Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Health Place. 2015 Jun 18;34:215–228. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.05.008

Black carbon exposure and extreme concentrations of residential economic and racial/ethnic privilege and deprivation

Nancy Krieger a,*, Pamela D Waterman a, Alexandros Gryparis b, Brent A Coull c
PMCID: PMC4681506  NIHMSID: NIHMS740761  PMID: 26093080

Abstract

Background

Scant data quantify associations between economic and racial/ethnic spatial polarization and individual’s exposure to pollution.

Methods

We linked data on the socioeconomic position (SEP) of 1757 urban working class white, black, and Latino adults (age 25–64; Boston, MA: 2003–2004–2008–2010) to: (1) spatiotemporal model-based estimates of cumulative black carbon exposure at their exact residential address, and (2) their census tract values for the Index of Concentration at the Extremes (ICE) for SEP and race/ethnicity.

Results

ICE measures, but not individual- and household-SEP, remained independently associated with black carbon exposure.

Conclusions

The ICE may be useful for environmental health research.

Keywords: Black carbon, Income, Race/ethnicity, Residential segregation, Socioeconomic

1. Introduction

Links between the spatial patterning of social vulnerability and exposure to environmental pollution is a topic of growing concern (Kruize et al., 2014; Nweke et al., 2011). To date, most studies have primarily focused on either racial/ethnic segregation at the city or regional level (Downey et al., 2008; Lopez, 2002), or on within-city concentrations of groups adversely affected by racial discrimination or economic deprivation, such as census tract percent black or below poverty (Goodman et al., 2011; James et al., 2012; Hajat et al., 2013). Much less research, however, has investigated how exposure to pollution (Morello-Frosch and Lopez, 2006; Brulle and Pellow, 2006; Pulido, 2000; Smith, 2009), or other public health outcomes (Ludwig et al., 2012), is shaped by the joint influences of within-city extreme concentrations of social position that reflect inequitable social class and race relations (Krieger et al., 1997; Krieger, 2012; Winant, 2000; Shaw et al., 2007). Recent social science research, however, has underscored the need to investigate consequences of rising spatial economic segregation, above and beyond the impact of individual- and household-level income (Massey, 1996; Dwyer, 2010).

In this study, we build on prior research investigating the social–spatial patterning of airborne pollutants, which have primarily focused on urban areas and whose findings typically show that neighborhoods with higher concentrations of poverty and of populations of color have higher exposure to traffic-related pollutants (Lopez, 2002; Goodman et al., 2011; James et al., 2012; Hajat et al., 2013; Krieger et al., 2014). We accordingly sought to improve understanding of the social determination of these patterns by analyzing exposure to one spatially patterned traffic-related air pollutant – black carbon – in relation to the recently developed Index of Concentration at the Extremes (ICE) (Massey, 2001). First proposed by Massey (2001), a leading scholar on racial/ethnic and economic segregation (Massey, 1996, 2012), the ICE simultaneously measures concentration of privilege and deprivation and can be computed at multiple levels and scales, and in the social science literature, has been used solely in relation to socioeconomic measures, such as income and education (Massey, 2001; Kramer, 2013). To our knowledge, the ICE has been employed in only 7 public health studies, none of which focused on environmental exposures (Kramer, 2013; Doh et al., 2007; Carpiano et al., 2009; Finch et al., 2010; Casciano and Massey, 2012; Eastwood et al., 2013; Rudolph et al., 2013). All of these studies employed solely socioeconomic ICE measures and all found evidence of associations between the ICE and the outcomes they studied that were independent of individual or household social characteristics. Guiding our decision to focus on black carbon as an exposure with notable public health significance, substantial evidence indicates that, as summarized in a recent major review article, black carbon is “causally involved in all-cause, lung cancer, and cardiovascular mortality, morbidity, and perhaps adverse birth and nervous system effects” (Grahame et al. 2014, p. 620). In the US, the largest single source of black carbon is transportation (2010 estimate: 216 out of 321 Gg emitted) (Grahame et al., 2014), rendering black carbon an important indicator of ambient traffic-related fine particulate matter, especially for urban air pollution (Grahame et al., 2014; Gryparis et al., 2007).

Our hypothesis was that, after controlling for individual and household social characteristics, higher extreme concentrations, at the census tract level, of economic and racial/ethnic privilege would be associated with lower residential black carbon exposure, as measured by long-term averages of black carbon levels. Conversely, high concentrations of economic deprivation and lack of racial/ethnic privilege would be associated with higher exposure, even after controlling for individual and household characteristics. Possible mechanisms leading to these associations, as discussed in prior research on social variation in exposure to traffic-related pollutants, involve the disproportionate siting of traffic routes with high exposure to car and truck exhaust in neighborhoods that are more likely to be low-income, with relatively few white residents, and with less distance between residential homes and the street curb, due to smaller (or no) sidewalks and smaller (or no) lawns or front-yards (Brulle and Pellow, 2006; Hajat et al., 2013; Lopez, 2002; Morello-Frosch and Lopez, 2006; Nweke et al., 2011; Pulido, 2000). To test our hypotheses, we conducted an epidemiological investigation using cross-sectional population-based observational data that linked rich individual- and household-level data of individual study participants to model-based spatiotemporal estimates of black carbon exposure at their residential address (exact latitutde–longitude) and to diverse census tract ICE measures pertaining to income, education, and race/ethnicity, singly and combined.

2. Materials/subjects and methods

2.1. Study population

We used the same study base employed in our prior analysis of black carbon exposure and census tract poverty (Krieger et al., 2014), whereby study participants comprised all persons enrolled, after written informed consent, in two prior Boston-based studies that employed identical socioeconomic and sociodemographic variables. The United for Health (UFH) study (2003–2004) recruited 1202 employed working class adults, age 25–64, who worked in wholesale meat and meat production, retail grocery stores, lighting fixtures manufacturing, and school bus services; the study response rate was 72% (Barbeau et al., 2007). The My Body, My Story (MBMS) study (2008–2010) recruited 1005 black and white non-Hispanic US-born members, age 35–64, from a random sample of members of four Boston community health centers; the study response rate was 82% (Krieger et al., 2011). Use of these previously collected data and their linkage, via geocoding, to census-derived area based data and to the black carbon data, was approved as exempt by the Harvard School of Public Health Institutional Review Board (Protocol #23169-101), effective November 5, 2012.

2.2. Sociodemographic and socioeconomic data: individual- and household-level

In our study, we conceptualized race/ethnicity as a social construct arising from inequitable race relations that shape living and working conditions and hence population health (Krieger, 2012; Winant, 2000). To measure this construct and to enable our data to be readily compared to other studies, we used the pre-specified self-report categories employed in the US census, which likewise employ a social definition of race/ethnicity (US Census Bureau, 2014). Data on nativity (US-born vs. foreign-born) and gender were also based on self-report.

We measured diverse aspects of socioeconomic position (SEP) at the individual- and household-level over the lifecourse, per our conceptualization of socioeconomic position as an inherently multidimensional, multilevel, and dynamic relational construct (Krieger et al., 1997; Shaw et al., 2007). Table 1 details the validated self-report socioeconomic measures employed, pertaining to occupational class, educational attainment (of the participant and of her/his parents, as a measure of childhood socioeconomic position), annual household income, and poverty level.

