Abstract
Aggressive behaviors have become a major public health problem, and early-onset aggression can lead to outcomes such as substance abuse, antisocial personality disorder among other issues. In recent years, there has been an increase in research in the molecular and genetic underpinnings of aggressive behavior, and one of the candidate genes codes for the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT). COMT is involved in catabolizing catecholamines such as dopamine. These neurotransmitters appear to be involved in regulating mood which can contribute to aggression. The most common gene variant studied in the COMT gene is the Valine (Val) to Methionine (Met) substitution at codon 158. We will be reviewing the current literature on this gene variant in aggressive behavior.
Keywords: Aggression, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), catechol-0-methyltransferase (COMT), genetics, schizophrenia, Val158Met (rs4680).
INTRODUCTION
Evolutionarily, aggression serves important roles in securing land and resources to increase an individual’s chances of survival. It also helps parents to protect their offspring from intruders, predators, and hostile environments. However, excessive or inappropriate aggression can have devastating consequences [1]. For instance, in children aggression has been found to be associated with substance abuse, antisocial personality disorder, and violent outbursts [2]. Aggressive behavior is often observed in mental illnesses such as schizophrenia [3], attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, borderline personality disorder, among others. Therefore, aggressive behavior is a serious public health issue, and research into its etiology and how it manifests can provide information on its treatment and prevention.
The mechanism underlying the development of aggressive behavior is unclear. Environmental factors seem to play a prominent role. For example, maternal stressors including exposure to alcohol, malnutrition, tobacco are associated with increased violent or criminal activities in the offspring [4]. Early-life stress has also been associated with the development of aggressive behaviors [5]. Aggressive behavior has also been shown to have a strong genetic component. Twin and adoption studies have shown aggressive behavior to be heritable [6-9]. Genetic factors explain as high as 40-50% of the risk for aggressive behavior [8, 10]. Craig and Halton [11] found that various genes have been associated with aggression, including dopamine system genes such as catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT). This review is to summarize the current status of research conducted on how the functional Val158Met variant in the COMT gene has been implicated in aggressive behaviors in various psychiatric populations. It will also discuss limitations in the field, and offer a number of avenues that may help to resolve the mixed findings in the literature regarding this gene marker and aggressive behavior.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Literature search was conducted using PubMed and Google Scholar. Key words used included “COMT AND aggression”, “COMT AND Val158Met AND aggression”, “COMT AND violen*”, and “COMT AND externalizing”. Additional papers were collected from the reference lists of the articles from the primary internet searches. The review will begin with an introduction to COMT. We will be summarizing findings from association studies of COMT Val158Met with aggressive behavior, either by itself or in the context of schizophrenia, ADHD, and other psychiatric disorders. Studies that investigated the COMT Val158Met in aggression are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Gene association studies between COMT Val158Met and aggression.
Sample Characteristics | Diagnosis | Assessment Tools for Aggression |
Findings | Refs. |
---|---|---|---|---|
149 youths, 58% males (6-18yo) |
N/A | Mother-rated CBCL | Met- > ValVal (aggression scale (p=0.016), direct aggression (p=0.007) and relational aggression (p=0.041)) | Albaugh et al. [24] |
