Abstract
Objective
Underutilization of needed mental health services continues to be the major mental health disparity affecting Asian Americans (Sue, Cheng, Saad, & Chu, 2012). The goal of the study was to apply a social psychological theoretical framework—the Health Belief Model (Rosenstock, 1966)—to understand potential reasons why Asian Americans underutilize mental health services relative to White Americans.
Method
Using a cross-sectional online questionnaire, this study examined how perceived severity of symptoms, perceived susceptibility to mental health problems, perceived benefits of treatment, and perceived barriers to treatment influenced intentions to seek help among a sample of 395 Asian American and 261 White American students experiencing elevated levels of psychological distress.
Results
Analyses using structural equation modeling indicated that Asian Americans in distress had relatively lower intentions to seek help compared to White Americans. Perceived benefits partially accounted for differences in help-seeking intentions. Although Asian Americans perceived greater barriers to help-seeking than White Americans, it did not significantly explain racial/ethnic differences in help-seeking intentions. Perceived severity and barriers were related to help-seeking intentions in both groups.
Conclusions
Outreach efforts that particularly emphasize the benefits of seeking mental health services may be a particularly promising approach to address underutilization. These findings have implications in help-seeking promotion and outreach.
Keywords: Asian Americans, help-seeking, psychological distress, underutilization, Health Belief Model
Most individuals in psychological distress do not seek mental health treatment. Studies show that less than half of individuals who are in need of services receive them (e.g., Wang et al., 2005). Underutilization has been particularly problematic for Asian Americans (Sue et al., 2012). Although epidemiological studies find that Asian Americans tend to have the lowest rates of diagnosable mental health problems compared to other racial/ethnic groups, Asian Americans considerably underutilize mental health services relative to their level of need (Alegria et al., 2008; Meyer, Zane, Cho, & Takeuchi, 2009). A better understanding of factors related to help-seeking is needed to eventually eliminate extant inequities in access to and utilization of mental health care.
The experience of Asian American college students lends itself well to the study of mental health disparities. Asian American students tend to report more depression and anxiety symptoms compared to White American students (e.g., Chang, 2002; Young, Fang, & Zisook, 2010), yet they underutilize services relative to their level of need (Eisenberg, Golberstein, & Gollust, 2007). In a large study of college students with depression and/or anxiety symptoms, Asian Americans were the least likely of all racial/ethnic groups to perceive a need for services (Eisenberg et al., 2007). University settings are an opportune context to examine help-seeking because most universities provide free on-campus counseling services. Reduced structural barriers (e.g., availability, cost) relative to community settings allows for better studying the psychological factors that impact help-seeking intentions, which is an important correlate of help-seeking behavior (Wilson, Deane, Ciarocchi, & Rickwood, 2005). Although recent studies have examined factors related to help-seeking among Asian American students (e.g., P. Y. Kim & Lee, 2013), few have utilized theoretical models to examine what factors influence specific help-seeking intentions among those who are currently experiencing psychological distress. We aimed to fill this need in understanding why Asian Americans who are presumably in need of services still do not seek help.
The Health Belief Model for Mental Health Help-Seeking Intentions
Rosenstock’s (1966) Health Belief Model (HBM) posits that threat of illness and expectations of treatment impact people’s decisions to undertake health-related behaviors. Threat involves perceptions of being susceptible or vulnerable to a health problem (referred to as perceived susceptibility) and perceptions regarding the severity of that problem (perceived severity). Expectations involve perceptions about the benefits and barriers of seeking and receiving treatment for the problem (perceived benefits and perceived barriers), and cues to action describe external determinants of health behaviors (e.g., family influences). The revised HBM (Rosenstock, Strecher, & Becker, 1988) includes self-efficacy within expectations, such that individuals believe they can change through appropriate help-seeking. The HBM has been useful in understanding many types of health-related behaviors (Janz & Becker, 1984), particularly as it relates to disease prevention behaviors and treatment adherence behaviors (e.g., Tanner-Smith & Brown, 2010).
Scholars have discussed the theoretical applicability of the HBM in examining mental health help-seeking (Henshaw & Freedman-Doan, 2009), though few studies to our knowledge have utilized it as such. One study found that the HBM was useful in predicting intentions to seek help for mental health among university students in Australia (O’Connor, Martin, Weeks, & Ong, 2014). However, their study did not account for level of need—a critical determinant of utilization (Pescosolido & Boyer, 2010)—nor did they examine group differences in help-seeking. We aimed to fill these gaps by examining help-seeking intentions of Asian and White American students experiencing psychological distress. The HBM is a promising framework from a mental health disparities perspective (Henshaw & Freedman-Doan, 2009) because cultural beliefs and preferences in terms of illness perceptions and treatment expectations may interfere with receiving timely and effective care for Asian Americans (Leong & Lau, 2001).
We extended the HBM to further understand why Asian Americans in psychological distress would still underutilize mental health services relative to White Americans. To delineate a parsimonious model of help-seeking intentions, we examined the four main variables of the HBM (i.e., perceived severity, susceptibility, benefits, and barriers). We did not include cues to action, as we were mainly interested in examining the psychological aspects of the model. Figure 1 depicts our conceptual model, and we discuss below our rationale for selection of specific variables that were chosen to represent each factor.
Figure 1.
Conceptual Model of Help-Seeking Intentions
Functioning as perceived severity
Functional impairment is a diagnostic criterion for many psychiatric disorders, often reflecting the severity of problems (Cauce et al., 2002). Because perceived severity in the HBM involves the extent to which people believe that a problem has serious consequences and will interfere with daily functioning (Henshaw & Freedman-Doan, 2009), we conceptualized it as individuals’ appraisals of functioning (Saleeby, 2000). Psychological distress and functional impairment do not necessarily co-occur, but distress with impaired functioning may serve as a cue that problems warrant professional attention (Cauce et al., 2002). Krumrei, Newton, and E. Kim (2010) found that most college students who sought counseling reported decrements in academic and social functioning. Relatively few studies, however, have examined the role of functioning in help-seeking intentions. Asian Americans tend to delay seeking help until problems become quite severe, such that they tend to have greater severity of symptoms, including reduced functioning, at the outset of mental health treatment relative to White Americans (Durvasula & Sue, 1996; Kearney, Draper, & Baron, 2005). We hypothesized that perceived severity, operationalized as functioning, may partly explain lower help-seeking intentions among Asian American students relative to White American students.