Table 1.

Social distribution of study participants’ residential black carbon exposure (cumulative 1-year exposure prior to study enrollment), overall and by race, ethnicity, for their census tract level Index of Concentration at the Extremes (ICE) and individual and household sociodemographic and economic characteristics: 1757 urban working class adults (age 25–64) enrolled in United for Health (UFH, Greater Boston Area, MA, 2003–2004) and My Body, My Story (MBMS, Boston, MA, 2008–2010).

VARIABLE Alla
(N=1757;
UFH=807;
MBMS=950)
White
(N=670;
UFH= 205;
MBMS = 465)
Black
(N=787;
UFH= 302;
MBMS=485)
Latino
(N=195;
UFH=195;
MBMS=0)
N %a Black carbon
(μg/m−3): mean
(SD)
F tes
p value
N % Black carbon
(μg/m−3):
mean (SD)
F test
p value
N % Black carbon
(μg/m−3): mean
(SD)
F test
p value
N % Black carbon
(μg/m−3): mean
(SD)
F test p value
ICE quintiles (cut-points)c
ICEincome
q1 (−0.12911) (lowest) 758 43.1 0.6725 (0.1358) 34.65 (<.0001) 174 26.0 0.7024 (0.1492) 25.44 (<.0001) 439 55. 8 0.6546 (0.1255) 8.07 (<.0001) 89 45.6 0.7136 (0.1388) 3.75 (0.0058)
q2 (0.06702) 525 29.9 0.6786 (0.1371) 213 31.8 0.6997 (0.1466) 228 29.0 0.6502 (0.1174) 56 28.7 0.6882 (0.1376)
q3 (0.21909) 285 16.2 0.6512 (0.1794) 163 24.3 0.6541 (0.1776) 80 10.2 0.6327 (0.1880) 31 15.9 0.6821 (0.1825)
q4 (0.35443) 133 7.6 0.5729 (0.1822) 85 12.7 0.5522 (0.1597) 25 3.2 0.6206 (0.2726) 15 7.7 0.5941 (0.1344)
q5 (0.76880) (highest) 56 3.2 0.4830 (0.1654) 35 5.2 0.4946 (0.1967) 15 1.9 0.4558 (0.1025) 4 2.0 0.5124 (0.0174)
ICeducation
q1 (0.01299) (lowest) 708 40.3 0.6707 (0.1395) 8.15 (<.0001) 179 26.7 0.7225 (0.1531) 17.45 (<.0001) 391 49.7 0.6393 (0.1224) 0.70 (0.5887) 85 43.6 0.7118 (0.1395) 2.67 (0.0334)
q2 (0.20797) 446 25.4 0.6644 (0.1686) 151 22.5 0.6888 (0.1963) 208 26.4 0.6474 (0.1545) 61 31.3 0.6663 (0.1431)
q3 (0.37484) 308 17.5 0.6247 (0.1389) 163 24.3 0.5961 (0.1337) 109 13.8 0.6612 (0.1354) 26 13.3 0.6307 (0.1448)
q4 (0.57532) 198 11.3 0.6692 (0.1910) 115 17.2 0.6566 (0.1895) 54 6.9 0.6611 (0.1920) 17 8.7 0.7471 (0.1973)
q5 (1.00000) (highest) 97 5.5 0.6055 (0.1312) 62 9.2 0.5827 (0.1128) 25 3.2 0.6470 (0.1564) 6 3.1 0.6517 (0.1132)
ICEwhite/black
q1 (0.45848) (lowest) 1206 68.6 0.6722 (0.1326) 28.60 (<.0001) 337 50.3 0.6969 (0.1392) 17.95 (<.0001) 656 83.4 0.6512 (0.1268) 12.48 (<.0001) 138 70.8 0.7044 (0.1209) 4.04 (0.0036)
q2 (0.72166) 304 17.3 0.6672 (0.1720) 168 25.1 0.6620 (0.1733) 85 10.8 0.6744 (0.1762) 33 16.9 0.6655 (0.1600)
q3 (0.85726) 89 5.1 0.5493 (0.1550) 50 7.5 0.5411 (0.1515) 24 3.0 0.5667 (0.1837) 9 4.6 0.5307 (0.1288)
q4 (0.92687) 97 5.5 0.6265 (0.2308) 67 10.0 0.6453 (0.2218) 15 1.9 0.5047 (0.1773) 12 6.2 0.7129 (0.3028)
q5 (0.99444) (highest) 61 3.5 0.5204 (0.1822) 48 7.2 0.5369 (0.1941) 7 0.9 0.4114 (0.1160) 3 1.5 0.5546 (0.0465)
ICEincome + white/black
q1 (0.06533) (lowest) 1026 58.4 0.6720 (0.1241) 26.85 (<.0001) 262 39.1 0.7022 (0.1301) 23.52 (<.0001) 572 72.7 0.6488 (0.1129) 5.33 (0.0003) 122 62.6 0.7104 (0.1260) 2.82 (0.0265)
q2 (0.21017) 376 21.4 0.6775 (0.1755) 176 26.3 0.6993 (0.1805) 144 18.3 0.6624 (0.1752) 37 19.0 0.6446 (0.1576)
q3 (0.31729) 153 8.7 0.6259 (0.1672) 89 13.3 0.6150 (0.1572) 36 4.6 0.6261 (0.1832) 20 10.3 0.6558 (0.1677)
q4 (0.42648) 147 8.4 0.5930 (0.2015) 109 16.3 0.5791 (0.1810) 19 2.4 0.6017 (0.2870) 13 6.7 0.6904 (0.2439)
q5 (0.79070) (highest) 55 3.1 0.5042 (0.1913) 34 5.1 0.5070 (0.1998) 16 2.0 0.5060 (0.2050) 3 1.5 0.5156 (0.0198)
ICEincome + white/of color
q1 (0.00096) (lowest) 1060 60.3 0.6752 (0.1237) 38.53 (<.0001) 284 42.4 0.7010 (0.1293) 28.89 (<.0001) 588 74.7 0.6517 (0.1128) 12.05(<.0001) 117 60.0 0.7216 (0.1241) 7.04(<.0001)
q2 (0.18464) 352 20.0 0.6761 (0.1848) 158 23.6 0.7017 (0.1903) 133 16.9 0.6642 (0.1868) 43 22.0 0.6284 (0.1487)
q3 (0.30584) 188 10.7 0.6258 (0.1581) 122 18.2 0.6285 (0.1520) 35 4.4 0.5766 (0.1350) 21 10.8 0.7129 (0.2072)
q4 (0.42062) 102 5.8 0.5618 (0.2032) 70 10.4 0.5393 (0.1759) 16 2.0 0.6466 (0.3354) 12 6.2 0.5585 (0.1259)
q5 (0.79070) (highest) 55 3.1 0.4760 (0.1660) 36 5.4 0.4929 (0.1953) 15 1.9 0.4361 (0.0818) 2 1.0 0.52025 (0.0255)
SOCIODEMOGRAPHIC
Age (years)
25–34 years 170 9.7 0.7379 (0.1751) 21.28 (<.0001) 34 5.1 0.7933 (0.1794) 8.68 (<.0001) 52 6.6 0.7340 (0.2086) 8.53 (<.0001) 56 28.7 0.7298 (0.1437) 4.41 (0.005)
35–44 years 561 32.0 0.6629 (0.1452) 208 31.1 0.6680 (0.1493) 248 31.6 0.6495 (0.1372) 73 37.4 0.6864 (0.1519)
45–54 years 649 37.0 0.6469 (0.1606) 258 38.6 0.6508 (0.1866) 309 39.4 0.6405 (0.1400) 52 26.7 0.6749 (0.1355)
