Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Feb 2.
Published in final edited form as: J Rehabil Res Dev. 2015;52(5):577–590. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2014.09.0207

Foot placement control and gait instability among people with stroke

Jesse C Dean 1,*, Steven A Kautz 1
PMCID: PMC4737555  NIHMSID: NIHMS754673  PMID: 26437301

Abstract

Gait instability is a common problem following stroke, as evidenced by increases in fall risk and fear of falling. However, the mechanism underlying gait instability is currently unclear. We recently found that young, healthy humans use a consistent gait stabilization strategy of actively controlling their mediolateral foot placement based on the concurrent mechanical state of the stance limb. In the present work, we tested whether people with stroke (n = 16) and age-matched controls (n = 19) used this neuromechanical strategy. Specifically, we used multiple linear regressions to test whether (1) swing phase gluteus medius (GM) activity was influenced by the simultaneous state of the stance limb and (2) mediolateral foot placement location was influenced by swing phase GM activity and the mechanical state of the swing limb at the start of the step. We found that both age-matched controls and people with stroke classified as having a low fall risk (Dynamic Gait Index [DGI] score >19) essentially used the stabilization strategy previously described in young controls. In contrast, this strategy was disrupted for people with stroke classified as higher fall risk (DGI </=19), particularly for steps taken with the paretic limb. These results suggest that a reduced ability to appropriately control foot placement may contribute to poststroke instability.

Keywords: balance, biomechanics, foot placement, frontal plane, kinematics, motor control, muscle activity, stability, stroke, walking

INTRODUCTION

Decreased gait stability, defined for the present work as a reduced ability to safely and confidently meet the mechanical demands of bipedal walking, can limit functional mobility following a stroke. People with stroke are more likely to experience a fall than uninjured individuals of the same age [1]. Among people with stroke classified as community ambulators, these falls often occur during walking [25]. Many falls within the poststroke population are not the result of environmental factors such as contact with an obstacle but are instead caused by internal factors related to balance abilities [3]. Mobility following a stroke can also be limited by an increased fear of falling [6]. Even in the absence of an injurious fall, fear of falling can be present [2], potentially decreasing activity participation and leading to eventual decreases in physical capacity [67].

Due to the clear importance of decreased stability among people with stroke, several clinical measures have been used to quantify balance performance or perception in this population. The Berg Balance Scale (BBS) is likely the most common clinical measure used to assess poststroke balance performance [8]. While this tool does not evaluate stability during walking, it has been used to predict fall risk among people with stroke with mixed success [4,910]. The Dynamic Gait Index (DGI) was developed explicitly to test stability in various gait tasks [11] and has been validated in people with stroke [12]. Fear of falling among people with stroke has been quantified using the Activities-Specific Balance Confidence scale (ABC) and is associated with functional mobility beyond the effects of balance ability alone [13].

In addition to clinical measures of function, several laboratory-based measures have been developed with the goal of predicting an individual's fall risk. Kinematic variability during gait has been used as an indicator of stability, with increased stride time variability predicting increased fall risk in older adults [14] and lower DGI scores in people with stroke [15]. The patterns of whole-body angular momentum in the frontal plane may provide insight into gait instability, with fluctuations in this measure during single limb stance on the paretic side associated with BBS and DGI scores among people with stroke [16]. More complex indicators of gait stability, including Lyapunov exponents, Floquet multipliers, and long-term correlations, have also been associated with fall risk in an older population [1719]. However, the ability of these metrics to predict fall risk among individuals with stroke has not yet been tested.

While both clinical and laboratory-based measures may allow predictions of which patients are at an increased risk for falls, they do not provide insight into the underlying mechanism of dysfunction. Such measures do not reveal which elements of the typical gait stabilization strategy have been disrupted and do not necessarily provide clinicians with information that would allow them to select the most effective focus for intervention. For example, a DGI score of ≤19 may suggest that a patient is at an increased risk for falls [20]. However, this score does not indicate whether the underlying cause of the decreased stability is reduced strength, disrupted sensory feedback, an inability to appropriately integrate sensory information, or some other factor. Similarly, stride time variability could be measured and classified as higher than typical, but this information would not indicate the cause of the increased variability.

