cho, et al. (2012).
Table 5.
Participant | Infinitive | Progressive | Total nonfinite |
Present singular |
Present plural |
Regular past |
Irregular past |
Future | Total finite |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
DSG | 100% | 100% | 100% | 90% | 80% | 80% | 100% | 100% | 90% |
EAS | 100% | 100% | 100% | 90% | 60% | 100% | 60% | 80% | 78% |
EJ | 100% | 80% | 90% | 90% | 20% | 0% | 40% | 0% | 30% |
JN | 100% | 100% | 100% | 90% | 70% | 80% | 100% | 90% | 86% |
JP | 100% | 100% | 100% | 10% | 20% | 60% | 80% | 100% | 54% |
JY | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA |
KC | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 80% | 40% | 20% | 0% | 48% |
MD | 100% | 100% | 100% | 60% | 60% | 40% | 20% | 30% | 42% |
MK | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA |
OC | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 0% | 100% | 100% | 90% | 78% |
Mean (SD) | 100% (0) |
97.5% (0.07) |
98.75% (0.04) |
78.75% (0.30) |
48.75% (0.31) |
62.5% (0.35) |
65% (0.35) |
61.25% (0.44) |
63.25% (0.22) |
NA = not administered.
Table 6.
DSG | EAS | EJ | JN | JP | JY | KC | MD | MK | OC | Mean (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
VNT (1Pl + 2Pl) | 100% | 95% | 90% | 100% | 65% | 100% | 85% | 100% | 95% | 100% | 93% (0.11) |
VNT (3Pl) | 71% | 71% | 57% | 100% | 43% | 71% | 71% | 100% | 86% | 43% | 71.3% (0.2) |
VCT (Total) | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% (0) |
ASPT (Total) | 97% | 94% | 94% | 94% | 88% | 97% | 88% | 84% | 94% | 91% | 92.1% (0.04) |
SPPT Canonical | 100% | 93% | 53% | 100% | 100% | 93% | 53% | 33% | 100% | 93% | 81.8% (0.25) |
SPPT Non-canonical | 100% | 67% | 7% | 100% | 27% | 100% | 20% | 0 | 87% | 60% | 56.8% (0.4) |
SCT Canonical | 100% | 87% | 100% | 100% | 87% | 100% | 53% | 87% | 93% | 40% | 84.7% (0.21) |
SCT Non-canonical | 87% | 73% | 73% | 100% | 53% | 87% | 67% | 80% | 87% | 73% | 78% (0.13) |
VNT = Verb Naming Test; VCT = Verb Comprehension Test; ASPT = Argument Structure Production Test; SPPT = Sentence Production Priming Test; SCT = Sentence Comprehension Test; 1Pl = 1 place verbs; 2Pl = 2 place verbs; 3Pl = 3 place verbs; Sub-wh = subject wh questions; Obj-wh = object wh questions; Sub relatives = subject relative clauses; Obj relatives = object relative clauses.
3.1.2. Stimuli
In this experiment, we constructed fifty-five sentences, with each containing a blank, which could be filled by one of four candidate words presented. To construct these sentences, we selected three complementizers (that, if and whether), five complement taking verbs (ask, care, know, see and wonder) and two auxiliary verbs (have and had).We also selected sixteen verbs and twenty-two nouns (see List 1 in the Appendix). All verbs and nouns were one- or two-syllable high-frequency words, selected from CELEX (Baayen, Pieenbrock, & van Rij, 1993).
The stimulus sentences were distributed across three conditions. Fifteen lacked complementizers (22) with the correct response if required for these sentences. Fifteen contained the temporal adverb today (23) and lacked the auxiliary verb have, therefore the correct response was have. Another fifteen sentences contained the temporal adverb yesterday and lacked the auxiliary verb had (24) so the correct response was had in these sentences. Another ten sentences were filler sentences, which lacked other kinds of words, for example, prepositions (25).
-
(22)
They wonder ___ the man is covering the box. (in, if, too, but)
-
(23)
Today the girls ___ covered the box. (have, in, and, had)
-
(24)
By yesterday the girls ___ covered the box. (have, in, and, had)
-
(25)
The cup is ___ the desk. (this, are, on, that)
In both the embedded clause in (22) and the sentences in (23) and (24), the subjects were animate nouns and the objects were inanimate nouns, so all the sentences were semantically non-reversible.
3.1.3. Procedures
Each stimulus sentence, together with four candidate words, was printed on a separate sheet. Participants read the stimulus sentence and chose a word to complete the sentence. Whenever patients encountered difficulty, experimenters read aloud the sentence and the four candidate words. The response accuracy was calculated as the percent of correct responses for each task. Before the experiment, aphasic patients were tested for comprehension of the words used in the experiment, including the three complementizers, the five complement taking verbs, the two auxiliary verbs, the sixteen verbs and the twenty-two nouns. They were asked to point to the word pronounced by the experimenter. Moreover, using a calendar, they were asked to demonstrate their understanding of yesterday and today. Additionally, participants were provided four practice items prior to administering the experimental sentences to insure their understanding of the task. The experiment stimuli were presented in a pseudo random order and they remained the same across participants.
3.1.4. Results
The response accuracies for the two groups of participants across conditions are shown in Table 7 and Fig. 2. Because the accuracy data were not normally distributed, we performed rationalized arcsine transformation prior to further statistical analysis according to formulas proposed in Studebaker (1985) and Thornton and Raffin (1978).7 The transformed accuracy data were entered into a mixed design ANOVA, with three test conditions (Comp, have and had) as the within subject factor and the two groups (aphasic participants and controls) as the between subject factor. Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2 (2) = 6.596, p = 0.037, therefore degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimate of sphericity (ε = 0.76). There was a significant main effect of test condition, F(1.513, 27.24) = 20.21, p < 0.001 as well as a significant main effect of group, F(1, 18) = 54.13, p < 0.001 and there was also a significant interaction effect for group and test condition, F(1.513, 27.24) = 9.85, p = 0.001. Response accuracy for the condition testing complementizers was significantly higher than for auxiliary verbs, t(1, 9) = 3.47, p = 0.007. In addition, response accuracy for have was significantly higher than for had, t(1, 9) = 8.18, p < 0.001.
Table 7.
