Abstract
Rhythms, or patterns in time, play a vital role in both speech and music. Proficiency in a number of rhythm skills has been linked to language ability, suggesting that certain rhythmic processes in music and language rely on overlapping resources. However, a lack of understanding about how rhythm skills relate to each other has impeded progress in understanding how language relies on rhythm processing. In particular, it is unknown whether all rhythm skills are linked together, forming a single broad rhythmic competence, or whether there are multiple dissociable rhythm skills. We hypothesized that beat tapping and rhythm memory/sequencing form two separate clusters of rhythm skills. This hypothesis was tested with a battery of two beat tapping and two rhythm memory tests. Here we show that tapping to a metronome and the ability to adjust to a changing tempo while tapping to a metronome are related skills. The ability to remember rhythms and to drum along to repeating rhythmic sequences are also related. However, we found no relationship between beat tapping skills and rhythm memory skills. Thus, beat tapping and rhythm memory are dissociable rhythmic aptitudes. This discovery may inform future research disambiguating how distinct rhythm competencies track with specific language functions.
Introduction
Rhythm is an integral part of our daily lives. Across cultures, music uses durations of sounds and silences to form repetitive rhythmic patterns [1]. Musicians, dancers, and listeners use rhythm to perform, synchronize to, and remember music. Similarly, speech contains rhythmic patterns on a number of time scales, from the slowing that occurs at the ends of phrases and sentences [2–5] to the lengthened durations that indicate stress in English [6]. This rhythmic information can help listeners pick out the component sounds of speech by facilitating detection of word boundaries [7–11] and stress patterns [7].
Rhythmic skill, therefore, may not only determine one’s success as a musician or dancer, but may also have wider consequences for language. Performance on a variety of rhythm skills has been linked to linguistic abilities. For example, rhythm discrimination tracks with verbal working memory [12–13] and grammatical skill [14]. Hausen et al. [15] reported that the ability to detect whether a sound has shifted off-beat correlates with word stress perception. Performance on several different rhythm tests tracks with reading ability, including discriminating rhythms [13,16–17], remembering and repeating back rhythms [18–20], synchronizing to a metronome [20–24], tempo reproduction [20,25], tapping to the beat of music [26], and perception of musical meter [27].
There is growing evidence, therefore, that language skills rely on rhythmic abilities that are not domain-specific and can therefore be assessed using non-linguistic stimuli. Consequently, rhythmic deficits may underlie delayed or disrupted language development in some children, which, if true, could lead to targeted assessment and rehabilitation. However, in order to successfully untangle the links between rhythm and language, it is necessary to understand rhythmic skill itself, and at present there is little agreement as to what are the fundamental rhythmic abilities involved in perceiving and producing speech or music. One possibility is that there is a single rhythmic competency underlying all of the rhythm skills mentioned above. If this were true, then perhaps a single rhythmic task might suffice for measuring a participant’s rhythmic skill, an assumption that underlies the use of tests of rhythmic aptitude which include only a single rhythm measure [12,28–29]. Supporting this idea, multiple studies have shown correlations between tests of a variety of rhythm skills. For example, Fujii and Schlaug [30] found weak-to-moderate correlations between performance on tests of tapping to the beat of music, metrical discrimination, and tracking tempo changes. Fitch and Rosenfeld [31] found that the ability to tap to the beat of a rhythm correlated with rhythm reproduction and rhythm discrimination. Pecenka and Keller [32] found that the ability to predict progressive tempo changes during beat tapping predicted accuracy when synchronizing with a partner. Finally, Keele et al. [33] found that motor timing variability correlated with perceptual timing accuracy, suggesting that rhythm perception and production form a single skill set.
An alternate hypothesis is that there are multiple rhythmic ‘intelligences’. Several lines of evidence converge to suggest that rhythm is best characterized as a multi-dimensional skill set. First, the brain networks underlying perception of sub- versus supra-second intervals are dissociable, with sub-second intervals linked to the cerebellum and frontal operculum and supra-second intervals linked to premotor and prefrontal cortex [34–35]. This suggests that rhythm skills may be dissociable based on whether they rely on perception of patterns in time at sub-second versus supra-second rates. Second, perception of metrically simple rhythms is linked to increased activation in subcortical and cortical motor structures such as the basal ganglia and supplementary motor area [36], suggesting that metrical rhythm perception may be dissociable from non-metrical rhythm perception. Finally, evidence from case-studies suggests that beat tapping and rhythm sequence perception are dissociable: problems with beat-tapping can coincide with preserved rhythm discrimination [37–40], and vice versa [41]. To date, however, evidence for this dissociation between beat tapping and rhythm sequence perception has come from isolated case studies rather than by examining performance across rhythm tests in the general population.