An alternative approach is to determine whether the typical gait stabilization strategies are disrupted following a stroke. To do so, we must first understand the control strategies that allow healthy humans to walk with a minimal fall risk and fear of falling. We recently identified a neuromechanical stabilization strategy that young, uninjured humans use to control mediolateral foot placement (FP) during both normal and mechanically perturbed walking [21]. Briefly, the mechanical state of the stance limb predictably influences simultaneous swing phase hip abductor activity in the contralateral limb; swing phase activity is higher when the center of mass (CoM) is displaced farther mediolaterally from the stance foot. Increases in swing phase hip abductor activity cause the swing foot to be placed more laterally, a relationship modulated by the mechanical state of the lower limb at the start of the swing phase. These results indicate that uninjured humans meet the mechanical demands of bipedal walking by actively controlling mediolateral FP based on their perception of the mechanical state of the stance limb [21]. Therefore, the typical stabilization strategy could potentially be disrupted by poor sensation, a reduced ability to integrate sensory information, or an inability to appropriately recruit the hip abductors during swing.

The purpose of the present study was to determine whether the previously observed mediolateral stabilization strategy is disrupted in individuals who have experienced a stroke. We investigated unperturbed walking in people with stroke with a low fall risk (DGI >19), people with stroke with a higher fall risk (DGI ≤19), and age-matched uninjured controls. In each of these populations, we quantified the relationship between the mechanical state of the stance limb and the simultaneous swing phase hip abductor activity. We also quantified the relationship between swing phase hip abductor activity and subsequent mediolateral FP. We anticipate that these results will be an early step in identifying a mechanism underlying reduced gait stability in some individuals who experience a stroke.

We hypothesized that the typical neuromechanical stabilization strategy would be disrupted in participants with stroke. Also, we hypothesized that a more severe disruption would be present in participants at potentially greater risk of falling, as measured by the DGI. Such results would provide clear motivation to identify the specific sensory sources and sensorimotor integration processes that humans use to control FP.

METHODS

Experimental Population

The present study involved people with chronic stroke and age-matched control participants. Hemiparetic participants conformed to the following inclusion criteria: hemiparesis secondary to a unilateral stroke; absence of significant lower-limb joint pain, limb contractures, or major sensory deficits; ability to independently walk >10 m on a level surface with an assistive device; and daily walking in the home. Potential participants were excluded if they had orthopedic or additional neurologic conditions beyond stroke or cardiovascular impairments contraindicative to walking. A total of 16 people with stroke and 19 age-matched controls participated in the experiments described.

Experimental Protocol

All participants performed overground walking trials in order to identify their self-selected overground walking speed and fastest-comfortable overground walking speed. Participants walked over a 14 ft instrumented mat (GAITRite; Sparta, New Jersey) used to quantify walking speed. For the two self-selected trials, participants were instructed to walk at their normal speed. For the two fastest-comfortable trials, participants were instructed to walk at the fastest speed at which they felt safe. The self-selected and fastest-comfortable speeds were calculated as the averages across these two trials for each condition.

Participants with stroke also walked on an instrumented treadmill (Tecmachine; Andezieux-Boutheon, France) at their self-selected and fastest-comfortable speeds for a series of trials. For treadmill trials, self-selected speed was identified by changing the treadmill speed until participants reported being comfortable. The fastest-comfortable speed was identified by increasing the treadmill speed until participants reported being uncomfortable walking any faster. In order to record comparable control data for a range of speeds, control participants walked on the treadmill at their self-selected speed, fastest-comfortable speed, and over a range of previously determined speeds (0.3, 0.6, 0.9, 1.2, 1.5, and 1.8 m/s). All trials lasted 30 s, and participants were provided with at least 1 min rest period between trials. During all tread-mill walking trials, participants wore a harness attached to an overhead rail. This harness did not support body weight but would have prevented a fall in case of a loss of balance.

Because walking speed may influence stabilization strategy [22] and mediolateral FP location [23], we restricted our analysis to trials within a relatively narrow range of speeds. For participants with stroke, we analyzed only treadmill trials in which participants walked at speeds of 0.8 m/s or higher (in at least one of the self-selected and fastest-comfortable trials), a value that has previously been related to the ability to effectively ambulate in the community [24]. For control participants, we analyzed only treadmill trials with speeds between 0.8 and 1.2 m/s, allowing us to match the speeds observed in the participants with stroke.

The participants with stroke were classified based on their DGI score, as measured by a licensed physical therapist. Participants with a DGI score of >19 were classified as low fall risk, while participants with a lower DGI score were classified as higher fall risk [20]. Table 1 lists characteristics of the control group, low fall risk stroke group, and higher fall risk stroke group. Participant age did not vary significantly across the three experimental groups (analysis of variance [ANOVA]: p = 0.34). The amount of time since experiencing a stroke was not significantly different between the low fall risk and higher fall risk groups (Wilcoxon rank sum test: p = 0.26).