Mental health literacy as perceived susceptibility
Perceived susceptibility describes the extent to which individuals believe that they are susceptible or vulnerable to a health problem. This is closely associated with problem recognition, as those who do not recognize problems would be unlikely to believe that they are susceptible (Henshaw & Freedman-Doan, 2009). The importance of problem recognition is underscored by other theoretical formulations of help-seeking (e.g., Cauce et al., 2002; Saunders, 1993). It has been hypothesized that inadequate problem recognition partly accounts for underutilization (Jorm, 2012), as individuals who do not recognize psychological problems and/or do not know that symptoms are treatable would be unlikely to seek help (Goldney, Fisher, Wilson, & Cheok, 2002). Mental health literacy describes knowledge and awareness of psychological symptoms and its treatment, and low mental health literacy among racial/ethnic minorities, such as Asian Americans, has been discussed as an explanation for differential rates of help-seeking (Jorm, 2012; Li & Keshavan, 2010). We operationalized perceived susceptibility as mental health literacy, or problem recognition, and examined the extent to which it influences help-seeking intentions among individuals in distress. We hypothesized that lower mental health literacy among Asian Americans relative to White Americans would partially account for differences in help-seeking intentions.
Perceived benefits of seeking and receiving help
Perceived benefits involve the extent to which people believe that treatment will be effective in reducing symptoms. Moreover, perceived benefits and self-efficacy are related for psychological help-seeking (Henshaw & Freedman-Doan, 2009) because one must believe that therapy can help relieve distress (perceived benefit) and that one is capable of making psychological and behavioral changes (self-efficacy). Broadly conceptualized as perceived benefits, we examined positive attitudes and beliefs regarding help-seeking (treatment credibility) and perceived benefits of self-disclosing to a mental health professional (anticipated self-disclosure utility). Treatment credibility refers to the general belief that therapy is effective and helpful (Zane et al., 2005), which is believed to influence subsequent engagement in treatment (Kung, 2004; Sue & Morishima, 1982). The extent to which credibility has influences on help-seeking intentions has not been well examined. Credibility is a domain in which there is likely to be racial/ethnic variation, as many Asian Americans may hold the belief that therapy is not a credible way to handle emotional problems (Kung, 2004; Sue & Zane, 1987). Akutsu, Lin, and Zane (1990) found that treatment credibility was particularly important in help-seeking intentions for Chinese American students, although they did not account for psychological distress.
Self-disclosure is a pivotal component of effective psychotherapy, and level of comfort with self-disclosing information influences help-seeking behavior (Hinson & Swanson, 1993). Anticipated utility of self-disclosure refers to the perceived value of the outcome for disclosing to a mental health professional (Vogel & Wester, 2003), which has been found to be strongly associated with positive attitudes towards help-seeking (Nam et al., 2013). Individuals from Asian cultural backgrounds, however, may not perceive self-disclosure to be very valuable or beneficial to improving their mental health problems. Self-disclosure runs counter to Asian values that place emphasis on keeping problems within the family, maintaining social harmony, restraining emotions, and avoiding loss of face (B. S. K. Kim, 2007). For these reasons, we hypothesized that perceived benefits, as measured by treatment credibility and self-disclosure utility, would partially explain lower help-seeking intentions among Asian Americans relative to White Americans.
Help-seeking stigma as perceived barriers
Perceived barriers to help-seeking may be physical, financial, or psychological. One prominent psychological barrier is help-seeking stigma, which includes social and self-stigma (Corrigan, 2004). Social stigma refers to the perception that a person who seeks help is undesirable or socially unacceptable (Komiya, Good, Sherrod, 2000), whereas self-stigma involves a reduced sense of self-worth by internalizing the notion that help-seeking is socially unacceptable (Vogel, Wade, & Haake, 2006). Both types of stigma have been found to be related to less favorable help-seeking attitudes (Nam et al., 2013). Prior studies have demonstrated that Asian American students tend to report greater stigma about mental illness and help-seeking compared to White American students (Golberstein, Eisenberg, & Gollust, 2008). Asian cultural values tend to emphasize avoidance of shame (B. S. K. Kim, 2007), and help-seeking for mental health problems may be regarded as especially shameful or embarrassing. We expected that greater concerns about stigma may partially explain lower help-seeking intentions for Asian American students relative to White American students.
In sum, we utilized the HBM as a framework to further understand potential reasons why Asian Americans in psychological distress would have lower intentions to seek help relative to White Americans. Using an indirect effects approach in structural equation modeling, we examined the extent to which perceived severity, susceptibility, benefits, and barriers accounted for lower help-seeking intentions among Asian Americans relative to White Americans.
Method
Participants
Participants were undergraduates enrolled in various psychology courses at a large, public west coast university. Initially, 1,005 participants were surveyed. Of these, 65.3% (n = 656, 39.8% White Americans, 60.2% Asian Americans) indicated at least moderate psychological distress and were included in further analyses. The majority of the sample was female (71.0%) with a mean age of 19.8 years (SD = 1.9). The Asian American participants included Chinese (40.0%), Vietnamese (14.9%), Korean (12.4%), Asian Indian (9.4%), Filipino (8.1%), and Japanese (2.3%). Approximately 15.9% indicated prior use of mental health services. Table 1 displays additional sample information.
Table 1.
Sample Characteristics by Racial/Ethnic Group
White Americans (n = 261) | Asian Americans (n = 395) | Total (N = 656) | p-value from t or chi-square | |
---|---|---|---|---|
% Female | 71.6 | 70.6 | 71.0 | .908 |
M Age in years (SD) | 19.7 (2.2) | 19.9 (1.7) | 19.8 (1.9) | .317 |
% Born in foreign country | 3.8 | 27.1 | 17.9 | < .001 |
Academic standing | .039 | |||
% First-year students | 34.5 | 28.6 | 30.9 | |
% Second-year students | 29.5 | 24.6 | 26.5 | |
% Third-year students | 20.7 | 24.1 | 22.7 | |
% Fourth-year students | 15.3 | 22.8 | 19.8 | |
M K6 score (SD) (0–4 scale) | 1.49 (.59) | 1.61 (.63) | 1.56 (.61) | .013 |
M Attributions of distress | ||||
(SD) (1–5 scale) | ||||
Financial concerns | 2.47 (1.18) | 2.77 (1.26) | 2.65 (1.24) | .002 |
Academic concerns | 3.94 (1.01) | 4.12 (.92) | 4.05 (.96) | .019 |
Physical health concerns | 2.53 (1.23) | 2.42 (1.22) | 2.47 (1.22) | .265 |
% Indicating prior use of services | 25.3 | 9.6 | 15.9 | < .001 |
Procedure
This cross-sectional online survey lasted 30–45 minutes per participant. Participants were recruited via the psychology department participant recruitment website. Individuals indicated their interest in participating by following a link, which routed them to the informed consent. After the initial demographic questionnaire, study measures were administered in a random order. Participants received course credit. The university IRB approved the study.