Here we set out to test the hypothesis that there are multiple dissociable rhythm skills by assessing several skills in individual participants, then examining relationships between performance on these tests. We predicted that performance on tests of metronome tapping and tempo adaptation would correlate, as beat-tapping relies upon the ability to use auditory feedback to correct for transient fluctuations in the timing relationship between motor output and auditory input [42–43]. We also predicted that performance on tests which require remembering and reproducing a temporal sequence would correlate, as both of these tests require subjects to hear a pattern of durations, remember that pattern, and produce a motor sequence that will reproduce the pattern. However, given the evidence from case studies that rhythmic skills may not be a unitary phenomenon, we hypothesized that beat-tapping and rhythm memory would not be linked. To test these hypotheses we assessed participants on a battery of four rhythm tests that align themselves in two dimensions: 1) beat-tapping: tapping to a metronome and rapid adaptation to a change in metronome tempo, and 2) memory/sequencing: drumming along to a repeating temporal sequence and reproducing a temporal sequence from memory.
Methods
Participants
67 participants were recruited (33 female) with a mean age of 17.99 years (standard deviation 1.02). All had normal hearing (pure tone thresholds < 20 dB for octaves between 250 and 8000 Hz) and no history of a neurological or language disorder. Informed written consent was obtained from participants over the age of 18. For participants under the age of 18 informed written assent was obtained, and written consent was obtained from the participants’ legal guardians. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University.
Rhythm testing
All four rhythm tests used the same system for stimulus presentation, collection of drumming data, and marking of stimulus and drum onset times. Stimuli were stored and presented on an iPod Nano (Apple) and participants were asked to drum with one hand on a conga drum. A vibration-sensitive drum trigger pressed against the underside of the head of the drum picked up the timing of the participant’s drum hits. A copy of the audio signal presented to participants via headphones and the output of the drum trigger were combined as two channels of a stereo input to a computer running the audio recording program Audacity. Thus, the participant’s drumming and the stimulus to which the participant was listening were simultaneously recorded to a single sound file.
Continuous stimulus and drum data were converted to a list of onsets by a custom-written Matlab program. This program marked drum onsets at any point in the stimulus or drum signal which satisfied two preconditions: 1) the amplitude at that point exceeded a preset amplitude threshold, and 2) the point was preceded by a length of time equal to or greater than a preset time threshold during which no point exceeded the amplitude threshold. This “refractory period” ensures that multiple adjacent high-amplitude time points resulting from a single drum hit are not marked as several drum hits. Because the exact manner in which the drum is hit can vary among participants, the marked onsets were visually compared to the raw continuous data and any errors were corrected by manually adjusting the time and amplitude thresholds. Amplitude thresholds were set to be as small as possible without resulting in false alarms, i.e. background noise being marked as drum onsets. This ensured that individual differences in the amplitude of drum hits did not affect the reliability with which drum hit onset times could be marked. These stimulus and drum onsets were then subjected to further analyses for each rhythm test, as described below.
Beat-tapping: Metronome
This test assessed participants’ ability to drum consistently along to an isochronously presented stimulus. Six trials were presented. Each trial consisted of a snare drum stimulus (duration 99 ms, acquired at freesound.org) repeated 40 times with a constant inter-onset-interval (IOI). In two of these trials the IOI was 667 ms (1.5 Hz presentation rate), in two the IOI was 500 ms (2 Hz), and in two the IOI was 333 ms (3 Hz). These trials were always presented in this order. Participants were asked to drum along to the metronome such that their drum hits occurred at the exact same time as the sounds they were hearing. To give the participant ample time to begin synchronizing to the beat, only the last twenty beats of each trial were analyzed. The consistency of each participant’s drumming was assessed by calculating the standard deviation of the IOI of the drum hits within each trial and dividing by the IOI of the stimulus for that trial, then averaging across all six trials.