Table 1.

Group characteristics of three experimental groups.

Characteristic Control Group Low Fall Risk Hemiparetic Group Higher Fall Risk Hemiparetic Group
No. of Participants 19 9 7
Sex (female/male) 13/6 2/7 1/6
Paretic Side (right/left) NA 6/3 4/3
Age, yr (mean ± SD) 62 ± 8 66 ± 7 59 ± 13
Time Poststroke, mo (median, range) NA 29 (19–116) 26 (9–201)
Dynamic Gait Index score (median, range) NA 18 (9–19) 23 (21–24)

Note: Normally distributed age data are reported as mean ± SD, while non-normally distributed time poststroke data are reported in terms of median and range.

NA = not applicable, SD = standard deviation.

Data Collection and Processing

Spatiotemporal gait characteristics were measured at a sampling rate of 100 Hz using reflective markers (Vicon; Denver, Colorado) placed on the sacrum and on the left and right heels. The sacrum marker was used as an estimate of mediolateral CoM position. This simple estimate does not consider motion of the limbs, but the timing of pelvis and CoM movements along a mediolateral axis is very similar [25]. For brevity, we will refer to this midline location as the mediolateral position of the CoM. For each step, mediolateral initial swing foot displacement was quantified with respect to the CoM, and mediolateral CoM displacement was quantified with respect to the stance foot (Figure 1). Marker velocities were calculated by low-pass filtering the position data at 20 Hz and then differentiating with respect to time. Anteroposterior velocities of the heel markers were used to identify heel-strike and toe-off events and thus to determine stance and swing phases of the gait cycle [26]. For use in subsequent regressions (described later), we calculated the mediolateral position and velocity of the swing foot relative to the CoM at the start of each step (initial swing foot displacement and velocity), the average mediolateral position and velocity of the CoM relative to the stance foot during the first half of the swing phase (CoM displacement and velocity), and the mediolateral location of the swing foot relative to the CoM at the end of the step (defined as mediolateral FP). Note that the magnitude of the initial swing foot velocity was largely determined by the CoM velocity because the mediolateral velocity of the foot is low at this point in the gait cycle.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mediolateral kinematic measures were quantified at various points in the gait cycle, as illustrated for a single step with the left lower limb (frontal plane view from the posterior side). At toe-off, initial swing foot displacement (xswf) was measured as the mediolateral distance from the center of mass (CoM) to the swing heel. During the first half of swing, mediolateral CoM displacement (xCoM) was quantified relative to the stance heel. Upon heel strike, mediolateral foot placement (FP) was measured as the distance from the CoM to the swing heel. For all steps, the direction toward the swing limb was considered positive.

Bilateral gluteus medius (GM) activity was measured using surface electromyography (EMG) electrodes (Konigsberg Electronics; Monrovia, California) sampled at 1,000 Hz. These EMG data were processed by band-pass filtering between 20 and 500 Hz, rectifying, and low-pass filtering at 50 Hz. EMG data were divided into strides, and the average EMG trace during a stride was calculated for each trial. EMG data were then normalized by the peak value during the average stride, which occurred during the stance phase (typically within the first 25% of a gait cycle). For use in our regressions, we calculated the average swing limb GM activity during the first half of swing and the average contralateral stance limb GM activity during the same time period.

Data Analysis and Statistics

Multiple ANOVAs were first performed to determine whether the three experimental groups (control group, low fall risk group, and higher fall risk group) differed in terms of self-selected overground walking speed, analyzed treadmill walking speed, and mediolateral FP location. Where appropriate, post hoc tests were performed (Tukey honestly significant difference). We interpreted p-values of less than 0.05 as significant.

To investigate the relationships between gait mechanics and the active control of FP, we performed a series of multiple linear regressions. Our first goal was to identify the factors that influence swing limb GM activity. For this analysis, the dependent variable in the regression was the average activity of the GM during the first half of the swing phase. The independent variables were the average mediolateral CoM displacement, average CoM velocity, and average contralateral stance limb GM activity during this same time period. We performed a multiple linear regression for each experimental group in order to identify significant associations. Our second goal was to identify the factors that influence mediolateral FP. For this analysis, the dependent variable in the regression was the mediolateral location of the swing foot with respect to the CoM at the time of heel-strike. The independent variables were the average GM activity during the first half of swing and the initial swing foot displacement and velocity relative to the CoM at the start of the swing phase. Again, a multiple linear regression was used to identify significant associations for each experimental group.