379 male air force recruits | N/A | Aggression Questionnaire | Not significant | Avramopoulos et al. [26] |
144 majority European, 72% males (6-16yo) | N/A | Pervasive aggression (CBCL and TRF) vs healthy adult controls | Not significant | Hirata et al. [27] |
37, 86% males, mixed ethnicity (40.6yo) | Schizophrenia | RAD | MetMet higher risk than ValVal (p=0.003) | Strous et al. [33] |
55, 62% males, European | Schizophrenia or schizo-affective | 27 with history of multiple assaults vs 28 non-violent patients | Met associated with risk (p=0.02) | Lachman et al. [35] |
92, 63% males, Jewish (39.1yo) |
Schizophrenia | 30 homicidal vs 62 non-violent | Trend for Met associated with risk (p<0.1) | Kotler et al. [36] |
122, 77% males, Jewish | Schizophrenia | LHA | MetMet with higher scores than Val- (p=0.005; aggression p=0.007; self-directed aggression p=0.033) |
Strous et al. [37] |
168 male East Asian | Schizophrenia | OAS | MetMet associated with highest scores on physical aggression against other people (p=0.02) |
Han et al. [39] |
132 male East Asian | First-episode schizophrenia | OAS | MetMet associated with highest scores on physical aggression against other people and total OAS scores (both p<0.001) | Han et al. [41] |
80, 51% males, European | Schizophrenia | OAS | MetMet with higher total OAS scores than ValVal (p=0.045) | Tosato et al. [42] |
180 European (75.6% males) |
Schizophrenia | OAS | ValVal with higher total scores than Met- (p<0.05); lowest scores in ValMet (p<0.01) | Jones et al. [43] |
330 European (180 from Jones et al.) |
Schizophrenia | OAS | Not significant | Zammit et al. [44] |
All males (18-65yo) | Schizophrenia | 93 Homicidal vs 100 non-homicidal |
Not significant | Hong et al. [45] |
198, 47% males, East Asian |
Schizophrenia | Aggressive behavior or gestures 2wks before hospital admission | Not significant | Liou et al. [46] |
70 males various ethnicities from South Africa | Schizophrenia | 40 violent vs 30 non-violent (OAS) | Not significant | Koen et al. [47] |
165, 58% males, East Asian (38.4yo) | Schizophrenia | 61 aggressive vs 104 non-aggressive (MOAS) | Met- with higher scores in verbal aggression within aggressive patients (p=0.032) | Kim et al. 2008 [48] |
East Asian, 59% males (36.8yo) | Schizophrenia | 99 criminal/ 133 homicidal patients vs healthy controls | Not significant | Koh et al. [49] |
Chinese, all males | Schizophrenia | 252 violent vs 332 non-violent patients; MOAS | Not significant with Val158Met; haplotypic association |
Gu et al. [115] |
516, 77% males, Brazilian majority European | ADHD | CD or ODD | ValVal > Met- (p=0.016; OR=1.58; CI95%=1.07–2.35) | Salatino-Oliveira et al. [61] |
240, 89% males, European children | ADHD | Comorbid CD | ValVal associated with risk | Thapar et al. [56] |
Sample Characteristics | Diagnosis | Assessment Tools for Aggression |
Findings | Refs. |
(1) 241 British (2) 2232 British birth cohort (3) 1037 New Zealand cohort |
ADHD | (1) CD symptoms in ADHD; (2) CBCLs in general population and ADHD; (3) adolescents followed to adulthood | (1) ValVal with higher CD symptom scores than Met1 (p=0.05); (2) ValVal with pervasive aggression in ADHD subgroup (p=0.04); (3) ValVal with higher scroes on antisocial behavior index than Met- in ADHD subgroup (p=0.03) |
Caspi et al. [57] |
444, 58% males, mainly European (6-55yo) | ADHD | Comorbid CD | Not significant with CD; ValVal with more aggressive subtype symtpoms than Met- (p=0.03) | Monuteaux et al. [58] |
171 East Asian boys | ADHD | Comorbid ODD | ValVal associated with ODD (p=0.019) | Qian et al. [59] |
4365, 51% males, European (7.5yo) | ADHD | Antisocial behavior symptoms | ValVal associated with highest loading of antisocial behavior symptoms (p<0.001) | Langley et al. [60] |
166, 84% males, German (6-13yo) | ADHD | Lifetime history of comorbid CD | Not significant with CD; Met- associated with highest ADHD symptom severity | Palmason et al. [62] |
446, 53% males, youths mostly Italian ancestry (10-14yo) | N/A | CBCL | Not significant with conduct behavioral problems; ValVal children in families of low socioeconomic status with highest ADHD symptom severity | Nobile et al. [63] |