Beat-tapping: Tempo adaptation
This test assessed how quickly participants could adapt to a change in tempo. Fifty-five trials were presented. Each trial consisted of a conga drum sound (duration 150 ms, acquired at freesound.org) repeated isochronously between 6 and 10 times with a 500 ms IOI, followed by five more presentations. (This same conga drum sound was used in all of the remaining rhythm tests described below.) In five of the trials these last five sounds continued to be presented with a 500 ms IOI, i.e. there was no shift in tempo. In twenty-five of the trials the last five sounds were presented with a shorter IOI (faster tempo), with five trials each using IOIs of 450, 460, 470, 480, and 490 ms. In the remaining twenty-five trials the last five sounds were presented with a longer IOI (slower tempo), with five trials each using IOIs of 510, 520, 530, 540, and 550 ms. Participants’ ability to switch to the new tempo was assessed by calculating, for each of the last two intervals the participant produced in each of the fifty trials containing a tempo shift, the absolute value of the difference between the target IOI and the produced IOI. These two values were then averaged to form a tempo adaptation score for that trial. Performance across all fifty shifted trials was then averaged to compute an overall tempo adaptation score.
Memory/sequencing: Drumming along to rhythmic sequences
This test assessed participants’ ability to rapidly perceive and drum along to a temporal sequence. Four trials were presented. In each trial, a 3.2-second four-measure sequence taken from Povel and Essens [44] was repeated ten times, for a total of forty measures. Each four-measure sequence consisted of the conga sound presented nine times. Every sequence used the same set of IOIs: five 200 ms, two 400 ms, one 600 ms, and one 800 ms. The sequences differed in the order in which these IOIs were presented, which gave rise to different temporal patterns. Two of the trials contained sequences taken from the set of strongly metrical sequences listed in Povel and Essens [44], while two of the trials were weakly metrical sequences which contained more rests in strongly metrical positions (greater syncopation). Participants were asked to listen to the sequences and then, once they had a good idea of what the sequences were, drum along, aligning their movements exactly with what they heard.
Both the stimulus and drumming data were converted to a sequence of hits and rests. First, it was determined whether each 200 ms time interval in the stimulus contained a drum hit or silence. The drumming data was then similarly converted to a sequence of hits and rests: if a drum hit took place between 100 ms before and 100 ms after the onset of a given 200 ms stimulus interval a hit was added to the drum sequence, otherwise a rest was assumed. The test was then scored by comparing the sequence of drum hits and rests to the sequence of stimulus hits and rests. For example, if the stimulus sequence were [0 1 1 0] and the drumming sequence were [1 1 1 0], where a zero indicates a rest and a one indicates a hit, the participant’s score on this small section of the test would be 75%. Performance was calculated across the second through tenth repetitions of each rhythm and averaged across all four trials.
Memory/sequencing: Remembering temporal sequences
This test assessed participants’ ability to remember and repeat back a temporal sequence. Thirty trials were presented. During each trial a 3.2-second four-measure sequence from Povel and Essens [44] was repeated three times, followed by a pause equal in length to the duration of the sequence. During this pause the participant attempted to repeat the sequence exactly when it would have occurred had it repeated a fourth time. The first fifteen trials consisted of strongly metrical sequences, while the second fifteen trials consisted of weakly metrical sequences. As described above for the drumming along to temporal sequences task, performance on this task was analyzed by converting the stimulus and drumming data to sequences of hits and rests, and then compared to assess performance. Drum hits and rests were marked by searching for drum onsets in a 200-ms window centered around the time points when each stimulus interval would have begun had the stimulus repeated a fourth time. Performance was averaged across all thirty trials.
Verbal memory
Our goal was to determine whether beat-tapping and rhythm memory/sequencing form dissociable rhythm skills. However, this analysis relies upon the assumption that these tasks test rhythm skills, rather than (or in addition to) more general cognitive abilities. In particular it is important to establish that performance on the rhythm memory/sequencing tasks cannot entirely be accounted for by general memory skill. To test this participants were given the Auditory Working Memory (AWM) and Digits Reversed (DR) subtests from the Woodcock-Johnson III Test of Cognitive Abilities [45].
Statistical analysis
One participant was an outlier on metronome tapping variability (> standard deviations from the mean); this participant was removed from all analyses. For no other measures were there outliers greater than three standard deviations from the mean. A second participant was removed from analysis because during the metronome test the average difference between the mean tempo they produced and the metronome tempo was 38.7 ms. All other participants were within 5 ms of the target IOI on average. The Jacque-Bera test for normality revealed that the paced variability and drumming along to rhythmic sequences measures were not normally distributed (p < 0.05). As a result, we log-transformed the paced variability and rau-transformed the drumming to rhythmic sequences data, after which these measures were normally distributed (p > 0.05).