These regression methods were identical to those used in our previous study investigating mediolateral gait stabilization in young, uninjured individuals, with the exception of not including acceleration terms [21]. Specifically, we did not test for effects of mediolateral CoM acceleration on swing limb GM activity or for effects of initial swing foot acceleration on eventual mediolateral FP location. Our reason for not including these acceleration factors is that many participants exhibited a clear relationship between mediolateral displacement and acceleration. Due to the relatively small number of data points included in each regression (as few as 481 in comparison to the 5,000 included in our previous regressions), such collinearity can make the interpretation of regression results more difficult. Importantly, adding these acceleration factors back into the regressions did not influence the presence of significant associations with any of the other independent variables.

For control participants, data from both lower limbs were combined in the regressions because no bilateral differences were observed. For participants with stroke, data from the nonparetic and paretic limbs were analyzed separately. In order to focus on step-to-step differences in behavior rather than differences between individuals, a “dummy” indicator variable was included in each of the regressions to represent limb number. The data distribution within each trial was normalized by subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard deviation. This allowed clearer interpretation of the regression results because a larger regression coefficient would indicate a stronger relationship [27]. For each regression, the maximum variance inflation factor was less than 1.5 (mean = 1.06), well below the value of 10 often taken as a sign of problematic multicollinearity [28]. For all regressions, p-values of less than 0.05 were interpreted as significant. For both the low fall risk and higher fall risk groups, cases in which the significance of the regression coefficient differed from the control group were further investigated. Specifically, we determined whether the difference between the coefficients fell outside of the 95 percent confidence interval calculated from the standard error values [29]. This test revealed whether the two regression coefficients should be considered to be significantly different.

RESULTS

In control participants, the state of the stance limb influenced swing limb GM activity in the contralateral limb, which in turn influenced mediolateral FP. In hemiparetic participants with a low fall risk (DGI >19), the same neuromechanical stabilization strategy was largely retained. However, this strategy was clearly disrupted in hemiparetic participants with a higher fall risk (DGI ≤19), particularly for steps taken with the paretic limb.

Group Characteristics

Table 2 describes gait characteristics for the experimental groups. The control group had a significantly faster self-selected overground walking speed than either of the hemiparetic groups. Importantly, however, the walking speeds during the analyzed treadmill trials did not differ significantly between the three groups. Participants in the higher fall risk hemiparetic group placed their paretic limb significantly more laterally than they placed their nonparetic limb and significantly more laterally than the FP exhibited by the other experimental groups.

Table 2.

Basic gait behavior measures for three experimental groups.

Gait Measure Control Group Low Fall Risk Hemiparetic Group Higher Fall Risk Hemiparetic Group p-Value
Self-Selected Overground Walking Speed (m/s) 1.31 ± 0.20* 0.96 ± 0.11 0.79 ± 0.26 <0.001
Analyzed Treadmill Walking Speed (m/s) 1.06 ± 0.14 0.98 ± 0.24 1.03 ± 0.26 0.21
Mediolateral Foot Placement (%LL) 11.3 ± 2.0 <0.001
 Both Limbs 11.3 ± 2.0
 Nonparetic Limb 9.7 ± 1.9 11.0 ± 2.3
 Paretic Limb 11.8 ± 2.7 16.9 ± 4.1*
Analyzed Steps (n) 3,037 NA
 Nonparetic Limb 707 481
 Paretic Limb 705 481

Note: Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation where appropriate. For control group, 57 total walking trials were performed between 0.8 and 1.2 m/s, meeting our walking speed requirement, and were included in analysis. For low fall risk group, 16 self-selected speed trials and 13 fastest-comfortable speed trials met walking speed requirement. For higher fall risk group, 13 self-selected speed trials and 9 fastest-comfortable speed trials met this requirement. Where reported, p-values were calculated using analysis of variance.

*

Indicates value significantly larger than all other comparison groups (p < 0.05, Tukey post hoc test).

%LL = percentage of lower-limb length, NA = not applicable.