55–64 years 373 21.3 0.6298 (0.1344) 168 25.2 0.6358 (0.1584) 176 22.4 0.6257 (0.1061) 14 7.2 0.5772 (0.1499)
(missing: N, %) 4 0.00 2 0.00 2 0.00
Nativity
US-born 1358 78.9 0.6617 (0.1520) 3.75 (0.053) 649 97.0 0.6601 (0.1714) 0.0 (0.952) 619 79.5 0.6584 (0.1298) 21.95 (<.0001) 50 28.7 0.6980 (0.1393) 0.02 (0.8793)
Not US-born 364 21.1 0.6440 (0.1635) 20 3.0 0.6624 (0.1636) 160 20.5 0.6013 (0.1641) 124 71.3 0.6941 (0.1569)
(missing: N, %) 35 0.02 1 0.00 8 0.01 21 0.11
Gender
Women 929 53.3 0.6575 (0.1469) 0.05 (0.8281) 367 54.9 0.6439 (0.1573) 7.34 (0.0069) 443 56.9 0.6596 (0.1390) 7.85 (0.0052) 80 58.3 0.6775 (0.1386) 0.88 (0.3502)
Men 813 46.7 0.6591 (0.1628) 302 45.1 0.6798 (0.1848) 335 43.1 0.6314 (0.1389) 112 58.3 0.6980 (0.1565)
(missing: N, %) 15 0.01 1 0.00 9 0.01 3 0.02
ECONOMIC
Individual-level: current
Occupational class
Working class: non-supervisory employee 753 48.3 0.6566 (0.1589) 1.2 (0.309) 262 43.0 0.6679 (0.1785) 0.66 (0.6203) 343 49.8 0.6362 (0.1411) 1.03(0.3916) 91 56.2 0.6954 (0.1523) 1.11 (0.3487)
Not working class:
supervisory employee 347 22.2 0.6710 (0.1659) 131 21.5 0.6746 (0.1801) 147 21.4 0.6582 (0.1632) 37 22.8 0.7076 (0.1534)
self-employed/freelance 120 7.7 0.6364 (0.1338) 52 8.5 0.6414 (0.1343) 39 5.7 0.6410 (0.1147) 22 13.6 0.6369 (0.1655)
own or run business 85 5.4 0.6547 (0.1632) 40 6.7 0.6472 (0.1623) 29 4.2 0.6436 (0.1831) 12 7.41 0.6775 (0.1348)
Not in paid labor force 255 16.4 0.6554 (0.1493) 125 20.5 0.6497 (0.1755) 130 18.9 0.6608 (0.1194)
(missing: N, %) 197 0.11 60 0.09 99 0.13 33 0.17
Educational attainment
<high school (HS) b 305 18.2 0.6693 (0.1714) 5.16(0.0058) 85 12.9 0.6687 (0.2151) 4.94(0.0074) 136 17.7 0.6541 (0.1426) 0.22(0.8013) 67 42.1 0.7002 (0.1636) 2.0 (0.1385)
≥ HS and < 4 yrs college 1089 64.9 0.6600 (0.1516) 409 61.9 0.6720 (0.1655) 536 69.9 0.6453 (0.1414) 80 50.3 0.6860 (0.1425)
≥ 4 yrs college 285 17.0 0.6314 (0.1393) 167 25.3 0.6240 (0.1500) 95 12.4 0.6455 (0.1156) 12 7.6 0.6056 (0.1264)
(missing: N, %) 78 0.04 9 0.01 20 0.03 36 0.18
Household-level: current
Annual household income
< $12,000 412 26.1 0.6775 (0.1536) 3.47(0.0021) 117 18.6 0.6860 (0.1698) 2.45 (0.024) 200 28.6 0.6690 (0.1396) 1.46(0.1896) 67 41.4 0.6968 (0.1675) 0.67(0.6481)
$12,00 to <$36,000 548 34.7 0.6628 (0.1565) 201 32.0 0.6732 (0.1746) 231 33.1 0.6412 (0.1430) 79 48.8 0.6857 (0.1403)
$36,000 to <$48,000 111 7.0 0.6633 (0.1838) 45 7.2 0.7056 (0.1634) 50 7.2 0.6302 (0.1936) 7 4.3 0.6968 (0.2177)
$48,000 to <$72,000 259 16.4 0.6295 (0.1587) 127 20.2 0.6321 (0.1884) 120 17.2 0.6331 (0.1256) 4 2.5 0.5760 (0.0553)
$72,000 to <$120,000 119 7.5 0.6340 (0.1202) 77 12.2 0.6303 (0.1222) 41 5.9 0.6368 (0.1159) 1 0.6 0.8124
$120,000 to < $144,000 41 2.6 0.6467 (0.1572) 27 4.3 0.6357 (0.1634) 13 1.9 0.6778 (0.1491) 0 0
> = $144,000 88 5.6 0.6352 (0.1413) 35 5.6 0.6284 (0.1797) 43 6.2 0.6286 (0.1101) 4 2.5 0.6451 (0.0810)
(missing: N, %) 179 0.10 41 0.06 89 0.11 33 0.17
Poverty level (household)
< 100% poverty 574 36.5 0.6722 (0.1508) 6.85 (0.0011) 152 24.2 0.6842 (0.1658) 3.41 (0.0337) 287 41.2 0.6610 (0.1390) 2.61 (0.0742) 98 60.9 0.6933 (0.1595) 1.68 (0.1905)
100 to 199% poverty 350 22.3 0.6639 (0.1722) 139 22.2 0.6729 (0.1882) 149 21.4 0.6399 (0.1616) 41 25.5 0.7054 (0.1458)
≥ 200% poverty 648 41.2 0.6404 (0.1489) 336 53.6 0.6440 (0.1653) 260 37.4 0.6348 (0.1276) 22 13.7 0.6344 (0.1337)
(missing: N, %) 185 0.11 43 0.06 91 0.12 34 0.17
Census tract: current
≥ 20% below poverty 840 47.8 0.6989 (0.1384) 69.57(<.0001) 221 33.0 0.7321 (0.1559) 34.18(<.0001) 466 59.2 0.6749 (0.1252) 27.94(<.0001) 90 46.2 0.7363 (0.1216) 16.5(<.0001)
10 to 19% below poverty 449 25.6 0.6601 (0.1548) 191 28.5 0.6725 (0.1563) 196 24.9 0.6351 (0.1463) 45 23.1 0.7247 (0.1694)
5 to 9 % below poverty 303 17.2 0.5950 (0.1360) 153 22.8 0.6069 (0.1469) 89 11.3 0.5729 (0.1252) 44 22.6 0.5970 (0.1267)
<5% below poverty 165 9.4 0.5521 (0.1758) 105 15.7 0.5623 (0.1886) 36 4.6 0.5170 (0.1721) 16 8.2 0.5629 (0.1000)
(missing: N, %) 0 0 0 0
Childhood: household education (highest level: mother, father, or guardian)
< high school (HS)b 394 27.4 0.6624 (0.1565) 4.39(0.0126) 91 14.9 0.6772 (0.1805) 4.6(0.0104) 208 32.6 0.6492 (0.1445) 0.38(0.687) 76 65.5 0.6836 (0.1542) 0.63(0.5342)
≥ HS and < 4 yrs college 750 52.2 0.6570 (0.1572) 338 55.2 0.6649 (0.1735) 339 53.0 0.6469 (0.1392) 33 28.4 0.6744 (0.1621)
≥ 4 yrs college 292 20.3 0.6296 (0.1381) 183 29.9 0.6238 (0.1460) 92 14.4 0.6346 (0.1215) 7 6.0 0.6151 (0.1282)
(missing: N, %) 321 0.18 58 0.09 148 0.19 79 0.41