Performance across all four measures of rhythm ability was correlated using Person’s correlations. If all four measures broadly assess a single rhythmic competence, all four measures should correlate with one another. Conversely, we hypothesized that metronome tapping and tempo adaptation would be linked, due to a shared reliance on fine temporal acuity, and that drumming along to and remembering temporal sequences would be linked, as these tests require subjects to remember temporal patterns and produce a motor sequence that will reproduce them. However, we predicted that metronome tapping variability and temporal adaptation (beat-tapping set) would not relate to drumming along to and remembering temporal sequences (memory/sequencing set). See Fig 1 for a schematic of predicted relationships between performance on these rhythm tests. As a further test for the existence of multiple rhythm skills a generalized least squares factor analysis with varimax rotation was used to uncover latent variables reflecting shared variance across rhythm measures.
Results
Fig 2 shows scatterplots comparing performance across all four rhythm tests, while r-values and p-values for each correlation can be found in Table 1. Participants who tapped more consistently to a metronome were also better able to shift to a new metronome tempo (r = 0.51, p < 0.001). Moreover, participants who were highly accurate at drumming along to a temporal sequence also were more accurate when reproducing temporal sequences from memory (r = 0.73, p < 0.001). However, neither beat tapping measure (metronome tapping variability nor tempo adaptation) related to either memory measure (drumming along to or remembering temporal sequences (all p > 0.05)). Furthermore, factor analysis revealed that performance across the rhythm tests was best captured by two factors, which together accounted for 81% of the variance across the data set. The first factor consisted of performance on the drumming to sequences and remembering sequences tests. The second factor consisted of performance on the metronome tapping and tempo adaptation tests. See Table 2 for factor loadings across all four rhythm tests for each of the two factors.
Table 1. r-values and p-values for Pearson’s correlations comparing performance on all four rhythm tests.
r-value, p-value | Metronome tapping | Tempo adaptation | Drumming to sequences | Remembering sequences |
---|---|---|---|---|
Metronome tapping | ||||
Tempo adaptation | 0.51, 1.2x10 -5 | |||
Drumming to sequences | -0.03, 0.82 | -0.14, 0.28 | ||
Remembering sequences | -0.11, 0.37 | -0.22, 0.07 | 0.73, 4.8x10 -12 |
Table 2. Factor loadings from generalized least squares factor analysis.
Factor 1 | Factor 2 | |
---|---|---|
Metronome tapping | 0.049 | 0.882 |
Tempo adaptation | -0.165 | 0.857 |
Drumming to sequences | 0.928 | 0.010 |
Remembering sequences | 0.917 | -0.126 |
To illustrate the extent to which performance on beat tapping versus memory/sequencing tests was uncoupled, Fig 3 displays performance on beat tapping and memory/sequencing tests in individual subjects. Example subject #1 (Fig 3, top) tapped with little variability, producing a normalized metronome tapping variability of only 0.029, compared to the median across all participants of 0.043. Their ability to rapidly shift to a new metronome tempo was similarly impressive, with an average error of only 20.7 ms, putting them in the top 13% of participants tested in this study. However, their performance on the drumming to sequences test (62.3% correct) and the sequence memory test (61.8% correct) was quite low. Example subject #2 (Fig 3, bottom), on the other hand, performed more poorly on the beat tapping and tempo adaptation tests than every participant but one, producing a normalized metronome tapping variability of 0.079 and an average tempo adaptation error of 36.4 ms. However, their performance on both the drumming to sequences test (92.6%) and the sequence memory test (84.4%) was better than average.