94 Finnish/ 54 US | Schizophrenia/ Schizo-affective | Suicide attempt | Met allele associated with risk (p=0.015); male-specific association (p=0.008) | Nolan et al. [67] |
393, 79% males, Croatian | Alcoholism | Suicide attempt | MetMet associated with risk in males (p<0.001) | Nedic et al. [68] |
477, 42% males, German | N/A | 149 suicide attempters vs 328 healthy controls; STAI | Met allele over-represented in violent attempters vs non-violent attempters; Val allele associated with state anger, anger-in, and Met allele associated with anger-out | Rujescu et al. [69] |
107 Swiss/ 320 French (42% males, 18-75yo) | 427 suicide attempters vs 185 controls | Val associated with risk (p=0.019) | Baud et al. [73] | |
305 small nuclear families; 38% males, mainly European | Bipolar disorder | Suicide attempt | Not significant | De Luca et al. [75] |
270, 73% males, mainly European | Schizophrenia | 92 suicide attempters | Not significant | De Luca et al. [74] |
486 | Mood disorder | 201 suicide attempters | Not significant | Zalsman et al. [76] |
163, 69% males, East Asian | N/A | Suicide completers | ValVal male-specific association with low risk (p=0.016) | Ono et al. [79] |
German, 50% males | BPD | 161 cases vs 156 healthy controls | MetMet associated with risk (adjusted p=0.034) | Tadic et al. [81] |
Italian | BPD | 19 cases vs 36 controls | Met associated with risk (p=0.02) | Lazzaretti et al. [82] |
(1) 99 US young adults from low-income families; (2) 136 Hungarian; 29% males | (1) N/A; (2) mood disorder | BPD | Not significant | Nemoda et al. [83] |
296 Chinese female | Heroine dependence | 61 BPD vs 235 without BPD | Not significant | Yang et al. [84] |
Male inmates convicted of violent impulsive crimes | APD | 47 cases vs 43 healthy controls | Not significant for Val158Met; significant for Ala146Val with Val- associated with violent cases | Vevera et al. [85] |
Colombian male inmates | APD | 310 cases vs 200 controls | Met allele associated with APD (p=0.002) | Cuartas Arias et al. [86] |
Sample Characteristics | Diagnosis | Assessment Tools for Aggression |
Findings | Refs. |
2 treatment-seeking samples (293 German, 190 Polish); >=18 years of age |
Alcoholism | Violent or non-violent crimes; LHA, BDHI, SSAGA | Not significant | Soyka et al. [34] |
97 male Korean | Alcoholism | History of violence | Met associated with violence (p=0.012) | Kweon et al. [90] |
38, 39% males, children | 22q11 deletion | CBCL | Val allele associated with higher scores (p<0.01) | Bearden et al. [91] |
174 adolescent male Russian delinquents | N/A | CD | MetMet associated with highest ADHD symptom counts, and ValVal associated with highest symptom counts for CD (p<0.01) | DeYoung et al. [92] |
112 female German | BPD | BDHI | ValVal and history of childhood sexual abuse associated with lowest BDHI scores (p<0.05) | Wagner et al. [100] |
191, 87% males, European (6-12yo) | ADHD | Antisocial behaviour symptom severity | Not significant | Sengupta et al. [102] |
430 mainly European (15-20yo) | N/A | CBCL | ValVal and maternal smoking associated with adolescent aggression | Brennan et al. [103] |
Turkish, 63% males | Intellectual disability | 36 Violent criminals with MR vs 36 non-MR controls | Not significant | Isir et al. [116] |
112, 68% males, European | Axis II personality disorder | BDHI | Not significant with Val158Met; 3’UTR marker rs165599 associated | Flory et al. [117] |
Male Afro- Caribbean adults |
N/A | 29 Convicted vs 26 never | Not significant | Vinkers et al. [118] |
ADHD attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; CD conduct disorder; BPD borderline personality disorder; APD antisocial personality disorder; ODD oppositional defiant disorder; BDHI Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory; CBCL child behavior checklist; OAS Overt Aggression Scale; AQ Aggression Questionnaire; LHA Life History of Aggression Scale; RAD Risk Assessment for Dangerousness; TRF Teacher Report Form.
Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT)
COMT is an enzyme that is involved in metabolizing various catecholamine neurotransmitters, including dopamine and epinephrine. The COMT gene is 27.22kb in length, and is located on chromosome 22q11.2 [12]. There are two isoforms expressed from two promoters, the soluble S-COMT isoform that is expressed in most tissues, such as liver, blood, and kidneys, and the membrane-bound form MB-COMT that is more common in the brain [13]. The MB-COMT form is of particular interest because of its role in regulating extracellular dopamine levels in the prefrontal cortex [14, 15]. A common non-synonymous single-nucleotide polymorphism (rs4680) changes the 158th amino acid residue of the membrane-bound isoform (or 108th amino acid of the soluble form) from Valine (Val) to Methionine (Met). The presence of the Met variant leads to a four-fold reduction in COMT enzyme activity due to increased thermolability at physiological temperature [16, 17]. This in turn increases the dopamine levels in the prefrontal cortex [16].
COMT in Aggression
While the etiopathophysiology of aggression is unclear, previous research indicates the dopamine neurotransmission system to play a prominent role. For example, dopamine levels are elevated before, during, and after aggressive social encounters in rodents [18]. In addition, dopamine-augmenting pharmacological agents, including amphetamine and apomorphine, have been associated with the induction of aggressive behavior [19, 20]. Furthermore, frontal cortical dopamine depletion by 6-hydroxydopamine-induced lesions resulted in diminished foot-shocked induced fighting behavior in rats [21]. The role of COMT in aggression was first suggested by Kuperman et al. [22], where the authors followed up on 31 men who were seen for issues of hyperactivity as children. They found that COMT activity was negatively correlated to a measure of hostility and positively correlated to a measure of impulsivity. Animal models have provided further support for a role of COMT in aggressive behavior. More specifically, male mice lacking one copy of the comt gene displayed enhanced fighting behavior toward one another [23]. These previous observations, as well as the role of the COMT enzyme in the degradation of dopamine, support the study of the COMT gene in aggressive behavior.
Albaugh et al. [24] reported that youths carrying at least one copy of the low-activity Met allele had higher aggression scores on the mother-rated Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; [25]) than the ValVal genotype carriers, particularly in aggression scale, direct aggression and relational aggression. In a sample of 379 male air force recruits, aggression scores measured by Aggression Questionnaire were not significantly different across the three Val158Met genotypes [26]. Hirata et al. [27] analyzed the COMT gene for possible association with pervasive aggression assessed by Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; [25]) (both mother and teacher rated), but did not find Val158Met to be associated with risk for aggression. The results remained not significant when the analysis was stratified by gender or ADHD diagnosis. The inconsistent findings across these studies suggest that the relationship between COMT Val158Met and aggression may not be straightforward. It is especially relevant as these studies were conducted on samples of different ages, different sex ratios using different assessments of aggressive behaviors, making these studies not directly comparable. Thus, it was not surprising that a recent meta-analysis did not find Val158Met to be significantly associated with aggression in general [28]. Many studies attempted to control for this disparity by narrowing the focus of Val158Met in aggression to within specific psychiatric populations, with schizophrenia being often considered.
COMT and Aggression in Schizophrenia
In numerous studies, aggression and the COMT gene have been examined was in the context of schizophrenia. Part of the reason was the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia, as well as the use of antipsychotics in the treatment of aggressive behaviors in various populations [29-31], including schizophrenia [32]. The first genetic study examined the role of Val158Met in aggressive or potential dangerous behaviors, as measured using the Risk Assessment for Dangerousness (RAD), in schizophrenia inpatients [33]. The dangerous behaviors included having caused bodily harm, alcohol/cocaine abuse, violent crime, and/or threatening behaviors. The authors reported that homozygotes for the low-activity MetMet allele had higher risk for these dangerous behaviors than the high-activity ValVal homozygotes. The small sample of 37 patients, and the mixed ethnicity meant that these findings were preliminary. Nonetheless, it was a novel finding implicating COMT Val158Met to aggressive behavior in schizophrenia.