Swing Phase Gluteus Medius Activity

In the control group, swing limb GM activity was associated with the simultaneous state of the contralateral stance limb (Figure 2(a)). Swing limb GM activity was most strongly associated with mediolateral CoM displacement (p < 0.001), with higher GM activity during steps in which the CoM was farther from the contralateral stance foot. Swing limb GM activity was also positively associated with CoM velocity (p = 0.03), with greater activity when the CoM was moving more slowly toward the stance foot. Finally, swing limb GM activity was positively associated with simultaneous GM activity in the contralateral stance limb (p < 0.001), with increased swing phase activity during steps with higher contralateral stance phase activity.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Swing phase gluteus medius activity was significantly associated with several metrics quantifying the state of the stance limb. The regression coefficients are illustrated for (a) the control group, (b) the low fall risk hemiparetic group, and (c) the higher fall risk hemiparetic group. For all panels, the bar represents the regression coefficient, the error bar represents standard error, and asterisks (*) indicate a significant (p < 0.05) association. For purposes of visual comparison, the control group regression coefficients are indicated in panels (b) and (c) using horizontal dashed lines. Exclamation marks (!) indicate a significant difference between the indicated regression coefficient and the matching regression coefficient in the control group. xCoM = center of mass displacement, vCoM = center of mass velocity, GMst = stance limb gluteus medius activity.

The factors that influence swing limb GM activity in the low fall risk hemiparetic group were largely similar to those in control participants (Figure 2(b)). For steps taken with the nonparetic limb, swing limb GM activity was positively associated with mediolateral CoM displacement (p < 0.001), mediolateral CoM velocity (p = 0.03), and simultaneous stance limb GM activity in the contralateral limb (p = 0.009). For steps taken with the paretic limb, swing limb GM activity was positively associated with CoM displacement (p < 0.001) and stance limb GM activity in the contralateral limb (p < 0.001).

In contrast, the factors that influence swing limb GM activity in the higher fall risk hemiparetic group differed from those in control participants (Figure 2(c)). For nonparetic steps, only mediolateral CoM displacement was significantly (p = 0.001) associated with swing limb GM activity. The differences from control behavior were more apparent for steps taken with the paretic limb. Mediolateral CoM displacement was not significantly associated with swing limb GM activity, with a regression coefficient significantly smaller than for controls. Mediolateral CoM velocity was negatively correlated (p = 0.03) with swing limb GM activity, indicating greater GM activity during steps in which the CoM was moving more quickly toward the stance foot. This negative correlation was significantly different than the positive correlation present in the control group. A trend for a positive association between swing limb GM activity and contralateral stance limb GM activity nearly achieved the level of significance (p = 0.051).

Mediolateral Foot Placement

In the control group, mediolateral FP was influenced by a combination of swing limb GM activity and the initial mechanical state of the swing limb (Figure 3(a)). FP was positively associated with swing limb GM activity (p < 0.001), with increased hip abductor activity preceding more lateral FP. FP was negatively associated with the mediolateral initial swing foot displacement relative to the CoM (p < 0.001), with the foot placed more medially for steps in which it began the swing phase farther from the CoM. Similarly, FP was negatively associated with the initial mediolateral velocity of the swing foot relative to the CoM (p < 0.001), with the foot placed more medially for steps in which the CoM was initially moving more quickly toward the contralateral stance limb.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Mediolateral foot placement was significantly associated with various combinations of swing phase gluteus medius activity (GMsw) and metrics quantifying the initial mechanical state of the swing limb. The regression coefficients are illustrated for (a) the control group, (b) the low fall risk hemiparetic group, and (c) the higher fall risk hemiparetic group. For all panels, the bar represents the regression coefficient, the error bar represents standard error, and asterisks (*) indicate a significant (p < 0.05) association. For comparison purposes, the control group regression coefficients are indicated in panels (b) and (c) using horizontal dashed lines. Exclamation marks (!) indicate a significant difference between the indicated regression coefficient and the matching regression coefficient in the control group. xswf = initial swing foot displacement, vswf = initial swing foot velocity.

In the low fall risk hemiparetic group, the factors that influence FP location varied between the nonparetic and paretic limbs (Figure 3(b)). For steps taken with the nonparetic limb, FP was associated with swing limb GM activity (p = 0.001), initial swing foot displacement (p = 0.04), and initial swing foot velocity (p = 0.002), just as in the control group. For paretic steps, FP was only significantly associated with swing limb GM activity (p < 0.001). The magnitude of the regression coefficient for the initial foot velocity was significantly smaller in this group compared with control participants.