Complementizers | have | had | |
---|---|---|---|
DSG | 93.3 | 93.3 | 80 |
EAS | 100 | 93.3 | 86.7 |
EJ | 100 | 93.3 | 86.7 |
JN | 93.3 | 100 | 86.7 |
JP | 93.3 | 100 | 86.7 |
JY | 93.3 | 93.3 | 86.7 |
KC | 93.3 | 93.3 | 80 |
MD | 100 | 93.3 | 80 |
MK | 93.3 | 86.7 | 73.3 |
OC | 100 | 93.3 | 80 |
Mean | 96 | 94 | 82.7 |
Because the stimuli in the three conditions ((22), (23) and (24)) have different lengths, respectively, we also carried out a one-way ANOVA on the accuracy data with the three kinds of sentence length (8 words in (22), 6 words in (23) and 7 words in (24)) as the independent variable. The analysis revealed a significant effect, F(2, 27) = 20.51, p < 0.001. We found a significant difference between the complementizer condition and the had condition (t(1, 9) = 6.7, p < 0.001), plus a significant difference between the have condition and the had condition, t(1, 9) = 8.18, p < 0.001. There was, however, no significant difference between the complementizer condition and the have condition, t(1, 9) = 1.01, p = 0.337. Although sentence length appeared to have a significant effect on accuracy, the significant differences among the three conditions in this experiment are better attributable to the experimental manipulation among complementizers, have and had. As demonstrated in Fig. 2, accuracy for the complementizer condition in (22), which contained the longest stimuli, was the highest while the accuracy for the had condition in (24), which included stimuli of medium length among the three conditions, was the lowest.
3.2. Experiment 2: sentence completion testing demonstratives and numerals in nominal phrases
This experiment examined patients’ use of demonstratives and numerals, employing the same task as in Experiment 1. The TPH predicts that response accuracy will be significantly lower for demonstratives than for numerals. Conversely, the DMH predicts that response accuracy for this and numerals will be close to ceiling and that response accuracy for these will be below chance.
3.2.1. Participants
The same aphasic and healthy individuals participated in this experiment as in Experiment 1.
3.2.2. Stimuli
This experiment was aimed at testing participants’ ability to choose a correct demonstrative or numeral. Each participant saw eighty sentences, with each sentence missing a word. The participants’ task was to choose one from among four candidate words to complete the sentence. To construct these sentences, we selected fifteen verbs and twenty nouns (see List 2 in the Appendix), which were one- or two-syllable high frequency words selected from CELEX (Baayen et al., 1993). We also used two demonstratives (this and these), four numerals and four color adjectives (green, yellow, black and red). All stimulus sentences were semantically non-reversible.
Thirty stimuli lacked a demonstrative, half of them lacking this (26) and half of them lacking these (27).
-
(26)
The man covered ___ car. (this, for, these, on)
-
(27)
The man covered ___ cars. (this, for, these, on)
The two sentences were the same except that the noun in (26) (car) was singular and the noun in (27) (cars) was plural. The correct response for (26) was this and the correct response for (27) was these.
Another thirty of the sentences lacked a numeral. Each sentence in this set was presented with a picture depicting a different number of objects. Some pictures showed only one object (e.g., one shirt) (n = 15), whereas others showed four objects (e.g., four shirts) (n = 15). Sentences as in (28) and (29) were presented followed by four numerals. The task of the participants was to choose a numeral for the sentence based on what was depicted in the picture.
-
(28)
In this picture, we can see ___ shirt(s). (one, two three, four)
-
(29)
In this picture, we can see ___ shirt(s). (one, two three, four)
Additionally, there were twenty fillers that were like the other stimuli except that a word in another position was missing:
-
(30)
The clock is ___ the table. (this, three, these, on)
-
(31)
In this picture, we can see ___ shirt(s). (yellow, black, green, black)
In (31), participants were asked to choose a word matching the color of the object depicted in the picture.
3.2.3. Procedures
Prior to the experiment, patients were tested on their comprehension of the fifteen verbs and twenty nouns used to construct the sentences in this experiment. Moreover, we also tested patients’ understanding of numerals. Patients were presented with pictures that depicted different numbers of objects. Then patients were asked to choose the card that contains the numeral that correctly depicted the number of the objects in the picture. Additionally, patients were also tested on their comprehension of the color adjectives as used in the fillers in (31). As in Experiment 1, the stimuli were presented in a pseudo random order and were the same across participants.
3.2.4. Results
Results of the experiment for the two participant groups are shown in Table 8 and Fig. 3. After being transformed according to Studebaker (1985) and Thornton and Raffin (1978), the response accuracy data were entered into a mixed design ANOVA, with the four conditions as the within-subject factor and the two groups (aphasic participants and controls) as the between-subject factor. There was a significant main effect of test condition (F(3, 54) = 21.24, p < 0.001) and group (F(1, 18) = 25.88, p < 0.001). There also was a significant interaction effect of test condition and group, F(3, 54) = 11.16, p < 0.001. Pairwise comparisons indicated that accuracy for NumP was significantly higher than for DP, t(1, 9) = 5.16, p = 0.001. Among all the words that fill the category of DP, there was a dissociation between the singular demonstrative and the plural demonstrative, as accuracy for this was significantly higher than for these, t(1, 9) = 7.08, p < 0.001.
Table 8.
this | these | Sg | Pl | |
---|---|---|---|---|
DSG | 93.3 | 66.7 | 100 | 100 |
EAS | 100 | 80 | 100 | 100 |
EJ | 100 | 60 | 100 | 100 |
JN | 100 | 80 | 100 | 100 |
JP | 100 | 73.3 | 100 | 100 |
JY | 100 | 80 | 93.3 | 100 |
KC | 80 | 80 | 100 | 100 |
MD | 93.3 | 66.7 | 93.3 | 86.7 |
MK | 93.3 | 73.3 | 100 | 100 |
OC | 93.3 | 73.3 | 93.3 | 93.3 |
Mean | 95.3 | 73.3 | 98 | 98 |
Additionally, we carried out an ANOVA of the accuracy data with the two kinds of sentence length as the independent variable (4 words in (26) and (27); 7 words in (28) and (29)), which revealed a significant effect, F(1, 38) = 14.74, p < 0.001. A pairwise comparison indicated a significant difference between the accuracy for the condition of demonstratives (this and these) and the condition of numerals, t(1, 19) = 4.08, p = 0.001, with the accuracy of the former condition significantly lower than that of the latter condition. This is because, as demonstrated in Fig. 3, the accuracy of the these condition was very low while the accuracy of the other three conditions was close to ceiling.