To determine whether the rhythm sequencing/memory tasks assess truly rhythmic skills or whether they simply reflect general memory capacity we assessed the relationship between performance on these rhythm tasks and verbal memory. Correlations between the rhythm memory tests and the verbal memory tests were weaker than correlations between the two rhythm memory/sequencing tests: drumming to sequences correlated with backwards digit span at r = 0.25 (p < 0.05) and with verbal auditory working memory at r = 0.26 (p < 0.05), while remembering sequences correlated with backwards digit span at r = 0.38, p < 0.01 and with verbal auditory working memory at r = 0.35, p < 0.01). To determine whether verbal memory and rhythm memory are dissociable we ran two hierarchical regressions. In the first, we entered auditory working memory and backwards digit span on the first step and found that they predicted 17% of the variance in memory for rhythmic sequences (p < 0.01). However, adding drumming to sequences performance on the second step improved model fit by 41% (p < 0.001). For the second regression, we entered drumming to sequences and memory for sequences performance on the first step, and found that they predicted 15% of the variance in backwards digit span (p < 0.01). However, adding auditory working memory on the second step improved model fit by 25% (p < 0.001). Thus, verbal memory and rhythmic memory are overlapping but dissociable skills.
Discussion
Here we find that the ability to consistently synchronize and the ability to quickly adapt to a changing metronome tempo while synchronizing are linked (beat tapping measures). We also find that participants who are better able to remember rhythms and reproduce them are also better able to drum along to those rhythms (memory/sequencing measures). However, there was no relationship between either test of beat tapping and either test of rhythm memory/sequence processing. These results support the theory that there are multiple dissociable rhythm skills, but do not support the existence of a single overarching rhythmic competence or “rhythm IQ”.
If beat tapping and rhythm memory/sequencing are separable skills they may also rely on dissociable neural foundations. Neuroimaging studies have shown links between beat tapping and the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and primary motor cortex [46–49]. While rhythm memory/sequence perception has been linked to these same areas, it has also been associated with the premotor cortex and the supplementary motor area [50–54]. One difference between the neural structures on which these two sets of tasks rely, therefore, seems to be that the rhythm memory/sequence set relies upon motor planning regions in addition to the subcortical and primary cortical motor regions on which beat tapping depends. Furthermore, beat tapping may place more stringent demands on cerebellar processing than does rhythm sequence perception, as children with cerebellar lesions have been shown to be impaired on a beat tapping task but not a rhythm discrimination task that requires remembering rhythmic sequences [39]. Alternately, rhythm memory and beat tapping may rely on different subregions of the cerebellum. Beat tapping may also place greater demands on the stable representation of microsecond timing of sounds in the subcortical auditory system [55].
A crucial difference between beat tapping and rhythm memory/sequence processing that may drive the dissociation between the two sets of tasks is that the rhythm memory/sequence set requires integration of acoustic information across a greater length of time than does beat tapping. To track even a relatively short rhythm spanning a few measures requires retaining and integrating temporal information over several seconds, while the auditory-motor error-correction processes that comprise the beat tapping set operate over time spans of tens of milliseconds [56–59]. Reviews of the time perception literature [60–63] have indicated a dissociation between time perception at the millisecond scale, which is associated with the cerebellum [34,63–70], and time perception at the supra-second time scale, associated with the basal ganglia and cortical regions [63,65,70]. Integration of temporal information over longer durations may require motor planning processes [71]. Motor planning areas (premotor and supplementary motor areas) have strong internal connections that may make self-sustaining activity possible, enabling integration of information across longer durations, while cerebellar circuity lacks such internal excitatory connections [72].
Developing an informed taxonomy of rhythm skills could have important consequences for the study of links between rhythm aptitude and language proficiency. Previous studies of links between rhythm and language skills have used a wide variety of rhythm tasks, including rhythm discrimination [12–14.16–17], detection of off-beat sounds [15], remembering and repeating back rhythms [18–20], synchronizing to a metronome [20–24], tempo reproduction [20,25], tapping to the beat of music [26], and perception of musical meter [27]. Learning how these rhythm skills relate to one another could lead to insights regarding the relationship between rhythm and language. For example, different rhythm aptitudes may be linked to different language functions. Beat-tapping may not be linked to verbal memory because a simple continuous beat is easy to extract and because strictly periodic beats are not present in spoken language, but the temporal precision of auditory perception required by auditory-motor error-correction processes may also be vital for discrimination of speech sounds and development of phonological awareness [73]. Conversely, remembering rhythmic sequences may be more strongly linked to verbal memory, given the non-periodic rhythmic patterns which are found in speech [74].
It should be noted that all of our rhythm tasks require both perception and production. How are perception and production integrated and dissociated? A recent paper introducing a battery of tests of beat processing [30] reported surprisingly low correlations between perceptual and production metrics produced by each test. Moreover, unattended musical rhythms can affect perceptual processing, even when modalities of the rhythms and perceptual task differ [75–76]. Rhythm perception and production skill may, therefore, be dissociable. Given that our tasks here included both perceptual and production elements, future work should determine whether rhythm perception on fast and slow time scales can be dissociated in the absence of a production component.