ICEincome: N of households in 80th (highest) versus 20th (lowest) household income percentile

ICEeducation: N of adults ≥ 25 years old who have 4+ years of college vs. < high school

ICEwhite/black: N of persons who are white versus black

ICEincome + white/black N of persons who are both high income (top quintile) and white vs are both low income (bottom quintile) and black

ICEincome + white/of color N of persons who are both high income (top quintile) and white vs are both low income (bottom quintile) and of color

a

All includes 78 participants from United for Health who self-identified as belonging to other populations of color and 17 for whom race/ethnicity was missing; all participants in My Body, My Story had to self-identify as either white or black and none had missing data on race/ethnicity.

b

<high school includes persons who have left school prior to obtaining a high school degree or 12 years of education or who have not obtained a GED certificate

c

ICE range: −1 (least privileged) to 1 (most privileged); the ICE comparison (extreme) groups are as follows, with quintile cut-points based on the distribution of values within the black carbon catchment area

2.3. Census tract data: geocoding, poverty level, and Index of Concentration at the Extremes

Informed by the methods and findings of our Public Health Disparities Geocoding Project (Krieger et al., 2005), we were able to geocode 93% of the UFH and 95% of the MBMS participants to latitude–longitude based on residential street address. We used these geocodes to link the study participant data to the census tract and black carbon data. We obtained the census tract data from 2006 to 2010 from the American Community Survey (ACS) (US Census Bureau, 2014), and computed the percent of persons below poverty and determined each tract’s income, education, and racial/ethnic composition, needed to compute the ICE measures. We used the 2006–2010 ACS data because research shows that single-year estimates from the ACS at the census tract level are highly imprecise and vary across years (due both to sampling frames and sampling size) (Spielman et al., 2014), and the more stable 5-year estimate we employed encompasses the enrollment period for the MBMS study, which the earliest available 5-year ACS estimate (2005–2009) does not (US Census Bureau, 2014).

The ICE is defined as (Massey, 2001):

ICEi=(Ai-Pi)/Ti

where, say, Ai = number of affluent persons in neighborhood i (e.g., in 80th income percentile), Pi = number of poor persons in neighborhood i (e.g., in 20th income percentile), and Ti = total population with known income level in neighborhood i. The ICE thus can range from −1 to 1, thereby capturing simultaneously concentrated extremes of privilege and deprivation while avoiding problems of multi-colinearity arising when separate measures of deprivation and advantage are used (Massey, 2001). Moreover, unlike most measures of residential segregation that are calculated at the city-level or higher (e.g., the index of dissimilarity, which typically is computed by determining the number of persons who would have to move across census tracts to achieve an even racial/ethnic distribution) (Massey, 2012), the ICE can be meaningfully quantified at diverse geographic or administrative levels, whether block, census tract, city, or region (Massey, 2001; Kramer, 2013). Another useful feature is that it shows the directionality of the concentration (i.e., −1 to 1), as opposed to showing only that inequality in distribution exists (as per the index of dissimilarity and other residential segregation indices).

To generate the ICE socioeconomic measures, we set as the extremes: (a) for income, the ACS household income categories that most closely approximated cut-points for the US 20th and 80th household income percentiles for this time period (i.e., < $25,000 and ≥$100,000; US Census Bureau, 2014), thereby using percentile cut-points routinely employed for analyzing income inequality (Shaw et al., 2007); and (b) for education among persons ≥25, persons with <high school education and ≥4 years of college. We also created three novel ICEs. The first was for race/ethnicity, setting as the extreme groups persons who self-identified as white vs those who self-identified as black. The other two combined data on income and race/ethnicity: both set as most privileged white persons whose household income was ≥80th income percentile and set as least privileged persons in households below the 20th income percentile and who additionally were classified as either black or as a person of color (i.e., inclusive of all persons other than white). We computed the ICEs for all CTs in which the study participants resided, and additionally computed them for all CTs within the air pollution monitoring catchment area, which encompasses the Greater Boston Area (Gryparis et al., 2007; Krieger et al., 2014).

2.4. Exposure to black carbon

We estimated each individual’s 1-year cumulative average exposure to ambient black carbon exposure at the longitude–latitude of their residential address in the year prior to their study enrollment. The model is described in detail elsewhere (Gryparis et al., 2007), but briefly, it is informed by data collected over the period of 1999–2008, involving over 8700 daily observations obtained from 134 locations, most of which monitored black carbon continuously using aethalometers. Covariates in the prediction model included cumulative traffic density within 100 m, geographic information system (GIS) location (latitude, longitude), daily meteorological factors (apparent temperature, wind speed, and height of the planetary boundary layers), and other characteristics (day of week, day of season), and separate models were fit for warm and cold seasons. Using this model, a validation sample at an additional 30 monitoring sites found an average correlation of 0.59 between the predicted and observed black carbon levels, indicating the model is appropriate (Gryparis et al., 2007).

2.5. Analytic methods

The analytic dataset included the 1757 participants (UFH: 807; MBMS: 905) (Krieger et al. 2006, 2011) who resided in the air monitor catchment area (Gryparis et al., 2007) and whose records we were able to geocode to latitude-longitude. Building on our prior analyses (Krieger et al., 2014) we first analyzed the distribution of the study participants’ residential black carbon exposure (mean and standard deviation (SD)) in relation, separately, to each category of their census tract ICE measures, their census tract poverty level, and their individual- and household-level social characteristics. We next analyzed the Pearson correlation value for associations between the ICE measures and also between each ICE measure and the census tract poverty level, for the census tracts in which the study participants resided. We then analyzed, for all census tracts in the air pollution monitoring catchment area, the proportion of the census tract population within the extreme groups defined for each ICE measure, so as to determine, empirically, what proportion of census tracts with ICE values equal to 0 belonged to which of the three hypothetically possible scenarios that could give rise to these values: (i) an equal number of persons in the top and bottom groups, with these two groups together comprising some proportion (>0%) of the total population; (ii) an equal number of persons in the top and bottom groups, with these two groups comprising the total population; and (iii) no persons in both the top and bottom group; we carried out analogous analyses for census tracts with ICE values close to 0.