3.3. Interim summary and discussion
We designed two sentence completion experiments to test the TPH and the DMH regarding aphasic participants’ impairment of functional morphemes in clauses and nominal phrases. Patient’s response accuracy for complementizers was significantly higher than that for auxiliary verbs and this result goes against the predication of the TPH, which predicts that morphemes in CP should be more impaired than those in TP. Meanwhile, response accuracy for have was significantly higher than that for had, and this result is consistent with the prediction of the DMH, which predicts that inflectionally derived forms will be more impaired than forms involving no inflection. In case of nominal phrases, accuracy for the category of DP (i.e. demonstratives) was significantly lower than that for the category of NumP (i.e. numerals), which seems to support the prediction of the TPH. On the other hand, among all the words that fill the category of DP, there was a dissociation between the singular demonstrative this and the plural demonstrative these, as accuracy for the former was significantly higher than that for the latter. This result is not predicted by the TPH, as it predicts that all functional morphemes in DP will be impaired. But this result supports the prediction of the DMH, which predicts that the plural demonstrative will be more impaired than the singular demonstrative. The dissociation between this and these in terms of accuracy may also explain why the accuracy for the category of DP was significantly lower than those for the category of NumP. In a word, in both clauses and nominal phrases, those functional morphemes that involve inflection (e.g. had and these) were more impaired than those that do not (e.g. have, this, complementizers and numerals). This result supports the DMH, instead of the TPH.
4. Grammaticality judgment experiments
4.1. Experiment 3: grammaticality judgment of functional morphemes in clauses
Experiment 3 tested grammaticality judgment of complementizers and tense-inflected auxiliary verbs in clauses. The TPH and the DMH have different predictions about participants’ behavior. The TPH predicts that accuracy for complementizers will be significantly lower than that for auxiliary verbs (including have and had). The DMH makes the prediction that accuracy for complementizers and have will be close to ceiling but accuracy for had will be below chance.
4.1.1. Participants
The patients and non-impaired controls who participated in Experiments 1 and 2 also participated in this experiment.
4.1.2. Stimuli
In this experiment, participants’ task was to judge the grammaticality of 85 sentences that involved complementizers and tense-inflected auxiliary verbs. 25 of the sentences included embedded complement clauses introduced by either a complementizer (32a) or an ungrammatical preposition substitute (32b); 25 included either have in the present perfective tense (33a) or an ungrammatical use of have (33b); 25 involved the use of had in the past perfective tense (34a) or an ungrammatical use of had (34b). The ratio of grammatical to ungrammatical sentences was 3 to 2 in each condition. In addition, there were 10 fillers involving bare verbs (35a, b). Among the fillers, the ratio of grammatical to ungrammatical sentences was 1 to 1.
-
(32)Complementizers
- They see that the man cuts the shirt.
- They see *for the man cuts the shirt.
-
(33)have
- Today the men have cut the shirt.
- By yesterday the men *have cut the shirt.
-
(34)had
- By yesterday the men had cut the shirt.
- Today the men *had cut the shirt.
-
(35)Bare-V
- They saw the man cut the shirt.
- They saw the man *is cut the shirt.
To construct these sentences, we selected three complementizers (that, if and whether), five complement taking verbs (ask, care, know, see and wonder) and two auxiliary verbs (have and had). We also selected fifteen verbs and fifteen nouns (see List 3 in the Appendix) from CELEX (Baayen et al.,1993). All verbs and nouns were one- or two-syllable high-frequency words. The embedded sentences in (32) and all the other sentences were semantically non-reversible as the subjects were animate nouns and the objects were inanimate nouns.
4.1.3. Procedures
The experiment was implemented in Superlab 4.0., which can record participants’ responses. Each sentence was presented visually and while the sentence appeared on the screen, it was read out so that participants could both see and hear the sentence. Immediately after participants heard a sentence stimulus, they were asked to press a response pad button (the red button was labeled grammatical and the blue button was labeled ungrammatical) to indicate whether the sentence was grammatical or not. The stimuli were pseudo-randomized and were the same for all participants.
Before the experiment, patients were familiarized with the nouns and the verbs used in the experiment. Moreover, they were presented with six practice items, three of them read by the experimenter and another three displayed by Superlab 4.0.
4.1.4. Results
Response accuracy data for the two participant groups across conditions are shown in Table 9 and Fig. 4. As in Experiments 1 and 2, the accuracy data, which was computed over both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences, were rationalized-arcsine transformed before they were subjected to a mixed design ANOVA, with the three conditions as the within subject factor and the two groups of participants as the between subject factor. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of test condition, F(2, 36) = 62.76, p < 0.001 and a significant group effect, F(1, 18) = 212.15, p < 0.001, as well as a significant interaction effect of group and condition, F(2, 36) = 48.41, p < 0.001. Pairwise comparisons indicated that the aphasic participants’ accuracy for complementizers was significantly higher than that for auxiliary verbs (including have and had), t(1, 9) = 6.45, p < 0.001. Patients’ accuracy for have was significantly higher than that for had, t(1, 9) = 9.97, p < 0.001. There was no significant difference between the complementizer condition and the have condition, t(1, 9) = 0.36, p = 0.726.
Table 9.
Complementizers | have | had | |
---|---|---|---|
DSG | 88 | 88 | 80 |
EAS | 92 | 92 | 76 |
EJ | 96 | 96 | 80 |
JN | 96 | 92 | 80 |
JP | 96 | 92 | 68 |
JY | 92 | 88 | 76 |
KC | 92 | 92 | 76 |
MD | 92 | 96 | 72 |
MK | 92 | 96 | 84 |
OC | 92 | 92 | 72 |
Mean | 92.8 | 92.4 | 76.4 |
In order to check whether patients exhibited any yes-bias in the judgment experiment, we carried out an ANOVA of the transformed accuracy data with grammaticality as a within subject factor and with the three conditions (complementizers, have and had) as a between-subject factor. We found no significant effect of grammaticality (F(1, 27) = 2.91, p = 0.1), although there was a significant effect of condition (F(2, 27) = 56.32, p < 0.001) and a significant interaction effect (F(2, 27) = 7.17, p = 0. 003). This means that patients did not exhibit a yes-bias to grammatical sentences vs. ungrammatical sentences.