Rhythm memory and verbal memory were dissociable skills, with the drumming to sequences task accounting for an additional 40% of the variance in the sequence memory task over the variance accounted for by both verbal memory tests. Nevertheless, rhythm memory and verbal memory were related: the rhythm memory/sequencing tests predicted 15% of the variance in backwards digit span scores, supporing the link between auditory short-term memory span and rhythm reproduction reported by Grahn and Schuit [77]. These results suggest that verbal memory depends to a limited extent on perception of and memory for temporal patterns. Efficient tracking of temporal patterns in speech such as the lengthened durations that mark phrase boundaries and stressed syllables may facilitate detection of these elements, freeing up cognitive resources which can then be devoted to rehearsal and manipulation of the material to be remembered.
Categorizing rhythm abilities into clusters that reflect the neural foundations on which they draw could have important clinical consequences. While there is some preliminary evidence that rhythm-based training can improve language skills [78–80], these studies have used wide-ranging training regimens which trained many different rhythm skills. Understanding how language relies on rhythm within the framework of the two rhythm competencies discovered here (beat tapping and memory/sequencing) could lead to targeted interventions for the subset of children whose language problems stem from these specific rhythmic deficits.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Trent Nicol, Jess Slater, and Kali Woodruff Carr for their helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. This research is funded by NSF SMA1015614, NIH DC009399, the Mathers Foundation, the National Association of Music Merchants, and the Knowles Hearing Center, Northwestern University.
Data Availability
Data are available on Figshare at http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1476252.
Funding Statement
This research is funded by NSF SMA1015614, NIH DC009399, the Mathers Foundation, the National Association of Music Merchants, and the Knowles Hearing Center, Northwestern University. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Brown S, Jordania J. Universals in the world’s musics. Psychology of Music 2013;41: 229–248. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Klatt D, Cooper W. Perception of segment duration in sentence contexts In: Structure and Process in Speech Perception. Berlin: Springer; 1975. pp. 69–89. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fant G, Kruckenberg A, Nord L. Durational correlates of stress in Swedish, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics 1991;19: 351–365. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Vaissiére J. Rhythm, accentuation and final lengthening In: Sundberg J, Nord L, Carlson R, editors. Music, Language, Speech and Brain, Vol. 59 (pp. 108–120). International Symposium Series. Stockholm: Wenner-Gren; 1991. pp. 108–120. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Venditti J, van Santen J. Modeling segmental durations for Japanese text-to-speech synthesis. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (Sydney, Australia); 1998. Available: http://www.cs.columbia.edu/~jjv/pubs/icslp_dur.pdf
- 6. Lieberman P. Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English. JASA 1960;32: 451–454. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Nakatani L, Schaffer J. Hearing “words” without words: prosodic cues for word perception. JASA 1978;63: 234–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Smith M, Cutler A, Butterfield S, Nimmo-Smith I. The perception of rhythm and word boundaries in noise-masked speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1989;32: 912–920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Cutler A, Butterfield S. Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language 1992;31: 218–236. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Mattys S. Stress versus coarticulation: toward an integrated approach to explicit speech segmentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2004;30: 397–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Spitzer S, Liss J, Mattys S. Acoustic cues to lexical segmentation: a study of resynthesized speech. JASA 2007;122: 3678–3687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wallentin M, Nielsen A, Friis-Olivarius M, Vuust C, Vuust P. The Musical Ear Test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences 2010;20: 188–196. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Strait D, Hornickel J, Kraus N. Subcortical processing of speech regularities predicts reading and music aptitude in children. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2011;7: 44 10.1186/1744-9081-7-44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Gordon R, Shivers C, Wieland E, Kotz S, Yoder P, McAuley J. Musical rhythm discrimination explains individual differences in grammar skills in children. Developmental Science 2015;18: 635–644. 10.1111/desc.12230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Hausen M, Torppa R, Salmela V, Vainio M, Sarkamo T. Music and speech prosody: a common rhythm. Frontiers in Psychology 2013;4: 566 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. McGivern R, Berka C, Languis M, Chapman S. Detection of deficits in temporal pattern discrimination using the seashore rhythm test in young children with reading impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities 1991;24: 58–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Douglas S, Willatts P. The relationship between musical ability and literacy skills. Journal of Research in Reading 1994;17: 99–107. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Dellatolas G, Watier L, Le Normand M, Lubart T, Chevrie-Muller C. Rhythm reproduction in kindergarten, reading performance at second grade, and developmental dyslexia theories. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2009;24: 555–563. 