Measures
Demographics
Respondents provided their age, gender, race/ethnicity, generation status, college major, academic standing (e.g., first year, second year), and any current or prior use of mental health services.
Psychological distress
The 6-item Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K6; Kessler et al., 2002) assessed nonspecific depressive and anxiety symptoms in the past 30 days. A sample item is “During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel hopeless?” on a scale of 0 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time). Scores range from 0 to 24, with 13+ indicating probable serious mental illness (Kessler et al., 2002). The scale has been used with good reliability in a sample of Asian Americans (Alegria et al., 2008) and Asian American college students (Bahrassa, Syed, Su, & Lee, 2011). We included respondents with a K6 score of 5+, as scores between 5 and 12 have been found to be valid indicators of probable moderate psychological distress (Prochaska, Sung, Max, Shi, & Ong, 2012). Cronbach’s α was .74.
Attributions of distress
Three questions assessed respondents’ attributions of distress in areas related to financial concerns, academic concerns, and physical health concerns. Following the K6, respondents were asked “To what extent have financial concerns or financial stress (or academic concerns; or concerns about physical health) been the main cause of these feelings?” on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much).
Perceived severity
Perceived severity was measured by the Short Form-36 Health Survey (SF-36; Ware & Sherbourne, 1992), consisting of 36 items covering eight areas of health-related quality of life in the past month. We used the subscales of role limitations due to personal or emotional problems (RL; 3 items) and social functioning (SF; 2 items). A sample RL item is “During the past 4 weeks, have you had any of the following problems with your work or other regular daily activities as a result of any emotional problems (such as feeling depressed or anxious)? Cut down the amount of time you spent on work or other activities” with a yes/no response. A sample SF item is “During the past 4 weeks, to what extent has your physical health or emotional problems interfered with your normal social activities with family, friends, neighbors, or groups?” with response options including not at all, a little bit, moderately, quite a bit, and extremely. In accordance with scoring instructions, a score was assigned to each response option, and subscale scores were derived by computing the mean (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Both RL and SF scores range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating fewer limitations and greater functioning, respectively. McHorney, Ware, & Raczek (1993) found that mental health subscales of the SF-36 (e.g., RL) were able to distinguish groups with and without psychiatric disorders. The SF-36 has been previously used in a study examining health-related quality of life among racial/ethnic minorities, including Asian Americans (Jackson-Triche et al., 2000). Cronbach’s αs in the current study were .83 (RL) and .70 (SF).
Perceived susceptibility
Perceived susceptibility, or problem recognition, was measured using mental health literacy vignettes for anxiety and depression. Anxiety literacy was assessed using a vignette by Coles and Coleman (2010), which described an individual with general anxiety disorder based on DSM-IV criteria. Respondents read the vignette and selected a diagnosis in a multiple-choice format with 12 choices (e.g., general life stress, general anxiety disorder). Respondents selected the primary cause of the problem (e.g., stress, mental illness) and indicated their professional help-seeking recommendations (yes, no, undecided). They then imagined themselves as the portrayed individual and responded with how they would personally deal with this experience. Coles and Coleman reported that their measure was developed based on clinical experience, published case studies, and Jorm et al.’s (1997) depression vignette. We used Jorm et al.’s vignette to assess mental health literacy for depression, which used the same sequence of questions as above. Goldney, Fisher, and Wilson (2001) reported that individuals with depression were nearly 10 times more likely than individuals without depression to indicate that they had similar problems as the portrayed person. Prior studies using both of these vignettes have assessed recognition as a dichotomous response (correct or incorrect) on each disorder. We created a continuous measure for each disorder by taking the sum of all correct responses, with a possible range of 0 to 9.
Perceived benefits
The Treatment Goals Measure (TGM; Zane et al., 2005) assessed credibility regarding therapy and attitudes and beliefs about the importance of focusing on certain issues/problems in therapy. The TGM includes 18 items (e.g., “How important is it that therapy helps you reduce your anxiety and discomfort?”) on a scale of 1 (unimportant) to 5 (important). A factor analysis by Zane et al. showed that all items loaded onto a single factor reflecting the extent to which therapy had multiple benefits, with higher scores indicating greater benefits from therapy. Zane et al. used this measure with Asian American therapy clients and reported a reliability of .95. In the current study, we modified terminology for relevance (e.g., “How important would it be that therapy helps you reduce your anxiety and discomfort?”). Cronbach’s α was .98.
The Anticipated Utility subscale of the Disclosure Expectation Scale (DES; Vogel & Wester, 2003) assessed beneficial attitudes toward self-disclosing to a counselor. It consists of four items (e.g., “Would you feel better if you disclosed feelings of sadness or anxiety to a counselor?”) on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (very). Vogel and Wester reported adequate construct validity and a reliability of .83. We modified terminology to reflect self-disclosing to a broader range of mental health professionals, and the term “counselor” was substituted by “therapist/counselor/mental health professional.” Cronbach’s α was .81.
Perceived barriers
The Social Stigma for Receiving Psychological Help Scale (SSRPH; Komiya et al., 2000) assessed perceived social stigma regarding help-seeking. It consists of 5 items (e.g., “Seeing a psychologist for emotional or interpersonal problems carries social stigma”) on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), with higher scores indicating greater social stigma. Komiya et al. reported that lower SSRPH scores were related to more favorable help-seeking attitudes, and reported a reliability of .72. SSRPH has been used with acceptable reliability in previous research with Asian American students (Shea & Yeh, 2008). We modified terminology in the current study by substituting “psychologist” for “therapist/counselor/mental health professional.” Cronbach’s α was .76.
The Self-Stigma for Seeking Help Scale (SSOSH; Vogel, Wade, & Haake, 2006) measured one’s internalized social stigma about help-seeking. SSOSH contains 10 items (e.g., “I would feel inadequate if I went to a therapist for psychological help”) on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Vogel et al. reported that the SSOSH differentiated between those who had and had not sought help in a two-month period and reported a reliability of .91. SSOSH has been found to be cross-culturally valid (Vogel et al., 2013) and has been used previously with Asian American students (Cheng, Kwan, & Sevig, 2013). We modified terminology in the current study by substituting “therapist” for “therapist/counselor/mental health professional.” Cronbach’s α was .86.