We then conducted multivariable linear regression to quantify the association between the study participants’ census tract ICE measures or poverty level and their residential annual average black carbon exposure, in models that controlled for their individual- and household-level SEP and other relevant covariates. The models were of the form: Y=β1X1 + β2X2 + … βnXn + ε, where Y is the black carbon exposure, X1Xn refer to the exposure (ICE) and covariates included in the models, and ε is the error term. In all models, we examined the extent of variation (R2) in the exposure explained by the included variables.

3. Results

Table 1 provides data on the distribution (mean and SD) of the study participants’ residential black carbon exposure (i.e., average cumulative exposure for the year prior to their enrollment in their health study) in relation to: (1) quintile of their census tract ICE measures, and (2) their additional individual and household sociodemographic characteristics. We present these results for the total study population and by race/ethnicity. The participants’ black carbon exposure exhibited positive associations with virtually all of their census tract ICE measures (more concentrated privilege, less exposure). Thus, for example, for ICEincome, the average annual black carbon exposure, among all study participants, ranged from 0.6725 μg/m3 (SD=0.1368) in the lowest quintile to 0.4830 μg/m3 (SD=0.1654) in the highest quintile (F test p value <0.0001), and the same pattern was observed among the white non-Hispanic, black non-Hispanic, and Hispanic/Latino participants (Table 1). The only exception, showing no variation, was the ICE for educational level among black non-Hispanic participants (Table 1). To provide context for these data, in Table 1 we also show that, as we have reported previously: (a) the black and Latino participants were, compared to white participants, more subjected to socioeconomic deprivation, as both children and as adults, at the individual, household, and census tract level (Krieger et al. 2006, 2011), and (b) the association of estimated black carbon exposure at the participants’ residential address was greater for their census tract poverty level as compared to their individual and household SEP (Krieger et al., 2014).

The mean value for the ICEincome (Table 2) was notably highest among white participants in MBMS (mean=0.030, SD 0.272), intermediate and similar by race/ethnicity among UHF participants (mean −0.121, SD 0.243), and lowest among black MBMS participants (mean= −0.202, SD 0.249). For ICEwhite/black, among black participants, the mean value ranged from a low of −0.200 (SD 0.499) in MBMS to 0.116 (SD 0.483) in UFH, whereas among white participants, the mean value ranged from 0.057 (SD 0.530) in UFH to high of 0.486 (SD 0.403) in MBMS (Table 2). Additionally, correlations between the ICE measures and census tract poverty were high (r>0.65) in all racial/ethnic groups, except for ICEwhite/black (r≤0.5) (Table 2). As further shown in Fig. 1, in the catchment area outside of Boston, the extreme concentration of white populations were located both in areas of extreme concentrations of high income and of low income. Within Boston, census tracts with extreme concentrations of high education and white populations did not necessarily have extreme high income, e.g., in areas known to have large student populations.

Table 2.

Distribution of and correlation between study participants’ census tract measures of Index of Concentration of Extremes and poverty, for the 1757 urban working class adults (age 25–64) enrolled in United for Health (UFH, Greater Boston Area, MA, 2003–2004) and My Body, My Story (MBMS, Boston, MA, 2008–2010).

Group Variable Mean (SD) Median Inter-quartile range
(IQR)
Minimum Maxi-
mum
Pearson correlation coefficient (p-value)
ICE income ICE education ICE white/
black
ICE income +
white/black
ICE income + white/
of color
Census tract
poverty
UFH
Total (N=807) ICEincome −0.121 (0.243) −0.115 0.301 0.794 0.601 1.000
ICEeducation 0.027 (0.251) 0.008 0.349 0.569 0.845 0.748 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.077 (0.514) 0.124 0.812 −0.889 0.970 0.552 0.491 1.000
ICEincome + white/black 0.005 (0.208) 0.012 0.284 −0.552 0.591 0.787 0.619 0.905 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.108 (0.265) −0.129 0.380 −0.727 0.591 0.916 0.748 0.799 0.921 1.000
Census tract poverty 20.8 (12.7) 20.4 17.0 0.0 86.3 −0.852 −0.575 −0.475 −0.661 −0.785 1.000
Black (N=302) ICEincome −0.131 (0.238) −0.134 0.295 −0.794 0.601 1.000
ICEeducation −0.001 (0.253) −0.002 0.346 −0.569 0.814 0.749 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.116 (0.483) 0.178 0.499 −0.889 0.963 0.521 0.398 1.000
ICEincome + white/black 0.018 (0.196) 0.026 0.187 −0.552 0.591 0.760 0.542 0.903 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.112 (0.255) −0.138 0.349 −0.686 0.591 0.922 0.732 0.764 0.889 1.000
Census tract poverty 21.0 (12.1) 20.9 16.9 0.0 67.3 −0.851 −0.564 −0.432 −0.605 −0.773 1.000
Latino (N=195) ICEincome −0.106 (0.248) −0.092 0.344 −0.794 0.509 1.000
ICEeducation 0.058 (0.249) 0.036 0.355 −0.414 0.845 0.787 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.016 (0.551) 0.037 1.039 −0.889 0.958 0.603 0.589 1.000
ICEincome + white/black −0.014 (0.221) −0.007 0.342 −0.552 0.477 0.824 0.716 0.913 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.108 (0.277) −0.146 0.422 −0.686 0.458 0.921 0.795 0.834 0.950 1.000
Census tract poverty 20.2 (13.9) 18.0 18.7 1.9 86.3 −0.818 −0.608 −0.463 −0.654 −0.745 1.000
White (N=205) ICEincome −0.106 (0.235) −0.092 0.258 −0.760 0.484 1.000
ICEeducation 0.036 (0.229) 0.002 0.310 −0.414 0.762 0.735 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.057 (0.530) 0.140 0.873 −0.889 0.969 0.594 0.564 1.000
ICEincome + white/black 0.000 (0.214) −0.001 0.284 −0.552 0.579 0.818 0.679 0.911 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.096 (0.267) −0.096 0.364 −0.727 0.579 0.911 0.742 0.835 0.945 1.000
Census tract poverty 20.2 (12.1) 18.0 17.6 0.6 57.1 −0.879 −0.592 −0.578 −0.747 −0.838 1.000
MBMS
Total (N=950) ICEincome −0.089 (0.285) −0.085 0.418 −0.794 0.631 1.000
ICEeducation 0.166 (0.276) 0.174 0.417 −0.483 0.885 0.710 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.136 (0.569) 0.162 1.007 −0.889 0.993 0.587 0.674 1.000
ICEincome + white/black 0.027 (0.241) 0.051 0.368 −0.552 0.605 0.846 0.767 0.895 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.066 (0.300) −0.049 0.466 −0.727 0.605 0.921 0.781 0.831 0.966 1.000
Census tract poverty 21.5 (15.6) 17.2 22.1 0.0 67.3 −0.896 −0.538 −0.486 −0.708 −0.800 1.000
Black (N=485) ICEincome −0.202 (0.249) −0.175 0.300 −0.794 0.619 1.000
ICEeducation 0.068 (0.251) 0.028 0.349 −0.483 0.877 0.597 1.000
ICEwhite/black −0.200 (0.499) −0.283 0.778 −0.889 0.986 0.363 0.641 1.000
ICEincome + white/black −0.108 (0.204) −0.143 0.313 −0.552 0.583 0.759 0.722 0.842 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color −0.222 (0.249) −0.256 0.318 −0.727 0.583 0.890 0.756 0.714 0.942 1.000
Census tract poverty 27.0 (15.5) 24.4 22.2 0.0 67.3 −0.898 −0.424 −0.229 −0.601 −0.738 1.000
White (N=465) ICEincome 0.030 (0.272) 0.078 0.287 −0.794 0.631 1.000
ICEeducation 0.269 (0.264) 0.265 0.325 −0.442 0.885 0.718 1.000
ICEwhite/black 0.486 (0.403) 0.616 0.627 −0.889 0.993 0.604 0.587 1.000
ICEincome + white/black 0.168 (0.192) 0.158 0.263 −0.455 0.605 0.884 0.743 0.844 1.000
ICEincome + white/of color 0.098 (0.259) 0.110 0.310 −0.621 0.605 0.932 0.735 0.820 0.965 1.000
Census tract poverty 15.7 (13.4) 11.3 14.1 0.0 57.2 −0.871 −0.523 −0.555 −0.725 −0.815 1.000