10.1093/arclin/acp044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. González-Trujillo M, Defior S, Guriérrez-Palma N. The role of nonspeech rhythm in Spanish word reading. Journal of Research in Reading 2014;37:316–330. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Flaugnacco E, Lopez L, Terribili C, Zoia S, Buda S, Tilli S, et al. Rhythm perception and production predict reading abilities in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2014;8: 392 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Thomson J, Fryer B, Maltby J, Goswami U. Auditory and motor rhythm awareness in adults with dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading 2006;29: 334–348. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Thomson J, Goswami U. Rhythmic processing in children with developmental dyslexia: auditory and motor rhythms link to reading and spelling. Journal of Physiology 2008;102: 120–129. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Corriveau K, Goswami U. Rhythmic motor entrainment in children with speech and language impairments: tapping to the beat. Cortex 2009;45: 119–130. 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Tierney A, Kraus N. The ability to tap to a beat relates to cognitive, linguistic, and perceptual skills. Brain and Language 2013;124: 225–231. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Moritz C, Yampolsky S, Papadelis G, Thomson J, Wolf M. Links between early rhythm skills, musical training, and phonological awareness. Reading and Writing 2013;26: 739–769. [Google Scholar]
- 26. David D, Wade-Wolley L, Kirby J, Smithrim K. Rhythm and reading development in school-age children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Reading 2007;30: 169–183. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Huss M, Verney J, Fosker T, Mead N, Goswami U. Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex 2011;47: 674–689. 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Gordon E. Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Peretz I, Champod A, Hyde K. Varieties of musical disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2003;999: 58–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Fujii S, Schlaug G. The Harvard Beat Assessment Test (H-BAT): a battery for assessing beat perception and production and their dissociation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2013;7: 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Fitch W, Rosenfeld A. Perception and production of syncopated rhythms. Music Perception 2007;25:43–58. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Pecenka N, Keller P. The role of temporal prediction abilities in interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization. Experimental Brain Research 2011;3–4:505–515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Keele S, Pokorny R, Corcos D, Ivry R. Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica 1985;60: 173–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Lewis P, Miall R. Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia 2003;41: 1583–1592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Schwartze M, Tavano A, Schröger E, Kotz S. Temporal aspects of prediction in audition: cortical and subcortical neural mechanisms. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2012;83: 200–207. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Grahn J, Brett M. Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Cognitive Neuroscience 2007;19: 893–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Fries W, Swihart A. Disturbance of rhythm sense following right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychologia 1990;28: 1317–1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Wilson S, Pressing J, Wales R. Modelling rhythmic function in a musician post-stroke. Neuropsychologia 2002;40: 1494–1505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Provasi J, Doyere V, Zelanti P, Differ V, Perdry H, El Massioui N, et al. Disrupted sensorimotor synchronization, but intact rhythm discrimination, in children treated for a cerebellar medulloblastoma. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2014;35: 2053–2068. 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sowinski J, Dalla Bella S. Poor synchronization to the beat may result from deficient auditory-motor mapping. Neuropsychologia 2013;51: 1952–1963. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Launay J, Grube M, Stewart L. Dysrhythmia: a specific congenital rhythm perception deficit. Frontiers in Psychology 2013;5: 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Semjen A, Vorberg D, Schulze H. Getting synchronized with the metronome: comparisons between phase and period correction. Psychol Res 1998;61: 44–55. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Thaut M, Miller R, Schauer L. Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: phase vs period correction. Biol. Cybern. 1998;79: 241–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Povel D, Essens P. Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception 1985;2: 411–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Woodcock R, McGre K, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-educational Battery (3rd ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Steele C, Penhune V. Specific increases within global decreases: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of five days of motor sequence learning. The Journal of Neuroscience 2010;30: 8332–8341. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5569-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Bijsterbosch J, Lee K, Hunter M, Tsoi D, Lankappa S, Wilkinson I, et al. The role of the cerebellum in sub- and supraliminal error correction during sensorimotor synchronization: evidence from fMRI and TMS. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2011;23: 1100–1112. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Steele, C. The relationship between brain structure, motor performance, and early musical training. Dissertation, Concordia University, Monreal, Quebec, Canada; 2012.