4.2. Experiment 4: grammaticality judgment of functional morphemes in nominal phrases
This experiment examined patients’ performance regarding functional morphemes in nominal clauses. The two hypotheses have different predictions about aphasic patients’ behavior. The TPH predicts that accuracy for both demonstratives (this and these) will be significantly lower than that for numerals (both one and plural numerals). The DMH makes the prediction that accuracy for this, one and plural numerals will be close to ceiling and that accuracy for these will be below chance.
4.2.1. Participants
The aphasic participants and healthy controls who participated in the previous three experiments also participated in Experiment 4.
4.2.2. Stimuli
The stimuli consisted of 25 sentences involving correct and incorrect uses of this (36a, b), 25 sentences involving correct and incorrect uses of these (37a, b), 25 sentences involving correct and incorrect uses of the singular numeral one (38a, b), and 25 sentences involving correct and incorrect uses of plural numerals like four (39a, b). The ratio of grammatical to ungrammatical sentences was 3 to 2 in each condition. To construct these sentences, we selected fourteen verbs and thirteen nouns (as in List 4 of the Appendix), in addition to two demonstratives (this and these) and four numerals (one, two, three and four). All the verbs and nouns were one- or two-syllable high-frequency words, selected from CELEX (Baayen et al., 1993). In all these sentences, the subjects were animate nouns and the objects were inanimate nouns therefore all the sentences were semantically non-reversible.
-
(36)this
- The man chased this car.
- The man chased *this cars.
-
(37)these
- The man chased these cars.
- The man chased *these car.
-
(38)Singular numerals (one)
- The man chased one car.
- The man chased *one cars.
-
(39)Plural numerals
- The man chased four cars.
- The man chased *four car.
In addition, ten fillers in this experiment involved dual nouns that have both a mass reading and a count reading (for example, a turkey vs. some turkey in (40)). Previous studies have demonstrated that aphasic patients cannot distinguish between the mass reading and the count reading of dual nouns in the sentence–picture matching task or in the lexical decision task (Taler, Jarema, & Saumier, 2004). The dual nouns in these fillers were preceded by determiners or quantifiers that trigger either the mass reading or the count reading. Half of the fillers were grammatical and the other half were ungrammatical.
-
(40)Fillers
- They ate a turkey.
- They killed *some turkey.
4.2.3. Procedures
Before the experiment, patients were familiarized with the nouns and the verbs used in the experiment. Stimuli were presented using Superlab 4 in the same way as in Experiment 3. Moreover, they were presented with six practice items, three of them read by the experimenter and another three displayed by Superlab 4. The stimuli were pseudo-randomized and were the same across participants.
4.2.4. Results
Accuracy data computed over both grammatical and ungrammatical sentences are shown in Table 10 and Fig. 5. As in the previous three experiments, all the accuracy data were transformed before they were entered into a mixed design ANOVA, with the four conditions as the within subject factor and the two groups of participants (patients and controls) as the between subject factor. There was a significant main effect of test condition, F(3, 54) = 24.53, p < 0.001, a significant main effect of group, F(1, 18) = 289.34, p < 0.001, and a significant interaction effect, F(3, 54) = 18.97, p < 0.001. Pairwise comparisons of the patients’s accuracy data indicated that accuracy for demonstratives was significantly lower than that for numerals, t(1, 9) = 5.28, p = 0.001. Accuracy for this was significantly higher than that for these, t(1, 9) = 8.36, p < 0.001.
Table 10.
this | these | Sg | Pl | |
---|---|---|---|---|
DSG | 92 | 88 | 92 | 92 |
EAS | 92 | 76 | 92 | 96 |
EJ | 96 | 72 | 96 | 96 |
JN | 92 | 76 | 96 | 88 |
JP | 92 | 72 | 92 | 88 |
JY | 100 | 84 | 92 | 96 |
KC | 92 | 72 | 92 | 92 |
MD | 92 | 72 | 96 | 92 |
MK | 92 | 76 | 96 | 96 |
OC | 92 | 84 | 92 | 92 |
Mean | 93.2 | 77.2 | 93.6 | 92.8 |
Additionally, we carried out an ANOVA of the transformed accuracy data of the patients, with grammaticality as the within-subject factor and the four conditions tested in this experiment as the between-subject factor. Except a significant effect of test condition (F(3, 36) = 29.92, p < 0.001), we did not find any significant effect of grammaticality (F(1, 36) = 1.86, p = 0.181), nor a significant interaction effect (F(3, 36) = 0.479, p = 0.699). This means that patients were not significantly more accurate on grammatical stimuli than on ungrammatical stimuli.
4.3. Interim summary and discussion
Experiment 3 and Experiment 4 tested aphasic and control participants’ ability to judge the grammaticality of functional morphemes in clauses and nominal phrases. Patients had low response accuracy for those functional morphemes that involve inflection, i.e. had and these, but they had high response accuracy for those that do not involve inflection, i.e. have, complementizers, this and numerals. These results can be better captured by the DMH, which predicts that patients are impaired on functional morphemes that involve inflection, instead of the TPH.
5. General discussions and conclusion
In this study, we tested two hypotheses concerning agrammatic aphasic individuals’ impairment of functional morphemes in both nominal phrases and clauses in English. The Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH) predicts that those functional morphemes that reside in a higher functional projection within the hierarchical syntactic structure will be more likely to be impaired than those in a lower functional projection. On the other hand, the Distributed Morphology Hypothesis (DMH) predicts that words that involve inflection are more likely to be impaired than those that do not. Previous studies examining deficits in inflectional morphology and function words have investigated these phenomena in clauses only. Our study compared aphasic participants’ impairment of inflectional morphemes and function words between nominal phrases and clauses because nominal phrases provide a testing ground for further teasing apart these two hypotheses. On one hand, nominal phrases and clauses in English are structurally parallel to each other and on the other hand, functional morphemes in nominal phrases exhibit different inflection patterns from clauses. Therefore, the two hypotheses predict different impairment patterns in nominal phrases as compared to those in clauses. The results of the experiments in our study reported similar impairment patterns between clauses and nominal phrases among aphasic individuals. As far as clauses are concerned, both the functional morphemes in the CP node (i.e. complementizers) and the morpheme have, which is located in TP, were preserved, but the morpheme had, which is also in TP, was impaired. In the case of nominal phrases, only the plural demonstrative (i.e. these) was impaired but other elements (i.e. the singular demonstrative this and numerals) were not impaired. In other words, in both clauses and nominal phrases, only those functional morphemes that involve inflection were impaired. This finding does not support the TPH, which predicts that the functional morphemes located in higher syntactic nodes (e.g. CP and DP) should be more likely to be impaired than those located in lower syntactic nodes (e.g. TP and NumP). Rather, the results of our experiments are more in line with the DMH, which predicts that patients have difficulty in converting phonological features into morphemes.