The factors that influence mediolateral FP in the higher fall risk hemiparetic group differed substantially from those in the control group (Figure 3(c)). For nonparetic steps, only the initial foot velocity was significantly (p = 0.002) associated with the eventual FP location. For paretic steps, only the initial foot displacement was significantly (p = 0.006) associated with FP. The positive association between initial displacement and final FP indicates that the foot was placed more laterally for steps in which it began the swing phase farther from the CoM, significantly different from the negative association observed in control participants. Additionally, the lack of an association between swing foot velocity and FP location differed significantly from controls.

Typical relationships between mediolateral gait motion and paretic limb swing limb GM activity are illustrated for representative participants from the low fall risk and higher fall risk groups (Figure 4). For participants categorized as low fall risk, steps in which the CoM was farther from the stance foot (increased CoM displacement) typically also exhibited clear bursts of paretic swing limb GM activity during swing and more lateral subsequent FP (Figure 4(a)). This mechanics-dependent use of GM activity to modulate FP appeared to be disrupted in the higher fall risk group. Rather than exhibiting a single burst of GM activity early in swing, participants in this group often activated the GM at less predictable points in the gait cycle (Figure 4(b)). These bursts tended to be present when the CoM moved more quickly toward the stance foot and did not consistently influence the subsequent mediolateral FP.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Mediolateral marker positions and paretic limb gluteus medius (GM) activity over the course of a stride are illustrated for typical walking trials. Each line represents the average of five strides (from paretic heel-strike to next paretic heel-strike). High-activity strides were those with the greatest paretic limb GM activity during the first half of swing (shaded area), and low-activity strides were those with the lowest paretic limb GM activity during this period. (a) For the low fall risk participant, the center of mass (CoM) displacement (xCoM) from the nonparetic stance foot was larger during high-activity strides, which ended with the paretic limb being placed more laterally (increased foot placement distance). (b) For the higher fall risk participant, the CoM moved more quickly toward the stance foot (indicated by steeper slope of CoM velocity [vCoM]) for the first half of swing during high-activity strides. The presence of increased paretic limb GM activity did not cause the paretic limb to be placed more laterally. EMG = electromyography.

DISCUSSION

The mechanical demands of bipedal gait require humans to actively ensure their stability. We have previously proposed that neurologically intact humans stabilize their walking patterns by sensing the mechanical state of their stance lower limb and use this information to drive active muscular control of their mediolateral FP [21]. In the current study, this neuromechanical stabilization strategy was found to be present among uninjured older adults. As hypothesized, however, this strategy was disrupted in participants with stroke classified as having an increased fall risk.

The strategy for active FP observed in the control group (of similar age as the participants with stroke) was qualitatively similar to that previously reported for young, uninjured participants [21]. Swing limb GM activity was most strongly associated with the mediolateral position of the CoM with respect to the contralateral stance foot and was more weakly associated with CoM velocity and contralateral stance limb GM activity. This result supports the proposal that older adults actively stabilize their gait based on their perception of CoM mechanics. The magnitude of the swing limb GM activity was associated with the subsequent mediolateral FP at the end of the step, indicating the presence of useful active control. As in the young [21], final FP location was also associated with the initial displacement and velocity of this foot relative to the CoM at the start of the step. This finding can likely be attributed to passive pendular behavior of the swing limb, with the leg's initial mechanical state likely influencing the mediolateral trajectory during swing [21,30]. The observed mechanism of actively controlling FP based on the mechanical state of the stance limb supports the predictions of previous model simulations [31]. Our results also provide evidence for a neuromechanical mechanism that humans may use to accomplish the apparent goal of maintaining a certain margin of stability between their extrapolated CoM (a metric combining CoM position and CoM velocity) and their FP [3233].

Among participants with stroke classified as low fall risk (DGI >19), the FP strategy was largely similar to that in control participants. For steps taken with both the nonparetic and paretic limbs, swing limb GM activity was greater when the CoM was farther from the stance foot and the stance limb GM activity was more active. Increased swing limb GM activity was followed by more lateral FP, indicating that the mechanism of actively controlling FP location is essentially retained within this group. However, the influence of the swing limb's initial mechanical state on its subsequent FP location was reduced for steps taken with the paretic limb. This apparent reduction in the importance of passive pendular mechanics may reflect an inability to allow the paretic limb to swing freely while stepping forward, possibly due to abnormal joint torques attributable to dyscoordination in muscle activation [34].