Intentions to seek help
The Intent to Seek Counseling Inventory (ISCI; Cash, Begley, McCown, & Weise, 1975) measured general help-seeking intentions. The ISCI consists of 17 items assessing likelihood of seeking counseling for certain problems. For each problem (e.g., academic problems, depression), respondents indicated their help-seeking intention on a scale of 1 (very unlikely) to 6 (very likely), with higher scores indicating greater general intention. Cash et al. provided evidence of construct validity of the ISCI as it predicted likelihood of seeking psychological help when therapists are presented as more or less attractive. ISCI has been used in previous research with Asian American students (Yakunina & Weigold, 2011). We utilized the total of all 17 items as a measure of general help-seeking intention. Cronbach’s α was .87.
The Intent to Seek Help Scale, modeled after Hammer and Vogel (2012), measured specific help-seeking intentions. This scale consists of 6 items (“I would intend to seek help from a mental health specialist (for example, a psychologist, a psychiatrist, a clinical social worker, a counselor, etc.) in the next 3 months”) rated from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate greater specific help-seeking intentions. Hammer and Vogel reported that this scale was constructed following Ajzen’s (2006) guidelines in assessing intentions of decision-based behaviors. It was significantly correlated with willingness and decision to seek help, with a reliability of .97. Cronbach’s α in the current study was .98.
Data Analysis
We used structural equation modeling in Mplus 7.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012), with each latent factor measured by two indicator variables. The measurement model was tested in each group separately to determine adequacy of model fit to each group’s data. To establish a baseline measurement model, we used multiple group confirmatory factor analysis, which examined whether latent constructs were comparable (Kline, 2011). To establish measurement invariance, a chi-square difference test compared the baseline measurement model with all factor loadings freely estimated against its equality constrained nested model. To determine racial/ethnic differences in help-seeking intentions, we examined mean differences on latent factors. To determine mediators of help-seeking intentions, we utilized an indirect effects multiple indicators multiple causes (MIMIC) model.
All estimation procedures used full information maximum likelihood (FIML) with raw data. Item-level missing data were less than 1%. FIML assumes multivariate normality, and transformations were not necessary as skewness and kurtosis were well within acceptable ranges (Kline, 2011). Adequacy of model fit was determined by root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) below .05 (Browne & Cudeck, 1993), comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) above .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) below .08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
Results
Table 1 displays the characteristics of the study sample by racial/ethnic group. Asian Americans indicated greater psychological distress than White Americans (d = .20) and less prior use of mental health services than White Americans (Cramer’s V = .21). Asian Americans attributed greater financial concerns (d = .24) and academic concerns (d = .19) to their distress in comparison to White Americans.
Table 2 displays descriptives and partial correlations of indicator variables in the model. We covaried prior use of services, which is a known predictor of future services use (Gulliver, Griffiths, & Christensen, 2010). Across both groups, lack of role limitations and better social functioning were negatively related to specific help-seeking intentions, whereas treatment credibility, self-disclosure utility, and general intentions were positively related. Self-disclosure risk and social stigma were negatively related to specific help-seeking intentions for White Americans but not Asian Americans.
Table 2.
Means, Standard Deviations, and Partial Correlations of Study Variables, Controlling for Prior Use of Services
White Americans (n = 261) | 1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Psychological distress | -- | ||||||||||
2. Role limitations (lack of) | −.39* | -- | |||||||||
3. Social functioning | −.48* | .55* | -- | ||||||||
4. Anxiety literacy | −.06 | .15* | .09 | -- | |||||||
5. Depression literacy | −.06 | .04 | .08 | .16* | -- | ||||||
6. Treatment credibility | .10 | −.02 | −.11 | .18* | .16* | -- | |||||
7. Self-disclosure utility | .09 | −.05 | −.12 | .13* | .18* | .54* | -- | ||||
8. Social stigma | .09 | −.02 | .01 | .02 | −.07 | −.06 | −.24* | -- | |||
9. Self-stigma | −.01 | .05 | .11 | −.02 | −.08 | −.30* | −.41* | .49* | -- | ||
10. Help-seeking intentions (gen.) | .15* | −.15* | −.19* | .05 | .18* | .44* | .40* | −.16* | −.39* | -- | |
11. Help-seeking intentions (sp.) | .30* | −.20* | −.21* | .04 | .08 | .36* | .37* | −.17* | −.33* | .45* | -- |
M | 1.49 | 54.02 | 70.07 | 1.73 | 2.78 | 3.34 | 3.52 | 2.29 | 2.48 | 2.31 | 3.29 |
SD | .59 | 42.14 | 22.98 | 1.44 | 1.52 | .89 | .86 | .54 | .79 | .55 | 1.19 |
Asian Americans (n = 395) | 1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Psychological distress | -- | ||||||||||
2. Role limitations (lack of) | −.32* | -- | |||||||||
3. Social functioning | −.46* | .46* | -- | ||||||||
4. Anxiety literacy | .01 | .04 | .08 | -- | |||||||
5. Depression literacy | −.06 | .05 | .07 | .22* | -- | ||||||
6. Treatment credibility | .09 | −.04 | −.09 | .14* | .14* | -- | |||||
7. Self-disclosure utility | .02 | −.11* | −.04 | .16* | .12* | .48* | -- | ||||
8. Social stigma | .22* | .05 | −.06 | .04 | .01 | −.10* | −.04 | -- | |||
9. Self-stigma | .18* | −.09 | −.08 | .03 | .05 | .01 | .01 | .01 | -- | ||
10. Help-seeking intentions (gen.) | .16* | −.09 | −.08 | .07 | .06 | .06 | .06 | .01 | −.07 | -- | |
11. Help-seeking intentions (sp.) | .32* | −.17* | −.33* | −.01 | .02 | .26* | .22* | −.04 | −.08 | .21* | -- |
M | 1.61 | 52.22 | 67.88 | 1.80 | 2.51 | 3.30 | 3.30 | 2.28 | 2.69 | 2.26 | 2.69 |
SD | .63 | 43.09 | 22.11 | 1.75 | 1.64 | .91 | .75 | .53 | .62 | .56 | 1.65 |
Note:
p < .05;
gen. = general; sp. = specific
Measurement Model
As shown on Table 3, the separate measurement models for the Asian American and White American groups fit the data relatively well. The multigroup baseline measurement model (Model 1) also demonstrated good fit to the data. All factor loadings of the indicators to their respective factors were significant at p < .001 (standardized factor loadings on Table 4). To establish measurement invariance, the baseline measurement model with all factor loadings freely estimated was compared against its equality constrained nested model (Model 2). A chi-square difference test between Models 1 and 2 was not significant, Δχ2 (5) = 9.10, p > .05, providing evidence of measurement invariance.
Table 3.