Building on the assumption in theoretical linguistics that nominal phrases and clauses show strong structural parallelisms, this study reveals that aphasic speakers have similar impairment patterns on functional morphology in clauses and nominal phrases. To our knowledge, only a few studies in the aphasia literature have examined impairment patterns across clausal and nominal domains (Rausch, Burchert, & De Bleser, 2005, 2007; Roeper, Ramos, Seymour, & Abdul-Karim, 2001). Interestingly, the results of these studies support our findings. For example, Rausch et al. (2005, 2007) found parallel impairments in both clauses and nominal phrases among aphasic patients. They reported that agrammatic individuals had difficulty with non-canonical word orders in clauses and nominal phrases, which arose in both production and comprehension tasks. In one study, Rausch et al. (2005) tested aphasic production of the following constructions in German:
(41) a. Maria beschreibt Peter Mary describes Peter b. *Maria Peter beschreibt Mary Peter describes c. dass Maria Peter beschreibt that Mary Peter describes d. *… dass Maria beschreibt Peter that Mary describes Peter (42) a. [prepn Marias] [n Beschreibung] Mary’s description b. *[n Beschreibung] [postpn Marias] description Mary’s c. dieDet [n Beschreibung] [postpn Marias] the description Mary’s d. *dieDet [prepn Marias] [n Beschreibung] the Mary’s description.
In matrix clauses, finite verbs are in the second position, following a constituent occupying the first position (41a–b), but in embedded clauses, finite verbs are at the end of the clause (41c–d). According to Rausch et al. (2005), the second position of finite verbs in matrix clauses is derived through movement. In nominal phrases, while the proper name obligatorily moves to the prenominal position when the determiner is absent (42a–b), it has to stay in the postnominal position in the presence of a determiner (42c–d). Their production experiment indicated that aphasic had difficulty producing (41a) and (42a), but not (41c) and (42c).
In another study, Rausch et al. (2007) tested aphasic individuals’ comprehension of the following constructions:
(43) a. Verbal active [cp Der JungeAgent verhaftet Herrn MüllerPatient] the boyAgent detains Mister MillerPatient ‘The boy is detaining Mister Miller.’ b. Verbal passive [cp Herrn MüllerPatient wird von dem JungenAgent verhaftet] Mister MillerPatient is by the boyAgent detained. ‘Mister Miller is being detained by the boy.’ c. Nominal ‘active’ [dp PetersAgent Verhaftung des MannesPatient] Peter’sAgent detention the man’sPatient ‘…Peter’s detention of the man’ d. Nominal ‘passive’ [dp Herrn MüllersPatient Verhaftung durch den JungenAgent] Mister Miller’sPatient detention by the boyAgent ‘…Mister Miller’s detention by the boy’
The construction in (43a) was an active sentence, which had the canonical agent-verb-patient order. The construction in (43b) was the passive counterpart of (43a), and therefore involved a non-canonical word order. In a similar way, the construction in (43c) had the agent-nominalized verb-patient word order, and the construction in (43d) was the passive counterpart of (43c) and involved a non-canonical word order. Their study found that agrammatic patients had more difficulty comprehending constructions involving non-canonical word orders ((43b) and (43d)) than they did comprehending constructions involving canonical word orders ((43a) and (43c)). In a word, the studies by Rausch et al. (2005, 2007) indicated parallel impairment of non-canonical word orders in both clauses and nominal phrases. The present study, which compares agrammatic patients’ impairment patterns between clauses and nominal phrases, indicates that agrammatic individuals’ morphological impairments manifest similarly in clauses and nominal phrases. These findings shed light on linguistic debates regarding structural parallelisms between clauses and nominal phrases, providing further evidence supporting this parallel.
One of our findings that require further discussion is that not all elements in TP were equally impaired. That is, our results showed that although both have and had are in TP, had was impaired but have was not. This finding goes against the hypotheses proposed in some previous studies, which predict that all functional morphemes associated with tense will be impaired. For example, Wenzlaff and Clahsen (2004, 2005), based on the finding that German agrammatic speakers were impaired in tense marking in production and grammaticality judgment tasks, propose that tense features are especially prone to underspecification in agrammatism. Another explanation is that interpretable features (i.e. tense) are more likely to be impaired than uninterpretable features (agreement) (Burchert et al., 2005; Nanousi et al., 2006). A third hypothesis is that the process of diacritic encoding and retrieval (DER) is impaired among agrammatic patients, therefore, patients have difficulty in encoding tense features (+PAST or +PRESENT) in appropriate inflected verb forms and retrieving them (Faroqi-Shah & Dickey, 2009; Faroqi-Shah & Thompson, 2007). However, all the three accounts predict that tense markings will be impaired across the board, which was not borne out in our study. Instead, our study shows that patients had difficulty with had but not with have. This result can be captured by the DMH, which claims that patients have difficulty implementing morphological rules. Meanwhile, the more severe impairment of had than have is consistent with a result reported in previous studies (Bastiaanse, 2008; Bastiaanse et al., 2011; Duman & Bastiaanse, 2009; Lee et al., 2008) that agrammatic speakers had difficulty with past tense forms. Bastiaanse (2008) and Bastiaanse et al. (2011) claimed that reference to the past involves establishing a link between the speech time and the event time, which operates on discourse, rather than on narrow syntax only (Avrutin, 2006). The results of the experiments in this study indicate that patients had more difficulty with had than with have in both sentence completion and grammaticality judgment tasks. These results are consistent with the explanation in Bastiaanse (2008) and Bastiaanse et al. (2011).