While the control and low fall risk groups appeared to base their swing limb GM activity on similar stance limb characteristics, this strategy was disrupted in the higher fall risk group (DGI ≤19). For steps taken with the nonparetic limb, swing limb GM activity was most strongly influenced by the CoM displacement from the stance foot, as in control participants. However, this relationship between CoM mechanics and swing limb control was altered for steps with the paretic limb. The CoM displacement from the stance foot did not influence GM activity, with a significantly weaker association than in control participants. The lack of a clear effect of CoM displacement may be explained by poststroke disruptions in sensory accuracy [35], sensorimotor integration [36], or precise muscular activation [37]. An inability to either develop an accurate perception of CoM and swing limb location or appropriately modulate swing limb GM activity could prevent the use of the typical stabilization strategy. Additionally, swing limb GM activity was greater during steps in which the CoM was moving more quickly toward the stance foot, opposite to the relationship in controls. Speculatively, this response may be attributed to a balance reaction in which the swing limb is used to control frontal plane angular momentum [38]. Such a balance reaction is reminiscent of behavior during unipedal standing, in which balance can be maintained by repositioning the limb not in contact with the ground [3839], as evidenced by bursts of GM activity in this limb immediately following pushes of the CoM toward the stance limb [40]. People with stroke are often overly reliant on their nonparetic limb musculature for stability [41] and walk with their CoM closer mediolaterally to their nonparetic limb than their paretic limb [42]. The present results are consistent with the higher fall risk group attempting to maintain their gait stability by balancing their CoM primarily over the nonparetic limb while placing their paretic limb quite laterally (Table 2) rather than following the typical strategy of controlling FP location to accelerate the CoM toward the midline with each step [33].

In addition to altered activation of the GM, the factors that influence mediolateral FP location also differed in the higher fall risk group. Unlike in control participants and the low fall risk group, swing limb GM activity was not significantly associated with FP location. The reduced ability of GM activity to cause more lateral FP may be due to the decreased hip abduction strength of many patients with stroke [43] or an inability to appropriately produce independent GM contractions and isolated frontal plane hip torques [4345]. Within the higher fall risk group, the initial mechanical state of the paretic swing limb also had an altered relationship with its eventual FP location. Specifically, the paretic limb's FP was positively related to its position at the start of the step; if the foot began the step relatively laterally, it also ended with the step being placed relatively laterally. Rather than FP being determined by a combination of passive lower-limb mechanics and active control [21], it seems that higher fall risk individuals approximately maintained the relative mediolateral position of the paretic limb across the course of a step. This finding may indicate that these individuals were unable to appropriately vary their FP location in response to step-to-step changes in the mechanical demands of the task, thereby reducing the “good variance” thought to typically contribute to mediolateral gait stabilization [4647]. Instead, people with stroke with a reduced ability to accurately position their paretic limb may simply target a FP location that successfully prevented a fall during the previous paretic step.

The present results are an early step toward a mechanistic understanding of gait instability poststroke. Such an understanding could allow for the development of more effective approaches to stroke rehabilitation. Based on previous [21] and current results, the typical neuromechanical stabilization strategy during gait appears to involve the perception of CoM mechanics. To determine how this strategy may be disrupted following a stroke, it will be necessary to identify the sources of sensory information used to form this perception, possibly including proprioceptive feedback from the hip, knee, or ankle [4850]; feedback of cutaneous pressure from the stance foot [51]; visual feedback [52]; or vestibular feedback [53]. An understanding of the relevant sensory sources could motivate the introduction of new therapy methods, such as training to improve proprioception [54]. A second component of the typical stabilization strategy is the modulation of mediolateral FP through active control of the swing limb. While in its early stages, gait training that includes practice stepping to targets may improve mobility by allowing more accurate FP [55].

An understanding of the mechanism underlying decreased gait stability following a stroke is particularly important given the recent failure of several intervention methods to reduce fall risk. In randomized controlled trials, the rate of falls among people with stroke was not preferentially reduced by either a multifactorial falls prevention program [56] or a task-related balance and strengthening program [57]. These results were surprising given the previous success of similar interventions in improving balance among uninjured older adults [5859]. It is possible that the cause of increased fall risk differs between these two populations [3]. Indeed, the present results suggest that a neuromechanical strategy for mediolateral stabilization initially suggested in a young population was essentially retained in an older control group but was disrupted among people with stroke thought to be at an increased risk for falls [21]. Additionally, an understanding of the factors contributing to gait instability may not directly follow from investigations of quiet standing posture. From a basic mechanical perspective, mediolateral stabilization strategies that are effective during standing posture (e.g., resisting perturbations solely by increasing nonparetic limb abduction or adduction torques) would not be effective during gait, in which the paretic limb is forced to play a role [41].