Summary of Goodness of Fit Indices
Model | χ2 (df) | CFI | TLI | RMSEA, 90% CI | SRMR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
White American model (N = 261) | 32.45 (26) | .99 | .98 | .03 [.00, .06] | .04 |
Asian American model (N = 395) | 44.37 (26) | .97 | .94 | .04 [.02, .06] | .04 |
Model 1: Multigroup baseline measurement model without equality constraints (N = 656) | 76.81 (52) | .98 | .96 | .04 [.02, .06] | .04 |
Model 2: Multigroup measurement invariance model (Model 1 with all factor loadings constrained equal) | 85.91 (57) | .97 | .96 | .04 [.02, .06] | .05 |
Multigroup structural model testing equivalence of factor means | 110.90 (62) | .96 | .94 | .05 [.03, .06] | .05 |
Single group MIMIC model testing indirect effects | 84.84 (32) | .95 | .92 | .05 [.04, .06] | .04 |
Notes: A chi-square difference test indicated p > .05 between Models 1 and 2; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CI = confidence interval; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual; MIMIC = multiple indicators multiple causes
Table 4.
Summary of Factor Loadings for Measurement Model
Factors and Indicator Variables | Standardized factor loadings (SE)
|
|
---|---|---|
White American model (N = 261) | Asian American model (N = 395) | |
Perceived severity | ||
Role limitations | .75 (.07) | .56 (.06) |
Social functioning | .79 (.07) | .85 (.07) |
Perceived susceptibility | ||
Anxiety literacy | .41 (.11) | .55 (.09) |
Depression literacy | .39 (.10) | .40 (.08) |
Perceived benefits | ||
Treatment credibility | .75 (.04) | .69 (.05) |
Self-disclosure utility | .75 (.04) | .71 (.05) |
Perceived barriers | ||
Self-stigma | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Social stigma | .46 (.05) | .47 (.04) |
Help-seeking intentions | ||
General intentions | .69 (.05) | .68 (.08) |
Specific intentions | .71 (.05) | .42 (.06) |
Notes. All factor loadings were significant a p < .001, with the exception of self-stigma, which was set to 1 for both groups.
Structural Model
Racial/ethnic differences in help-seeking intentions
In a multigroup framework, it is possible to test the relative equivalence or nonequivalence of latent factor means (Kline, 2011; Sörbom, 1974). After establishing measurement invariance, Model 2 (all factor loadings constrained equal) was modified such that the factor means, which were initially set to zero for both groups, were set to zero only for the reference group (White Americans). Intercepts were allowed to be freely estimated for model identification purposes (Byrne, 2012). Fit statistics are shown on Table 3. Results showed that Asian Americans had lower help-seeking intentions relative to White Americans, β (SE) = −.44 (.13), p = .001. Moreover, Asian Americans perceived less benefit, β (SE) = −.26 (.11), p = .02, and greater barriers, β (SE) = .33 (.09), p < .001, than White Americans.
HBM variables as mediators of help-seeking intentions
An indirect effects analysis was conducted using a MIMIC model, in which factors were regressed on a dichotomous indicator representing group membership (0 = White American; 1 = Asian American). We used a single group analysis as it met the assumption of measurement invariance (Kline, 2011). The model demonstrated adequate fit to the data (fit statistics on Table 3 and standardized path coefficients and standard errors on Figure 2). Consistent with the latent factor means analysis, Asian Americans perceived less benefit (p < .001) and greater barriers (p < .001) compared to White Americans. Path coefficients from racial/ethnic group to perceived severity and susceptibility were not significant. Perceived severity (p < .001), benefits (p < .001), and barriers (p = .045) were significantly related to help-seeking intentions.
Figure 2.
Summary of Standardized Path Coefficients and Standard Errors of the Indirect Effects MIMIC Model
Notes. * denotes individually significant path coefficients at p < .05, whereas bolded figures denote a significant indirect effect. The overall indirect effect was not significant, 95% CI [−.15, .01]; the specific indirect effect of perceived benefits was significant, 95% CI [−.15, −.01].
We examined whether individually significant paths were indicative of an overall significant indirect effect, which are considered significant when the 95% confidence interval (CI) does not include zero (Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The overall indirect effect was not significant (95% CI [−.15, .01]), but the specific indirect effect of perceived benefits was significant (95% CI [−.15, −.01]). Asian Americans perceived less benefit from help-seeking relative to White Americans, which partially explained differences in help-seeking intentions. Although Asian Americans perceived greater barriers than White Americans, and barriers were negatively related to help-seeking, this was not a significant indirect effect.
Discussion
We found that Asian Americans had lower help-seeking intentions compared to White Americans, despite reporting greater psychological distress. Asian Americans perceived relatively less benefit from help-seeking, which partially explained the group differences in help-seeking intentions. Although Asian Americans perceived relatively greater barriers, this did not significantly account for group differences in help-seeking intentions. Regardless of race/ethnicity, perceived severity, benefits, and barriers significantly influenced help-seeking intentions. Overall our results revealed both common and unique influences of help-seeking intentions across Asian and White American students.
Many Asian Americans may not consider mental health treatment to be that beneficial or helpful for a number of reasons. Culture influences the perceived meaning and expression of distress (Hwang, Myers, Abe-Kim, & Ting, 2008). For example, Asian cultures tend to emphasize a more holistic mind-body view, which may lead to a dual focus on the experience of physical and emotional symptoms of distress (Lin & Cheung, 1999). Treatment approaches viewed as targeting mostly the affective or cognitive symptoms of distress, such as psychotherapy, may not be perceived as credible ways to alleviate distress (Kung, 2004). Rather, help-seeking from those who focus on treating bodily symptoms, such as physicians and traditional healers, may be viewed as more credible (Uba, 1994).
Furthermore, individualistic ideals in psychotherapy (e.g., openly expressing emotions, seeking help outside of the family) may hold less value to individuals from Asian cultural backgrounds (B. S. K. Kim, Atkinson, & Yang, 1999; Sue & Morishima, 1982). Asian cultures place greater emphasis than Western cultures on secondary coping, such as acceptance of difficult situations, and less emphasis on primary coping, such as directly changing the source of the stressor (Hall, Hong, Zane, & Meyer, 2011; Lam & Zane, 2004). Emotional suppression and forbearance are common ways that Asian Americans handle stressors (Chang, 2014; Wei, Su, Carrera, Lin, & Yi, 2013). This difference in problem solving orientation (cf. Sue & Zane, 1987) may also help explain why Asian Americans tended to perceive therapy as being less credible and regarded self-disclosure to a mental health professional as less useful than White Americans.