Moreover, the results of our experiments indicate that in terms of response accuracy, our aphasic participants evinced similar performance patterns in both sentence completion and grammaticality judgment tasks. The aphasic participants had very low response accuracy for the stimuli with inflection, but high response accuracy for those stimuli without. This result is consistent with the claim that functional impairments in agrammatic patients reflect a general syntactic impairment, which is not modality-specific (Clahsen & Ali, 2009; Faroqi-Shah & Dickey, 2009; Nanousi et al., 2006; Varlokosta et al., 2006; Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004, 2005).
As a final point of discussion, we briefly address the neural substrate of morpho-syntactic processing. The basic finding of this study is that our aphasic patients evinced difficulty with functional morphemes involving inflection, suggesting perhaps that regions of the brain damaged in our patients are necessary for morpho-syntactic/inflectional processing. Because of the heterogeneity of lesions in our patients as well as limitations regarding lesion–deficit correlation studies, however, it is not possible to determine which of the damaged regions are required for this purpose. Furthermore, it is interesting to note that neuroimaging research examining morpho-syntactic processing has resulted in mixed findings. Some studies have shown left inferior frontal activation (i.e. Brodmann’s areas (BA) 44 and 45) for affixation as well as complex syntactic computation (Grodzinsky, 2000a; Hagoort, 2005; Indefrey, Hagoort, Herzog, Seitz, & Brown, 2001; Sahin, Pinker, Cash, Schomer, & Halgren, 2009; Tyler, Bright, Fletcher, & Stamatakis, 2004; Ullman et al., 2005; also see Ullman, 2001; for review), whereas, others have found more distributed activation, involving not only frontal regions, but also posterior perisylvian tissue (Friederici, Meyer, & von Cramon, 2000; Kielar, Milman, Bonakdarpour, & Thompson, 2011; Thompson, Bonakdarpour, & Fix, 2010; Thompson, den Ouden, Bonakdarpour, Garibaldi, & Parrish, 2010, and many others). In addition, the neurocomputation of regularly versus irregularly inflected forms has been debated, with some espousing that regular inflection processes (for verb tense) engages frontal regions, whereas, irregular inflection involves the posterior temporal region of the brain (Beretta et al., 2003; Dhond, Marinkovic, Dale, Witzel, & Halgren, 2003; Jaeger et al., 1996; Ullman et al., 2005). Interestingly, in the present experiments, our participants showed impaired performance of irregular forms (e.g., had and these) and these same participants also showed impairment on regular verb inflection (as tested by the NAVI). Hence, the neural mechanisms of morpho-syntactic processing remain unclear.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Mejrima Cosic, Ted Jenkins, Jim Kloet, Sarah Dove, Sladjana Lukic and Jennifer Mack for their assistance with this project. We would also thank all the aphasic individuals who participated in this project and their family members. This study was supported by the grant DC01948-19 to CKT.
Appendix. Lists of verbs and nouns in the experiments
- List of verbs and nouns used in Experiment 1
Verbs (n = 16) Nouns (n = 22) bite bat chase bell cover book cut box fix broom follow cake hug car kick desk kiss door pat drum paint flag pull kite save lamp watch lock weigh nail wrap pen ring sock spoon shirt train truck - List of verbs and nouns used in Experiment 2
Verbs (n = 15) Nouns (n = 20) bite bell chase book cover box cut broom fix cake follow car hug desk kick door kiss drum paint flag pull key see kite watch lock weigh nail wrap ring shirt skirt spoon train truck - List of verbs and nouns used in Experiment 3
Verbs (n = 15) Nouns (n = 15) bite baby chase book cover box cut car fix cat follow desk hug door kiss girl kick kite paint lamp pull mountain save movie watch plate weigh shirt wrap truck - List of verbs and nouns used in Experiment 4
Verbs (n = 15) Nouns (n = 15) chase bat cover box cut cake fix car follow child hug desk kiss door kick flag paint kite pull lock save nail watch ring weigh shirt wrap
Footnotes
The TPH focuses on the selective impairment of functional morphemes, therefore, it does not address whether the node of VP may be impaired.
Under the structure in (4), we assume that auxiliary verbs like have and had move to TP to check the relevant tense feature (Lasnik, 1995, 1999; Lasnik, Depiante, & Stepanov, 2000, chap. 3; Omaki, 2007).
The specific formulation of the TPH by Friedmann and Grodzinsky (1997) assumes AgrP in the clausal structure. But AgrP is not assumed in the recent minimalist framework (Chomsky, 1995, chap. 4, 2001), and the existence of the Agr node is not crucial for our current discussion of the parallelisms between clauses and nominal phrases. As a result, the structure in (4) does not include AgrP.
We assume with Abney (1987) that demonstratives occupy the head of DP based on the evidence that demonstratives and articles in English do not co-occur.
No reading scores were available for two participants (JY and MK), however, all demonstrated ability to read single words and phrases.
A paired T-test indicated that there was no significant difference between the patients and the normal controls in terms of their age.
Thanks for the anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.
Contributor Information
Honglei Wang, Email: hongleiwang@buaa.edu.cn.
Masaya Yoshida, Email: m-yoshida@northwestern.edu.
Cynthia K. Thompson, Email: ckthom@northwestern.edu.