Due to our focus on walking stability, participants were classified using their DGI score, a metric intended to quantify various aspects of balance during gait [11]. While DGI score has been related to self-reported falls among older adults, the sensitivity (59%) and specificity (64%) values were only moderate [20]. Among people with stroke, the DGI has been validated with respect to other measures of balance (e.g., BBS, ABC) but has not yet been shown to predict fall risk [12]. While the recently introduced modified DGI appears to have improved psychometric properties, this metric should still not be considered a gold standard for quantifying poststroke gait stability [60]. Despite the limitations of our classifications based on DGI score, we observed clear differences in walking behavior between the low fall risk and higher fall risk groups. Future work based on the present results may allow us to better understand the causes of gait instability that are currently only hinted at by clinical measures such as the DGI.

Our ability to extrapolate the present results to a more general understanding of poststroke falls is limited by several characteristics of the experiments. We restricted our analysis to relatively fast walking speeds (≥0.8 m/s) so the observed changes in gait stabilization strategy may not be relevant for individuals who walk more slowly. However, even people with stroke with an ability to walk faster than 0.8 m/s have a higher incidence of falls than uninjured older adults [57,61]. In the present analysis, we focused on a FP strategy during unperturbed walking. While our approach will not provide insight into the response to mechanical perturbations like trips or slips, many poststroke falls are due to “internal perturbations” that can be caused by the type of inaccurate control investigated here [3]. Finally, the muscle activity of the hip adductors was not quantified in the present study. Swing phase activity of the adductors may contribute to mediolateral FP, although the effects of adductor magnus activity in young controls were much smaller than the effects of GM activity [21].

Our use of regressions to quantify relationships between measures of gait kinematics and muscle activity is also a limitation. Most importantly, the reported significant associations cannot provide insight into causality. For example, the positive association between CoM displacement and swing limb GM activity in control participants does not necessarily mean that increased displacements cause increased muscle activity. However, our previous experiments that used perturbations to alter CoM displacement and subsequently quantified GM activity have provided stronger evidence for such a causal relationship [21]. The variable number of steps included in each regression (ranging from 481 to 3,037 steps) affects the probability that each regression would identify significant associations. A reduced number of steps included in the regression for the higher fall risk group likely explains why some associations reported as significant among controls did not reach significance in this group (see “GMst” in Figure 2(c)). Despite this limitation, several clear, statistically significant differences between gait behaviors were identified between groups.

CONCLUSIONS

Poststroke participants classified as having a higher fall risk exhibited an altered gait stabilization strategy, particularly for steps taken with the paretic lower limb. This subgroup of participants with stroke did not follow the typical strategy of actively controlling their mediolateral FP in response to step-by-step variation in CoM mechanics. The present results have the potential to serve as an early step in identifying mechanistic causes of limited gait stability poststroke and could lead to novel rehabilitation strategies.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Funding/Support: This material was based on work supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) Rehabilitation Research and Development Service (grant 1 IK2 RX000750) and the National Institutes of Health (grants HD46820 and GM109040).

Abbreviations

ABC

Activities-Specific Balance Confidence scale

ANOVA

analysis of variance

BBS

Berg Balance Scale

CoM

center of mass

DGI

Dynamic Gait Index

EMG

electromyography

FP

foot placement

GM

gluteus medius

VA

Department of Veterans Affairs.

Footnotes

Author Contributions: Study concept and design: J. C. Dean, S. A. Kautz.

Acquisition of data: S. A. Kautz.

Analysis and interpretation of data: J. C. Dean, S. A. Kautz.

Processing of data: J. C. Dean.

Drafting of manuscript: J. C. Dean, S. A. Kautz.

Study supervision: S. A. Kautz.

Financial Disclosures: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Institutional Review: Recruitment and informed consent procedures were approved by the University of Florida Institutional Review Board (protocol 46–2004).

Participant Follow-Up: The authors do not plan to inform participants of the publication of this study. Participants initially signed an informed consent form stating that the results of this study would be published.

Publisher's Disclaimer: The views expressed by the authors are their own and do not necessarily reflect the official policy of the VA or the U.S. Government.

REFERENCES

RESOURCES