- 49. Hove M, Fairhurst M, Kotz S, Keller P. Synchronizing with auditory and visual rhythms: an fMRI assessment of modality differences and modality appropriateness. NeuroImage 2013;67: 313–321. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Sakai K, Hikosaka O, Miyauchi S, Takino R, Tamada T, Iwata N, et al. Neural representation of a rhythm depends on its interval ratio. Journal of Neuroscience 1999;19: 10074–10081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Schubotz R, Friederici A, Von Cramon D. Time perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 2000;11: 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Lewis P, Wing A, Pope P, Praamstra P, Miaal R. Brain activity correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initialization, synchronization, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping. Neuropsychologia 2004;42: 1301–1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Penhune V, Doyon J. Cerebellum and M1 interaction during early learning of timed motor sequences. NeuroImage 2005;26: 801–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Bengtsson S, Ullén F, Ehrsson H, Hashimoto T, Kito T, Naito E, et al. Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex 2009;45: 62–71. 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Tierney A, Kraus N. The ability to move to a beat is linked to the consistency of neural responses to sound. Journal of Neuroscience 2013;33: 14981–14988. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0612-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Repp B. Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research 2000;63: 106–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Repp B. Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Human Movement Science 2001;20: 277–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Thaut M, Kenyon G. Rapid motor adaptations to subliminal frequency shifts during syncopated rhythmic sensorimotor synchronization. Human Movement Science 2003;22: 321–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Madison G, Merker B. Human sensorimotor tracking of continuous subliminal deviations from isochrony. Neuroscience Letters 2004;370: 69–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Ivry R. The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1996;6: 851–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Buhusi C, Meck W. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2005;6: 755–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Coull J, Cheng R, Meck W. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011;36: 3–25. 10.1038/npp.2010.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Wiener M, Turkeltaub P, Coslett H. The image of time: a voxel-wise meta-analysis. NeuroImage 2010;49: 1728–1740. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Ivry R, Keele S. Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1989;1: 136–152. 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Mangels J, Ivry R, Shimizu N. Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal and neocerebellar cortex to time perception. Cognitive Brain Research 1998;7: 15–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Ivry R, Spencer R. The neural representation of time. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2004;14: 225–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Koch G, Oliveri M, Torriero S, Salerno S, Lo Gerfo E, Caltagirone C. Repetitive TMS of cerebellum interferes with millisecond time processing. Exp Brain Res 2007;179: 291–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Lee K, Egleston P, Brown W, Gregory A, Barker A, Woodruff P. The role of the cerebellum in subsecond time perception: evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007;19: 147–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Coull J, Nobre A. Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2008;18: 137–144. 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Cope T, Grube M, Singh B, Burn D, Griffiths T. The basal ganglia in perceptual timing: timing performance in Multiple System Atrophy and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2014;52: 73–81. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Patel A, Iversen J. The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 2014; 8:57 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Buonomano D, Laje R. Population clocks: motor timing with neural dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010;14: 520–527. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Tierney A, Kraus N. Auditory-motor entrainment and phonological skills: precise auditory timing hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2014;8: 949 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Kotz S, Schwartze M. Cortical speech processing unplugged: a timely subcortico-cortical framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010;14: 392–399. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Escoffier N, Sheng D, Schirmer A. Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. Acta Psychologica 2010;135: 12–16. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Brochard R, Tassin M, Zagar D. Got rhythm… for better and for worse. Cross-modal effects of auditory rhythm on visual word recognition. Cognition 2013;127: 214–219. 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Grahn J, Schuit D. Individual differences in rhythmic ability: behavioral and neuroimaging investigations. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain 2012;122: 105–121. [Google Scholar]
- 78. Overy K. Dyslexia, temporal processing and music: the potential of music as an early learning aid for dyslexic children. Psychology of Music 2000;28: 218–229. [Google Scholar]
- 79. Overy K. Dyslexia and music: from timing deficits to musical intervention. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2003;999: 497–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Bhide A, Power A, Goswami U. A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: a comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervenction. Mind, Brain, and Education 2013;7: 113–123. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data are available on Figshare at http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1476252.