References
- Abney SP. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. [Google Scholar]
- Alexiadou A, Haegeman L, Stavrou M. Noun phrases in the generative perspective. Berlin/New York: Mounton de Gruyter; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Arabatzi M, Edwards S. Tense and syntactic processes in agrammatic speech. Brain and Language. 2002;80:314–327. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avrutin S. Linguistics and agrammatism. GLOT International. 2001;5:87–97. [Google Scholar]
- Avrutin S. Weak syntax. In: Grodzinsky Y, Amunts K, editors. Broca’s region. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 49–62. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen RH, Pieenbrock R, van Rij H. The CELEX lexical database (release 1) Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Bastiaanse R. Production of verbs in base position by Dutch agrammatic speakers: inflection versus finiteness. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2008;21:104–119. [Google Scholar]
- Bastiaanse R, Bamyaci E, Hsu C-J, Lee J, Duman TY, Thompson CK. Time reference in agrammatic aphasia: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2011;24:652–673. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastiaanse R, Thompson CK. Verb and auxiliary movement in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language. 2003;84:286–305. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00553-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benedet MJ, Christiansen JA, Goodglass H. A cross-linguistic study of grammatical morphology in Spanish- and English-speaking agrammatic patients. Cortex. 1998;34:309–336. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70758-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beretta A, Campbell C, Carr TH, Huang J, Schmitt LM, Christianson K, et al. An ER-fMRI investigation of morphological inflection in German reveals that the brain makes a distinction between regular and irregular forms. Brain and Language. 2003;85:67–92. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00560-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berndt RS, Caramazza A. A redefinition of the syndrome of Broca’s aphasia: implications of a neuropsychological model of language. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1980;1:225–278. [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein JB. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. CUNY; 1993. Topics in the syntax of the nominal structure across romance. [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein JB. The DP hypothesis: identifying clausal properties in the nominal domain. In: Baltin M, Collins C, editors. The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory. Blackwell; 2003. pp. 536–561. [Google Scholar]
- Brame M. The general truth of binding and fusion. Linguistic Analysis. 1981;7:277–325. [Google Scholar]
- Brame M. The head-selector theory of lexical specifications and the nonexistence of coarse categories. Linguistic Analysis. 1982;10:321–325. [Google Scholar]
- Burchert F, Swoboda-Moll M, De Bleser R. Tense and agreement dissociations in German agrammatic speakers: underspecification vs. hierarchy. Brain and Language. 2005;94:188–199. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caramazza A, Zurif EB. Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language. 1976;3:572–582. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(76)90048-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. A minimalist program for linguistic theory. MIT Ocassional Papers in Linguistics. 1992;1:1–71. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. The minimalist program. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. Derivation by phase. In: Kenstowicz MJ, editor. Ken Hale: A life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2001. pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
- Cinque G. On the evidence for partial N-movement in the Romance DP. In: Cinque G, Koster J, Pollock J-Y, Rizzi L, Zanuttini R, editors. Paths Towards Universal Grammar: Studies in Honor of Richard S. Kayne. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press; 1995. pp. 85–110. [Google Scholar]
- Clahsen H, Ali M. Formal features in aphasia: tense, agreement, and mood in English agrammatism. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2009;22:436–450. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR. Aphasia. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1992;326:531–539. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199202203260806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhond RP, Marinkovic K, Dale AM, Witzel T, Halgren E. Spatiotemporal maps of past-tense verb inflection. NeuroImage. 2003;19:91–100. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00047-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickey MW, Milman LH, Thompson CK. Perception of functional morphology in agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language. 2005;95:82–83. [Google Scholar]
- Dickey MW, Milman LH, Thompson CK. Judgment of functional morphology in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2008;21:35–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duman TY, Bastiaanse R. Time reference through verb inflection in Turkish agrammatic aphasia. Brain and Language. 2009;108:30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Embick D, Noyer R. Distributed morphology and the syntax/morphology interface. In: Ramchand G, Reiss C, editors. The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 289–324. [Google Scholar]
- Faroqi-Shah Y, Dickey MW. On-line processing of tense and temporality in agrammatic aphasia. Brain and Language. 2009;108:97–111. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faroqi-Shah Y, Thompson CK. Verb inflections in agrammatic aphasia: encoding of tense features. Journal of Memory and Language. 2007;56:129–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD, Meyer M, von Cramon DY. Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and Language. 2000;74:289–300. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Tel Aviv University; 1998. Functional categories in agrammatic production: A cross-linguistic study. [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N. Agrammatism and the psychological reality of the syntactic tree. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2001;30:71–90. doi: 10.1023/a:1005256224207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N. Question production in agrammatism: the tree pruning hypothesis. Brain and Language. 2002;80:160–187. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N. Speech production in Broca’s agrammatic aphasia: syntactic tree pruning. In: Grodzinsky Y, Amunts K, editors. Broca’s region. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 63–82. [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N, Grodzinsky Y. Tense and agreement in agrammatic production: pruning the syntactic tree. Brain and Language. 1997;56:397–425. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedmann N, Grodzinsky Y. Split inflection in neurolinguistics. In: Friedemann M-A, Rizzi L, editors. The acquisition of syntax: Studies in comparative developmental linguistics. Geneva: Longman; 2000. pp. 84–104. [Google Scholar]
- Gavarró A, Martínez-Ferreiro S. Tense and agreement impairment in Ibero-Romance. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2007;36:25–46. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9031-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grodzinsky Y. The syntactic characterization of agrammatism. Cognition. 1984;16:99–120. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(84)90001-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grodzinsky Y. Theoretical perspectives on language deficits. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Grodzinsky Y. The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2000a;23:1–71. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00002399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grodzinsky Y. Overarching agrammatism. In: Grodzinsky Y, Shapiro L, Swinney D, editors. Language and the brain: Representation and processing – studies presented to Edgar Zurif on his 60th birthday. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000b. pp. 73–86. [Google Scholar]
- Grodzinsky Y, Finkel L. The neurology of empty categories: aphasics’ failure to detect ungrammaticality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1998;10:281–292. doi: 10.1162/089892998562708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagiwara H. The breakdown of functional categories and the economy of derivation. Brain and Language. 1995;50:92–116. doi: 10.1006/brln.1995.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P. On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:416–423. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halle M, Marantz A. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In: Hale K, Keyser SJ, editors. The view from building. Vol. 20. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1993. pp. 111–176. [Google Scholar]
- Harley H, Noyer R. State-of-the-article: distributed morphology. GLOT International. 1999;4:3–9. [Google Scholar]