There may be important cultural variations in the functional value of self-disclosure and what is considered an appropriate context for the disclosure of intimate information (B. S. K. Kim, 2007). Chen and Danish (2010) found that individuals who adhered to Asian values were more likely to disclose their distress to friends rather than to unrelated, trusted adults. The hierarchical nature of interpersonal relationships in Asian cultures often demands emotional modesty and restraint, especially in the presence of those with seniority and authority (Chen & Danish, 2010). In contrast, authority figures in treatment (i.e., mental health professionals) encourage clients to openly disclose their feelings and problems. Whereas this type of self-disclosure is considered important and therapeutic in mainstream cultural contexts, this may be perceived as culturally gauche in Asian contexts. These factors may help explain why Asian Americans tended to perceive less benefit from therapy relative to White Americans.
We also found that Asian Americans perceived greater barriers to help-seeking than White Americans, such that they reported greater stigma related to help-seeking. Prior findings have shown that Asian cultural values are positively related to social and self-stigma, which then have influences on willingness to seek counseling (Choi & Miller, 2014). Although our study did not assess differences in Asian cultural values, importantly, our results showed that relatively greater stigma-related barriers among Asian Americans did not significantly explain racial/ethnic differences in help-seeking intentions. Rather, stigma-related barriers were negatively related to help-seeking intentions regardless of race/ethnicity. These results are consistent with a previous study utilizing the HBM to examine help-seeking intentions of university students in Australia, which found that both perceived benefits and barriers influenced help-seeking intentions (O’Connor et al., 2014). Our findings suggest that reducing the stigma associated with help-seeking continues to be promising universal avenue to promote help-seeking.
We found that perceived severity, operationalized as lack of role limitations and better social functioning, significantly influenced help-seeking intentions, although we did not find racial/ethnic differences in severity. The experience of functional impairment alongside distress appears to drive help-seeking intentions, as impairment is an indicator of the severity of distress (Cauce et al., 2002). Prior findings have shown that educational concerns are the most common presenting problems among Asian American students seeking counseling, versus interpersonal or emotional concerns among White American students (Tracey, Leong, & Glidden, 1986). This is congruent with our findings, in which Asian Americans attributed greater psychological distress from academic concerns than White Americans. Individuals are most likely to seek help in areas that are most valued or salient to them (Tracey et al., 1986), and functional impairment in those areas may serve as an important indicator that help-seeking is warranted (Cauce et al., 2002). This suggests that efforts to promote timely help-seeking should consider emphasizing the gains in areas of functioning as a result of seeking help.
We did not find racial/ethnic differences in perceived susceptibility, nor did we find an effect on help-seeking intentions. A few possibilities may account for the null finding. The perceived susceptibility construct of the HBM originated from beliefs about being vulnerable to different types of physical conditions that warrant preventive health behaviors (e.g., mammography screening for cancer). Perceived susceptibility may not readily translate to other health behaviors that are outside the realm of prevention-related health behaviors, such as mental health help-seeking. In the current study, mental health literacy was the closest approximation of perceived susceptibility as it relates to mental health help-seeking. Although we used mental health literacy vignettes used by prior studies on similar samples, it may not have actually captured the essence of susceptibility, which may partially explain the null finding. We note that our null finding is consistent with O’Connor et al. (2014) that examined perceived susceptibility in terms of perceptions about developing mental health problems in the future. In our study, we examined recognition because we specifically sampled from individuals who already reported elevated psychological distress. Overall, we acknowledge our choice of construct as a limitation because mental health literacy is related to but nonetheless distinct from perceived susceptibility to mental health problems.
Our findings should be interpreted in light of other limitations. The study sample comprised of 63.5% of all respondents who completed the study, who scored 5+ on the K6. This percentage is relatively high but is very similar to a prior study that used the longer version of the K6 (i.e., K10) among university students (Stallman, 2010). The cutoff score was based on population-based research demonstrating the validity of the K6 in screening for moderate mental distress (Prochaska et al., 2012). Prochaska et al. reported that respondents with K6 scores in the moderate range had greater rates of mental health care utilization and impairment than respondents in the none/mild range. Population-based research also shows that college-aged individuals have higher levels of psychological distress than their non-college-aged counterparts (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2012), but the extent to which the K6 may be over-inclusive for this age group remains to be studied.
We assumed that help-seeking intention was an appropriate approximation of actual help-seeking. The cross-sectional methodology did not allow for directly testing this, but previous findings have shown that intentions significantly influence behaviors (Ajzen, Joyce, Sheik, & Cote, 2011). Another assumption was that help-seeking is driven by a conscious and rational decision-making process, but some have argued that these processes do not adequately capture the complexity of help-seeking (Pescosolido & Boyer, 2010). However, there are clear challenges to testing other types of models (e.g., Pescosolido’s (1992) Network Episode Model), and this investigation was an attempt to empirically study the underutilization phenomenon from a theoretically relevant stance. Clearly, there is a need to test alternative models that include, but are not based solely on a conscious and rational decision-making process.
In terms of measurement-related limitations, we applied the HBM to mental health help-seeking by selecting representative operational variables for each construct, which captured specific elements of the HBM. For perceived barriers, for example, we did not consider possible non-stigma-related barriers (e.g., difficulty of setting appointments). Prior research does show that college students have relatively low levels of help-seeking stigma (Eisenberg, Speer, & Hunt, 2012), which begs a careful consideration of what other factors are barriers to help-seeking. Related to measurement limitations, each latent factor was measured by the required minimum of two indicators. These indicators were informed by prior research on help-seeking, but it would be important from a measurement perspective to test models with three or more indicators per factor (Kline, 2011).
These findings are also limited to college students with access to mental health services. The relative lack of structural barriers was an advantage to the current study, because barriers to care among college students have more to do with beliefs and attitudes than access (Eisenberg et al., 2012). Thus, we were able to examine the psychological factors that impact help-seeking, but the extent to which these findings can generalize to community samples, in which structural barriers are an important consideration, is unknown. Moreover, the HBM construct of cues to action was not explicitly considered in our study. Consistent with Pescosolido (1992), it would be important for future research to incorporate the role of family, friends, and other social and outside influences on help-seeking intentions and behavior. Lastly, we examined intentions to seek help by using formal mental health services because it is known to be the major mental health disparity affecting Asian American populations, but we acknowledge that this constitutes one of many ways that individuals can deal with psychological distress.
Underutilization of needed mental health services continues to be a major mental health disparity affecting Asian Americans, and our findings have implications in promoting use of mental health services. Alongside reduction of help-seeking stigma, outreach efforts particularly highlighting the benefits of help-seeking appears to be a promising avenue for Asian Americans. This may involve enhancing overall perceptions of treatment credibility and the utility of self-disclosure, as well as emphasizing the functional gains as a result of help-seeking. It may also be worthwhile to incorporate information that more directly emphasizes the relationship between untreated psychological symptoms and eventual decrements in functioning. We also recognize the need for the development of culturally congruent ways to promote help-seeking. Our investigation, which has uncovered similarities and differences in help-seeking intentions among Asian Americans and White Americans in psychological distress, contribute to the growing body of literature aimed at eventually eliminating one of the greatest disparities in mental health.