Note: p value<0.0001 for all Pearson correlation coefficients

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Map of census tract values for Index of Concentration at the Extremes for income and race/ethnicity and for poverty (most privileged: darkest green; least privileged: darkest brown): black carbon measurement catchment area and Boston, MA (2006–2010).

Table 3 provides empirical data regarding the meaning of an ICE value equal to 0 (or close to 0), analyzed in relation to all CTs included in the air pollution monitoring catchment area. In the case of an ICE value equal to 0, these occurred for only 0.1% to at most 0.6% of the 1069 census tracts in the catchment area, and for 4 of the 5 ICE measures, these 0s were because the extreme categories contained 0 persons, with the one exception being the ICE for income only (for which the top and bottom categories comprised 25% of the total population). Census tracts with ICE values close to 0 (ranging from [−0.5 to 0.5]) in turn comprised anywhere from just under 2% of the 1609 census tracts (for the ICE for white vs. black) to just over 11% (for the ICE for income only), and for all 5 ICE measures, the proportion of persons in the top plus bottom extreme categories ranged from 10.6% (for the ICE for high income white population vs. low income black population) to 57.8% (for the ICE for white vs. black), with a mean value of 35.2% (SD 24.7%). Together, these data indicate that a value of 0 (or close to 0) does arise from population distributions in which most (and rarely all) of the population is not in the extreme categories.

Table 3.

ICE measures: distribution of population for census tracts (CTs) in which the ICE equals 0 or is close to 0 (−0.5 to 0.5), for the black carbon air monitoring catchment area (N=1069 CTs), using 5-year average 2006–2010 census tract data from the American Community Survey.

ICE measure ICE value (CT) Nof CTs (%) TOTALNofPersons in CTs TOP CATEGORYNof persons (%) BOTTOM CATEGORYNof persons (%) TOP + BOTTOMNof persons (%)
ICEincome 0 4 (0.4%) 1323 164 (12.4%) 164 (12.4%) 328 (24.8%)
ICEeducation 1 (0.1%) 26 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%)
ICEwhite/black 1 (0.1%) 52 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%)
ICE income+white/black 6 (0.6%) 3001 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%)
ICEincome+white/of color 3 (0.3%) 36 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%) 0 (0.0%)
ICEincome 0.5 to 0.5 121 (11.3%) 214335 47193 (22.0%) 45798 (21.4%) 92991 (43.4%)
ICEeducation 72 (6.7%) 213985 43147 (20.2%) 41105 (19.2%) 84252 (39.4%)
ICEwhite/black 18 (1.7%) 58970 17303 (29.3%) 16794 (28.5%) 34097 (57.8%)
ICE income+white/black 107 (10.0%) 158283 9009 (5.7%) 7752 (4.9%) 16761 (10.6%)
ICEincome+white/of color 94 (8.8%) 158891 16300 (10.3%) 16665 (10.5%) 16761 (10.6%)

Table 4 presents results of the linear regression analyses for the ICE measures and black carbon. The observed inverse associations between the participants’ ICE measures with their residential black carbon exposure were greater (and more likely to have 95% CI that excluded 0) compared to those observed for their individual and household SEP (Models 1 and 2). The R-squared values (>0.10) for baseline models (Model 2) that included the ICE income-related measures along with study and exam date were also two to three times higher than those of the analogous baseline models with only ICEwhite/black (R2=0.036).

Table 4.

Regression estimates of association of ICE, sociodemographic, and socioeconomic variables with black carbon exposure (μg/m3): 1757 urban working class adult participants of United for Health (UFH, Greater Boston Area, MA, 2003–2004) and My Body, My Story (MBMS, Boston, MA, 2008–2010)

Variable Model 1 (univariable)
Model 2a
Model 2b
Model 2c
Model 2d
Model 2e
Model 2f
Model 3
beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI) beta (95% CI)
ICE
ICEincome −0.1448 (−0.1710, −0.1185) −0.1604 (−0.1864, −0.1344)
ICEeducation −0.0404 (−0.0667, −0.0140) −0.0434 (−0.0707, −0.0162)
ICErace/ethnicity (white/black) −0.0471 (−0.0601, −0.0339) −0.0553 (−0.0684, −0.0423)
ICEincome + white/of color −0.1292 (−0.1539, −0.1046) −0.1492 (−0.1739, −0.1246)
ICEincome+white/black −0.1549 (−0.1860, −0.1239) −0.1760 (−0.2069, −0.1452)
Sociodemographic
Race/ethnicity:Black −0.0137 (−0.0296, ,0.0022) 0.0007 (−0.0168, 0.0182)
Latino 0.0281 (0.0035, 0.0527) −0.0144 (−0.0417, 0.0130)
Other of color 0.0025 (−0.0337, 0.0387) −0.0173 (−0.0552, 0.0206)
White (referent) 0.0000 0.0000
Age (yr) −0.0026 (−0.0033, −0.0018) −0.0020 (−0.0029, −0.0012)
Gender: Women −0.0016 (−0.0162, 0.0129) 0.0047 (−0.0103, 0.0198)
Men (referent) 0.0000 0.0000
Socioeconomic
Annual household income −0.0090 (−0.0135, −0.0045)
Census tract poverty 0.0034 (0.0030, 0.0039) 0.0038 (0.0033, 0.0043)
Education (adult)
 < HS/12yr/GED 0.0379 (0.0131, 0.0627)
 ≥HS and < 4yr college 0.0286 (0.0086, 0.0486)
 ≥4yr college (referent) 0.0000
Education (child-hood household)
 < HS/12yr/GED 0.0328 (0.0095, 0.0560)
 ≥HS and < 4yr college 0.0274 (0.0066, 0.0481)
 ≥4yr college (referent) 0.0000
Covariates
Study: MBMS −0.0338 (−0.0483, −0.0194) 0.2727 (0.1973, 0.3480) 0.2176 (0.1388, 0.2965) 0.2534 (0.1762, 0.3305) 0.2923 (0.2161, 0.3684) 0.2716 (0.1957, 0.3475) 0.2824 (0.2094, 0.3555) 0.1674 (0.0787, 0.2560)
UFH (referent) 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000 0.000 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000
Exam date −0.00002 (−0.00002, −0.00001) −0.00014 (−0.00017, −0.00010) −0.00011 (−0.00015, −0.00008) −0.00013 (−0.00016, −0.00010) −0.00015 (−0.00018, −0.00011) −0.00014 (−0.00017, −0.00010) −0.00015 (−0.00018, −0.00011) −0.00009 (−0.00013, −0.00005)
R-square 0.1048 0.0355 0.0669 0.1024 0.0948 0.1523 0.0433

Among models containing the baseline and sociodemographic variables (Model 3 and 4), those with the ICE and census tract poverty variables (Model 4) likewise accounted for a greater amount of the variance in black carbon exposure (R2>0.12) compared to models including only the baseline and sociodemographic and household- and individual-level socioeconomic data (Model 3; R2=0.0433). In these models, the ICE measures remained independently inversely associated with black carbon exposure, whereas the individual- and household-level SEP measures did not (Model 5), with the exception of household income and education in the ICE models for education and for race/ethnicity.