- Horrocks G, Stavrou M. Bounding theory and Greek syntax: evidence for wh-movement in NP. Journal of Linguistics. 1987;23:79–108. [Google Scholar]
- Ihsane T. The layered DP: Form and meaning of French indefinites. John Benjamins; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Indefrey P, Hagoort P, Herzog H, Seitz RJ, Brown CM. Syntactic processing in left prefrontal cortex is independent of lexical meaning. NeuroImage. 2001;14:546–555. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger JJ, Lockwood AH, Kemmerer DL, Van Valin RDJ, Murphy BW, Khalak HG. A positron emission tomographic study of regular and irregular verb morphology in English. Language. 1996;72:451–497. [Google Scholar]
- Kertesz A. Western aphasia battery. New York: Grune and Stratton; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Kielar A, Milman L, Bonakdarpour B, Thompson CK. Neural correlates of covert and overt production of tense and agreement morphology: evidence from fMRI. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2011;24:183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolk HHJ. Canonicity and inflection in agrammatic sentence production. Brain and Language. 2000;4:558–560. [Google Scholar]
- Lasnik H. Verbal morphology: syntactic structures meets the minimalist program. In: Campos H, Kempchinsky P, editors. Evolution and revolution in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Carlos Otero. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press; 1995. pp. 251–275. [Google Scholar]
- Lasnik H. Verbal morphology: syntactic structures meets the minimalist program. In: Lasnik H, editor. Minimalist analysis. Malden: Blackwell; 1999. pp. 97–119. [Google Scholar]
- Lasnik H, Depiante M, Stepanov A. Syntactic structures revisited: Contemporary lectures on classic transformational theory. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Milman LH, Thompson CK. Functional category production in agrammatic speech. Brain and Language. 2005;95:123–124. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Milman LH, Thompson CK. Functional category production in English agrammatism. Aphasiology. 2008;22:893–905. doi: 10.1080/02687030701865670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lees RB. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT; 1960. The grammar of English nominalizations. [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Thompson CK. Northwestern assessment of verb inflection (experimental version) Evanston, IL: Northwestern University; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Leu T. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. New York University: 2008. The internal syntax of determiners. [Google Scholar]
- Linebarger MC, Schwartz MF, Saffran EM. Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition. 1983;13:361–392. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90015-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobeck A. Ellipsis: Functional heads, licensing, and identification. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall JC. The description and interpretation of aphasic language disorder. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:5–24. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90040-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nanousi V, Masterson J, Druks J, Atkinson M. Interpretable vs. uninterpretable features: evidence from six Greek-speaking agrammatic patients. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2006;19:209–238. [Google Scholar]
- Omaki A. Revisiting revived syntactic structures: the extended hybrid approach to verbal morphology. In: Omaki A, Ortega-Santos I, Sprouse J, Wagers M, editors. University of Maryland working papers in linguistics. Vol. 16. College Park, MD: UMWPiL; 2007. pp. 89–110. [Google Scholar]
- Parisi D, Pizzamiglio L. Syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cortex. 1970;6:204–215. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(70)80028-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollock J-Y. Verb movement, universal grammar, and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry. 1989;20:365–424. [Google Scholar]
- Rausch P, Burchert F, De Bleser R. Parallels in the breakdown of CP and DP-internal movement processes in agrammatism: a preliminary case study. Brain and Language. 2005;95:129–130. [Google Scholar]
- Rausch P, Burchert F, De Bleser R. Comprehension of canonical and non-canonical structures within and across the verbal and nominal syntax domains in agrammatism. Brain and Language. 2007;103:82–83. [Google Scholar]
- Ritter E. Two functional categories in noun phrases: evidence from modern Hebrew. In: Rothstein S, Anderson SR, editors. Syntax and semantics: Perspectives on phrase structure. Vol. 25. New York: Academic Press; 1991. pp. 37–62. [Google Scholar]
- Roeper T, Ramos E, Seymour H, Abdul-Karim L. Language disorders as a window on universal grammar: an abstract theory of agreement for IP, DP, and V-PP. Brain and Language. 2001;77:378–397. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahin NT, Pinker S, Cash SS, Schomer D, Halgren E. Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area. Science. 2009;326:445–449. doi: 10.1126/science.1174481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stowell T. Determiners in NP and DP. In: Leffel K, Bouchard D, editors. Views on phrase structure. Springer: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 1991. pp. 37–56. [Google Scholar]
- Studebaker GA. A “rationalized” arcsine transform. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1985;28:455–462. doi: 10.1044/jshr.2803.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szabolcsi A. The possessor that ran away from home. The Linguistic Review. 1983;3:89–102. [Google Scholar]
- Szabolcsi A. The noun phrase. In: Kiefer F, Kiss KF, editors. Syntax and semantics 27: The syntactic structure of Hungarian. New York: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 179–274. [Google Scholar]
- Taler V, Jarema G, Saumier D. Processing of mass/count information in semantic dementia and agrammatic aphasia. Brain and Language. 2004;91:160–161. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK. Northwestern assessment of verbs and sentences. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Bonakdarpour B, Fix SF. Neural mechanisms of argument structure processing in agrammatic aphasia and healthy age-matched listeners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:1993–2011. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Cho S, Hsu C-J, Wieneke C, Rademaker A, Weitner BB, et al. Dissociations between fluency and agrammatism in primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 2012;26:20–43. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.584691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, den Ouden D-B, Bonakdarpour B, Garibaldi K, Parrish TB. Neural plasticity and treatment-induced recovery of sentence processing in agrammatism. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3211–3227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Fix S, Gitelman D. Selective impairment of morphosyntactic production in a neurological patient. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2002;15:189–207. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(01)00038-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Kielar A, Fix S. Morphological aspects of agrammatic aphasia. In: Thompson CK, Bastiaanse R, editors. Perspectives on agrammatism. New York: Psychology Press; 2012. pp. 109–158. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Shapiro LP, Tait ME, Jacobs BJ, Schneider S, Ballard K. A system for the linguistic analysis of agrammatic language production. Brain and Language. 1995;51:124–129. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton AR, Raffin MJM. Speech-discrimination scores modeled as a binomial variable. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1978;21:507–518. doi: 10.1044/jshr.2103.507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyler LK, Bright P, Fletcher P, Stamatakis EA. Neural processing of nouns and verbs: the role of inflectional morphology. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:512–523. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman MT. A neurocognitive perspective on language: the declarative procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:717–726. doi: 10.1038/35094573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullmana MT, Panchevaa R, Love T, Yee E, Swinney D, Hickok G. Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: evidence from the production, reading, and judgment of inflection in aphasia. Brain and Language. 2005;93:185–238. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varlokosta S, Valeonti N, Kakavoulia M, Lazaridou M, Economou A, Protopapas A. The breakdown of functional categories in Greek aphasia: evidence from agreement, tense, and aspect. Aphasiology. 2006;20:723–743. [Google Scholar]
- Wenzlaff M, Clahsen H. Tense and agreement in German agrammatism. Brain and Language. 2004;89:57–68. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00298-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenzlaff M, Clahsen H. Finiteness and verb-second in German agrammatism. Brain and Language. 2005;92:33–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]