Acknowledgments
Work on this study was supported in part by the Asian American Center on Disparities Research (NIMH P50 MH073511), the Asian American Psychological Association Dissertation Research Award, NIDA TRC Center Grant (P50 DA09253), and NIDA Training Grant (T32 DA007250).
The authors would like to thank Gordon C. Nagayama Hall and Shelley Blozis for their helpful comments and suggestions.
References
- Ajzen I. Constructing a theory of planned behavior questionnaire. 2006 Retrieved from http://people.umass.edu/aizen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf.
- Ajzen I, Joyce N, Sheikh S, Cote NG. Knowledge and the prediction of behavior: The role of information accuracy in the theory of planned behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 2011;33(2):101–117. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2011.568834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Akutsu PD, Lin CH, Zane NWS. Predictors of utilization intent of counseling among Chinese and White students: A test of the proximal-distal model. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1990;37(4):445–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.37.4.445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Alegria M, Chatterji P, Wells K, Cao Z, Chen CN, Takeuchi D, Meng XL. Disparity in depression treatment among racial and ethnic minority populations in the United States. Psychiatric Services. 2008;59(11):1264–1272. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.59.11.1264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bahrassa NF, Syed M, Su J, Lee RM. Family conflict and academic performance of first-year Asian American undergraduates. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2011;17(4):415–426. doi: 10.1037/a0024731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. New York, NY: Routledge; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Cash TF, Begley PJ, McCown DA, Weise BC. When counselors are heard but not seen: Initial impact of physical attractiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1975;22(4):273–279. doi: 10.1037/h0076730. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cauce AM, Domenech-Rodríguez M, Paradise M, Cochran BN, Shea JM, Srebnik D, Baydar N. Cultural and contextual influences in mental health help seeking: A focus on ethnic minority youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(1):44–55. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang EC. Cultural differences in psychological distress in Asian and Caucasian American college students: Examining the role of cognitive and affective concomitants. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2002;49(1):47–59. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.49.1.47. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chang J. The interplay between collectivism and social support processes among Asian and Latino American college students. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2014 doi: 10.1037/a0035820. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chen JC, Danish SJ. Acculturation, distress disclosure, and emotional self-disclosure within Asian populations. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2010;1(3):200–211. doi: 10.1037/a0020943. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng HL, Kwan KLK, Sevig T. Racial and ethnic minority college students’ stigma associated with seeking psychological help: Examining psychocultural correlates. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2013;60(1):98–111. doi: 10.1037/a0031169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi NY, Miller MJ. AAPI college students’ willingness to seek counseling: The role of culture, stigma, and attitudes. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2014;61(3):340–351. doi: 10.1037/cou0000027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coles ME, Coleman SL. Barriers to treatment seeking for anxiety disorders: Initial data on the role of mental health literacy. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27(1):63–71. doi: 10.1002/da.20620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan P. How stigma interferes with mental health care. American Psychologist. 2004;59(7):614–625. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durvasula R, Sue S. Severity of disturbance among Asian American outpatients. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health. 1996;2(1):43–51. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.2.1.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg D, Golberstein E, Gollust SE. Help-seeking and access to mental health care in a university student population. Medical Care. 2007;45(7):594–601. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31803bb4c1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg D, Speer N, Hunt JB. Attitudes and beliefs about treatment among college students with untreated mental health problems. Psychiatric Services. 2012;63(7):711–713. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201100250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golberstein E, Eisenberg D, Gollust SE. Perceived stigma and mental health care seeking. Psychiatric Services. 2008;59(4):392–399. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.59.4.392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldney RD, Fisher LJ, Wilson DH. Mental health literacy: An impediment to the optimum treatment of major depression in the community. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2001;64(2–3):277–284. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00227-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldney RD, Fisher LJ, Wilson DH, Cheok F. Mental health literacy of those with major depression and suicidal ideation: An impediment to help seeking. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2002;32(4):394–403. doi: 10.1521/suli.32.4.394.22343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gulliver A, Griffiths KM, Christensen H. Perceived barriers and facilitators to mental health help-seeking in young people: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(1):113. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall GCN, Hong JJ, Zane NWS, Meyer OL. Culturally competent treatments for Asian Americans: The relevance of mindfulness and acceptance-based psychotherapies. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2011;18(3):215–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2011.01253.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer JH, Vogel DL. Assessing the utility of the willingness/prototype model in predicting help-seeking decisions. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0030449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henshaw EJ, Freedman-Doan CR. Conceptualizing mental health care utilization using the health belief model. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2009;16(4):420–439. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01181.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hinson JA, Swanson JL. Willingness to seek help as a function of self-disclosure and problem severity. Journal of Counseling & Development. 1993;71(4):465–470. [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang WC, Myers HF, Abe-Kim J, Ting JY. A conceptual paradigm for understanding culture’s impact on mental health: The cultural influences on mental health (CIMH) model. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28(2):211–227. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson-Triche ME, Sullivan JG, Wells KB, Rogers W, Camp P, Mazel R. Depression and health-related quality of life in ethnic minorities seeking care in general medical settings. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;58(2):89–97. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00069-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janz NK, Becker MH. The health belief model: A decade later. Health Education and Behavior. 1984;11(1):1–47. doi: 10.1177/109019818401100101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jorm AF. Mental health literacy: Empowering the community to take action for better mental health. American Psychologist. 2012;67(3):231–243. doi: 10.1037/a0025957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Pollitt P. Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1997;32:143–148. doi: 10.1007/BF00794613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kearney LK, Draper M, Barón A. Counseling utilization by ethnic minority college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2005;11(3):272–285. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.11.3.272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand SLT, Zaslavsky AM. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(06):959–976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim BSK. Adherence to Asian and European American cultural values and attitudes toward seeking professional psychological help among Asian American college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2007;54(4):474–480. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.4.474. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kim BSK, Atkinson DR, Yang PH. The Asian Values Scale: Development, factor analysis, validation, and reliability. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1999;46(3):342–352. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.46.3.342. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kim PY, Lee D. Internalized model minority myth, Asian values, and help-seeking attitudes among Asian American students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2013 doi: 10.1037/a0033351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Komiya N, Good GE, Sherrod NB. Emotional openness as a predictor of college students’ attitudes toward seeking psychological help. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2000;47(1):138–143. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.47.1.138. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krumrei EJ, Newton FB, Kim E. A multi-institution look at college students seeking counseling: Nature and severity of concerns. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy. 2010;24(4):261–283. doi: 10.1080/87568225.2010.509223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kung WW. Cultural and practical barriers to seeking mental health treatment for Chinese Americans. Journal of Community Psychology. 2004;32(1):27–43. doi: 10.1002/jcop.10077. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lam AG, Zane NWS. Ethnic differences in coping with interpersonal stressors: A test of self-construals as cultural mediators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2004;35(4):446–459. doi: 10.1177/0022022104266108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leong FTL, Lau ASL. Barriers to providing effective mental health services to Asian Americans. Mental Health Services Research. 2001;3(4):201–214. doi: 10.1023/A:1013177014788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li H, Keshavan M. Mental health of Asian American populations: Challenges and opportunities. Asian Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;3(4):161–162. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2010.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin K, Cheung F. Mental health issues for Asian Americans. Psychiatric Services. 1999;50:774–780. doi: 10.1176/ps.50.6.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loya F, Reddy R, Hinshaw SP. Mental illness stigma as a mediator of differences in Caucasian and South Asian college students’ attitudes toward psychological counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2010;57(4):484–490. doi: 10.1037/a0021113. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McHorney CA, Ware JE, Raczek AE. The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36): II. Psychometric and clinical tests of validity in measure physical and mental health constructs. Medical Care. 1993;31(3):247–263. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199303000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer OL, Zane N, Cho YI, Takeuchi DT. Use of specialty mental health services by Asian Americans with psychiatric disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77(5):1000–1005. doi: 10.1037/a0017065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 2. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 1998–2012. [Google Scholar]
- Nam SK, Choi SI, Lee JH, Lee MK, Kim AR, Lee SM. Psychological factors in college students’ attitudes toward seeking professional psychological help: A meta-analysis. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2013;44(1):37–45. doi: 10.1037/a0029562. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor PJ, Martin B, Weeks CS, Ong L. Factors that influence young people’s mental health help-seeking behaviour: a study based on the Health Belief Model. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2014;70(11):2577–2587. doi: 10.1111/jan.12423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pescosolido BA. Beyond rational choice: The social dynamics of how people seek help. American Journal of Sociology. 1992;97(4):1096–1138. doi: 10.2307/2781508. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pescosolido BA, Boyer CA. Understanding the context and dynamic social processes of mental health treatment. In: Scheid TA, Brown TN, editors. Handbook for the Study of Mental Health: Social Contexts, Theories, and Systems. 2. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2010. pp. 420–438. [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JJ, Sung HY, Max W, Shi Y, Ong M. Validity study of the K6 scale as a measure of moderate mental distress based on mental health treatment need and utilization. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2012;21(2):88–97. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM. Why people use health services. The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1966;44(3):94–127. doi: 10.2307/3348967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM, Strecher VJ, Becker MH. Social learning theory and the health belief model. Health Education & Behavior. 1988;15(2):175–183. doi: 10.1177/109019818801500203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleeby JR. Health beliefs about mental illness: An instrument development study. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2000;24(2):83–95. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.24.2.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders SM. Applicants’ experience of the process of seeking therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training. 1993;30(4):554–564. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.30.4.554. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shea M, Yeh CJ. Asian American students’ cultural values, stigma, and relational self-construal: Correlates of attitudes toward professional help seeking. Journal of Mental Health Counseling. 2008;30(2):157. [Google Scholar]
- Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(4):422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sörbom D. A general method for studying differences in factor means and factor structure between groups. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 1974;27(2):229–239. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1974.tb00543.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stallman HM. Psychological distress in university students: A comparison with general population data. Australian Psychologist. 2010;45(4):249–257. doi: 10.1080/00050067.2010.482109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2010 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Mental health findings. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2012. HHS Publication No. (SMA) 11-4667. [Google Scholar]
- Sue S, Cheng JKY, Saad CS, Chu JP. Asian American mental health: A call to action. American Psychologist. 2012;67(7):532–544. doi: 10.1037/a0028900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sue S, Morishima JK. The mental health of Asian Americans. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Sue S, Zane N. The role of culture and cultural techniques in psychotherapy: A critique and reformulation. American Psychologist. 1987;42(1):37–45. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.42.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanner-Smith EE, Brown TN. Evaluating the Health Belief Model: A critical review of studies predicting mammographic and pap screening. Social Theory and Health. 2010;8(1):95–125. doi: 10.1057/sth.2009.23. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey TJ, Leong FT, Glidden C. Help seeking and problem perception among Asian Americans. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1986;33(3):331–336. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.33.3.331. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Uba L. Asian Americans: Personality patterns, identity, and mental health. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel DL, Armstrong PI, Tsai PC, Wade NG, Hammer JH, Efstathiou G, Topkaya N. Cross-cultural validity of the Self-Stigma of Seeking Help (SSOSH) scale: Examination across six nations. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2013;60(2):303–310. doi: 10.1037/a0032055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel DL, Wade NG, Haake S. Measuring the self-stigma associated with seeking psychological help. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2006;53(3):325–337. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.325. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel DL, Wester SR. To seek help or not to seek help: The risks of self-disclosure. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2003;50(3):351–361. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.50.3.351. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wang PS, Lane M, Olfson M, Pincus HA, Wells KB, Kessler RC. Twelvemonth use of mental health services in the united states: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):629–640. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care. 1992;30(6):473–483. doi: 10.2307/3765916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei M, Su JC, Carrera S, Lin SP, Yi F. Suppression and interpersonal harmony: A cross-cultural comparison between Chinese and European Americans. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2013;60(4):625–633. doi: 10.1037/a0033413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson CJ, Deane FP, Ciarrochi J, Rickwood D. Measuring help-seeking intentions: Properties of the general help seeking questionnaire. Canadian Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy. 2005;39(1) [Google Scholar]
- Yakunina ES, Weigold IK. Asian international students’ intentions to seek counseling: Integrating cognitive and cultural predictors. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2011;2(3):219–224. doi: 10.1037/a0024821. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Young CB, Fang DZ, Zisook S. Depression in Asian–American and Caucasian undergraduate students. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2010;125(1–3):379–382. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zane N, Sue S, Chang J, Huang L, Huang J, Lowe S, Lee E. Beyond ethnic match: Effects of client–therapist cognitive match in problem perception, coping orientation, and therapy goals on treatment outcomes. Journal of Community Psychology. 2005;33(5):569–585. doi: 10.1002/jcop.20067. [DOI] [Google Scholar]