4. Discussion

Our results indicate that extreme concentrations of socioeconomic resources and racial/ethnic privilege, structured by social class and race relations, as manifested at the census tract level, are inversely associated with individuals’ residential exposure to black carbon, an important airborne pollutant, even after controlling for individual and household social characteristics. Two additional noteworthy findings are that: (a) extreme concentrations of high and low income were more strongly associated with residential black carbon exposure than were extreme concentrations of race/ethnicity (e.g., black vs. white), and (b) ICE measures for both income and race/ethnicity were more strongly associated with black carbon exposure than were individual- and household-level socioeconomic and sociodemographic characteristics.

The plausibility of our findings rests in part on the study strengths and limitations. Strengths include reliance on two multiethnic working class population-based studies with high response rates (Krieger et al. 2006, 2011), and for whom a very high percent (>95%) had residential addresses that could be geocoded to latitude–longitude and thus linked to validated model-based spatiotemporal estimates for residence-specific long-term ambient black carbon exposure (Gryparis et al., 2007). Two limitations pertain to measurement error. First, annual ACS estimates have been shown to have high uncertainty due to the small number of persons sampled (Spielman et al., 2014); we accordingly used only the 5-year average ACS estimates. Second, the black carbon exposures estimates are model based, also potentially introducing imprecision of measurement (Gryparis et al., 2007; Krieger et al., 2014). Both of these limitations, each involving non-differential measurement error, would likely bias results to the null. Also potentially leading to conservative estimates of association was the largely working class composition of the study participants (Barbeau et al., 2007; Krieger et al., 2011); had the study base included more affluent and more highly educated professionals, greater associations might have been observed, given the likely wider range of black carbon exposure and also ICE values.

Our finding that the ICE measures remained associated with the specified outcome (in our case, black carbon), independent of individual- and household-level socioeconomic position, is consistent with the findings of both prior social science studies (e.g., Massey, 2001; Casciano and Massey, 2008; Kubrin and Stewart, 2006) and also the handful of public health studies that have used the ICE (Kramer, 2013; Doh et al., 2007; Carpiano et al., 2009; Finch et al., 2010; Casciano and Massey, 2012; Eastwood et al., 2013; Rudolph et al., 2013). None of these studies, however, used an ICE for race/ethnicity, alone or combined with income, nor did any investigate environmental pollution. Our findings further clarify, empirically, that, at least in the case of a major US city, values of or close to 0 for an ICE overwhelmingly occur because the numbers of persons in the top and bottom groups is equal or close to equal and typically comprise about 1/3 of the total population, as opposed to arising because 0 persons are in the top and bottom groups. To our knowledge, our results are the first empirical findings analyzing the population distributions that contribute to an observed ICE value of 0 (or close to 0).

Considered together, these prior studies and our results support the need for analysis of the inequitable health consequences of increasingly polarized concentrations of high and low income, as linked to high and low wealth (Piketty, 2014), in conjunction with present and past patterns of racial/ethnic residential segregation (Massey, 1996, 2012) and on-going evidence of racial discrimination in the housing market (Massey, 2012; Hanson and Hawley, 2011). These patterns of spatial social polarization are, as indicated by prior research, likely to shape risk of exposure to traffic-related air pollution, both because of siting of densely-trafficked streets and because neighborhoods with fewer economic and social resources are likely to have smaller (or no) sidewalks and residences with smaller (or no) front yards or lawns (Brulle and Pellow, 2006; Hajat et al., 2013; Lopez, 2002; Morello-Frosch and Lopez, 2006; Nweke et al., 2011; Pulido, 2000). A particular advantage of the novel ICEs we created that combine income and racial/ethnic data, moreover, is that in contrast to analytic approaches that treat these two variables separately (and, in some cases, attempt to model interaction effects) (Smith, 2009), the new ICEs we have deployed enable capturing, in a way that is socially meaningful and statistically tractable, the joint realities of spatial concentrations of economic and racial/ethnic privilege and lack of such privilege.

In conclusion, research on environmental exposures will likely benefit from using area-based measures, such as the ICE, that that capture growing social extremes. As suggested by our results, a focus solely on individual- and household socioeconomic and so-ciodemographic characteristics does not suffice, and nor is it adequate to focus solely on racial/ethnic segregation without also data on economic segregation. The larger implication is that research on health equity, including in environmental health, must reckon with the intertwining of economic and racial/ethnic polarization at multiple scales and political levels of geography (Polednak, 1997; Morello-Frosch, 2002; Krieger, 2012; Kruize et al. 2014). Such data are likely to be useful to the policy makers, scientists, and community advocates seeking to redress social inequalities in exposure to environmental pollution (Brulle and Pellow, 2006; Downey et al., 2008; Goodman et al., 2011; Hajat et al. 2013; James et al., 2012; Kruize et al., 2014; Morello-Frosch and Lopez, 2006; Morello-Frosch, 2002; Nweke et al., 2011; Pulido, 2000; Smith, 2009).

Supplementary Material

Suppl

Acknowledgments

Source of funding: This study was supported in part by Pilot Project funding from the HSPH-NIEHS Center for Environmental Health (ES000002), EPAR-834798, and EPARD 83479801. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the grantee and do not necessarily represent the official views of the USEPA. Further, USEPA does not endorse the purchase of any commercial products or services mentioned in the publication.

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.05.008

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Barbeau EM, Hartman C, Quinn MM, Stoddard AM, Krieger N. Methods for recruiting white, black, and Hispanic working class women and men to a study of physical and social hazards at work: the United for Health Study. Int J Health Serv. 2007;37:127–144. doi: 10.2190/B0N2-5850-6467-0230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brulle RJ, Pellow DN. Environmental justice: human health and environmental inequalities. Annu Rev Public Health. 2006;27:103–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.27.021405.102124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Carpiano RM, Lloyd JEV, Hertzman C. Concentrated affluence, concentrated disadvantage, and children’s readiness for school: a population-based, multi-level investigation. Soc Sci Med. 2009;69:420–432. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Casciano R, Massey DS. Neighborhoods, employment, and welfare use: assessing the influence of neighborhood socioeconomic composition. Soc Sci Res. 2008;37:544–558. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Casciano R, Massey DS. Neighborhood disorder and anxiety symptoms: new evidence from a quasi-experimental study. Health Place. 2012;18:180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Doh DP, Dubowitz T, Bird CE, Lurie N, Escarce JJ, Finch BK. Neighborhood context and ethnicity differences in body mass index: a multilevel analysis using the NHANES III survey (1988–1994) Econ Hum Biol. 2007;5:179–203. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2007.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Downey L, Dubois S, Hawkins B, Walker M. Environmental inequality in Metropolitan America. Organ Environ. 2008;21:270–294. doi: 10.1177/1086026608321327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dwyer RE. Poverty, prosperity, and place: the shape of class segregation in the age of extremes. Soc Probl. 2010;57:114–137. [Google Scholar]
  9. Eastwood JG, Jalaludin BB, Kemp LA, Phung HN, Barnett BEW. Immigrant maternal depression and social networks. A multilevel Bayesian spatial logistic regression in South Western Sydney, Australia. Spat Spat-Temp Epidemiol. 2013;6:49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.sste.2013.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Finch BK, Do DP, Heron M, Bird C, Seeman T, Lurie N. Neighborhood effects on health: concentrated advantage and disadvantage. Health Place. 2010;16:1058–1060. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Goodman A, Wilkinson P, Stafford M, Tonne C. Characterising socioeconomic inequalities in exposure to air pollution: a comparison of socioeconomic markers and scales of measurement. Health Place. 2011;17:767–774. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Grahame TJ, Klemm R, Schlesinger RB. Public health and components of particulate matter: the changing assessment of black carbon. J Air Waste Manag. 2014;64:620–660. doi: 10.1080/10962247.2014.912692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gryparis A, Coull BA, Schwartz J, Suh HH. Semiparametric latent variable regression models for spatiotemporal modeling of mobile source particles in the greater Boston area. Appl Stat. 2007;56:183–209. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hajat A, Diez-Roux AV, Adar SD, Auchincloss AH, Lovasi GS, O’Neill MS, et al. Air pollution and individual and neighborhood socioeconomic status: evidence from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121:1325–1333. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1206337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hanson A, Hawley Z. Do landlords discriminate in the rental housing market? Evidence from an internet field experiment in US cities. J Urban Econ. 2011;70:99–114. [Google Scholar]
  16. James W, Jia C, Kedia S. Uneven magnitude of disparities in cancer risk from air toxics. Int J Env Res Public Health. 2012;9:4365–4385. doi: 10.3390/ijerph9124365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kramer M. Some thoughts on measuring place and health. Presentation at Health Places Research Group, Georgia Institute of Technology; March 26, 2013; 2013. Available: < http://www.cqgrd.gatech.edu/sites/files/cqgrd/measuringneighborhoods_march2013.pdf> (accessed 08.05.15) [Google Scholar]
  18. Krieger N. Methods for the scientific study of discrimination and health: from societal injustice to embodied inequality – an ecosocial approach. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:936–945. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Krieger N, Chen JT, Waterman PD, Rehkopf DH, Subramanian SV. Painting a truer picture of US socioeconomic and racial/ethnic health inequalities: the Public Health Disparities Geocoding Project. Am J Public Health. 2005;95:312–323. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2003.032482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Krieger N, Waterman PD, Gryparis A, Coull BA. Black carbon exposure more strongly associated with census tract poverty compared to household income among US black, white, and Latino working class adults in Boston, MA (2003–2010) Environ Pollut. 2014;190:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.envpol.2014.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Krieger N, Waterman PD, Hartman C, Bates LM, Stoddard AM, Quinn MM, et al. Social hazards on the job: workplace abuse, sexual harassment, and racial discrimination – a study of black, Latino, and white low-income women and men workers (US) Int J Health Serv. 2006;36:51–85. doi: 10.2190/3EMB-YKRH-EDJ2-0H19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Krieger N, Waterman PD, Kosheleva A, Chen JT, Carney DR, Smith KW, et al. Exposing racial discrimination: implicit & explicit measures—the My Body, My Story study of 1005 US-born black & white community health center members. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(11):e27636. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Krieger N, Williams D, Moss N. Measuring social class in US public health research: concepts, methodologies and guidelines. Annu Rev Public Health. 1997;18:341–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kruize H, Droomers M, van Kamp I, Ruijsbroek A. What causes environmental inequalities and related health effects? An analysis of evolving concepts. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11:5807–5827. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110605807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kubrin CE, Stewart EA. Predicting who reoffends: the neglected role of neighborhood context in recidivism studies. Criminology. 2006;44:165–198. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lopez R. Segregation and black/white differences in exposure to air toxics 1990. Environ Health Perspectives. 2002;110(Suppl 2):289–295. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110s2289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ludwig J, Duncan GJ, Gennetian LA, Katz LF, Kessler RC, Kling JR, et al. Neighborhood effects on the long-term well-being of low-income adults. Science. 2012;337:1505–1510. doi: 10.1126/science.1224648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Massey DS. The age of extremes: concentrated affluence and poverty in the twenty-first century. Demography. 1996;33:395–412. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Massey DS. The prodigal paradigm returns: ecology comes back to sociology. In: Booth A, Crouter A, editors. Does It Take a Village? Community Effects on Children, Adolescents, and Families. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2001. pp. 41–48. [Google Scholar]
  30. Massey DS. Reflections on the dimensions of segregation. Soc Forces. 2012;93:39–43. doi: 10.1093/sf/sos118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morello-Frosch RA, Lopez R. The riskscape and the color line: examining the role of segregation in environmental health disparities. Environ Res. 2006;102:181–196. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Morello-Frosch RA. Discrimination and the political economy of environmental inequality. Env Plann C-Gov Policy. 2002;20:477–496. [Google Scholar]
  33. Nweke O, Payne-Sturges D, Garcia L, Lee C, Zenick H, Grevatt P, et al. Symposium on integrating the science of environmental justice into decision-making at the Environmental Protection Agency: an overview. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(Suppl 1):S19–S26. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Piketty T. In: Capital in the Twenty-First Century. Goldhammer Arthur., translator. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  35. Polednak AP. Segregation, Poverty, and Mortality in Urban African Americans. Oxford University Press; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  36. Pulido L. Rethinking environmental racism: white privilege and urban development in Southern California. Ann Assoc Am Geogr. 2000;90:12–40. [Google Scholar]
  37. Rudolph AE, Crawford ND, Latkin C, Fowler JH, Fuller CM. Individual and neighborhood correlates of membership in drug using networks with a higher prevalence of HIV in New York City (2006–2009) Ann Epidemiol. 2013;23:267–274. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2013.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Shaw M, Galobardes B, Lawlor DA, Lynch J, Wheeler B, Davey Smith G. The Handbook of Inequality and Socioeconomic Position: Concepts and Measures. The Policy Press; Bristol, UK: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  39. Smith CL. Economic deprivation and racial segregation: comparing Super-fund sites in Portland, Oregon, and Detroit, Michigan. Soc Sci Res. 2009;38:681–692. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Spielman SE, Filch D, Nagle N. Patterns of causes of uncertainty in the American Community Survey. Appl Geogr. 2014;46:147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. US Census Bureau. [accessed 08.05.15];American Community Survey, 2006–2010. 2014 Available: < http://www.census.gov/acs/www/>.
  42. Winant H. Race and race theory. Annu Rev Soc. 2000;26:169–185. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Suppl

RESOURCES