A number of studies have attempted to replicate the original Strous et al. [33] finding of an association between COMT MetMet genotype and aggression in schizophrenia/ schizoaffective disorder patients (e.g., reviewed in [34]). Lachman et al. [35] reported a significant over-representation of the low-activity Met allele in 27 patients with documented history of multiple assaults compared to 28 non-violent patients. Similarly, Kotler et al. [36] found a trend for the MetMet genotype to be more frequently observed in 30 homicidal patients than in 62 nonviolent patients. In the Strous et al. [37] study, the authors used the Life Time History of Aggression scale (LHA; [38]) which has a variety of measures of aggression including self-directed aggression, antisocial behavior subscale, and aggression. The authors found in a Jewish sample that the low-activity MetMet genotype carriers had significantly higher LHA scores than carriers of the other genotypes, particularly due to the Aggression and Self-directed aggression subscales [37]. Han et al. [39] studied an East Asian sample of 168 male schizophrenia patients from South Korea. In this study, they assessed aggressive behavior with the Overt Aggression Scale (OAS; [40]). The OAS included subscales for verbal aggression, aggression against objects, physical aggression against self, and physical aggression against other people. The authors found that among the COMT genotypes, the homozygous MetMet genotype was associated with the highest scores on the subscale measuring physical aggression against other people. Han et al. [41] followed up with another paper examining only first-episode male patients and reported the similar results for physical aggression against other people and total OAS scores. More recently, Tosato et al. [42] studied a cohort of schizophrenia patients comparing the MetMet homozygotes with the ValVal homozygotes, taking into consideration substance use as well as antipsychotic use and dosage. They also found that the MetMet patients had the higher total OAS scores than the ValVal patients.
As described above, there have been numerous reports of the low-activity Met allele being associated with aggression in schizophrenia patients; however, there have also been a number of studies reporting negative or opposite findings. Jones et al. [43] measured aggressive behavior in a European sample of 180 schizophrenia patients using OAS. They reported that homozygotes for high-activity (ValVal) allele showed higher total OAS scores than the other two genotypes. They also found the lowest scores in heterozygotes. Zammit et al. [44] continued on with Jones et al. [43] with an additional 150 schizophrenia patients, but the results were not significant for either this stage-two sample or the combined sample. Hong et al. [45] found that the frequencies of Val158Met genotypes and alleles did not differ significantly between schizophrenia patients who had committed homicides and non-homicidal schizophrenia patients. Liou et al. [46] investigated incidents of aggressive behavior towards others or threatening gestures in the two weeks before hospital admission in a sample of 198 Chinese schizophrenia patients, and failed to find an association with Val158Met. Koen et al. [47] examined male schizophrenia patients of various ancestries from South Africa and did not report significant association between Val158Met and violent behaviors. Kim et al. [48] studied a group of 61 aggressive and 104 non-aggressive schizophrenia patients. They found that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism had no significant effect on the presence of aggression in the Korean schizophrenia sample; however they found the Met-allele carriers to score higher in verbal aggression within the aggressive schizophrenia subgroup. Koh et al. [49] examined inpatients from a forensic psychiatric hospital in Korea. When they compared the frequencies of criminal or homicidal schizophrenia patients with healthy controls, they did not find Val158Met to be associated.
Despite the mixed findings of COMT Val158Met in aggressive behavior in schizophrenia samples, a number of recent meta-analyses showed an overall effect of the low-activity Met allele carrying genotypes in risk for violence (pooled odds ratio for the MetMet genotype = 1.737, 95% confidence interval = 1.103 to 2.735, p=0.017) [50, 51].
Besides schizophrenia, there have been a considerable number of studies looking at how COMT Val158Met may play a role in aggressive behavior in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
COMT and Aggression in Attention-Deficit Hyper-activity Disorder
ADHD and disruptive behavior disorders are highly co-morbid (reviewed in [52]), and longitudinal studies demonstrate that ADHD is often antecedent to antisocial behavior [53]. The dopamine system has been studied extensively in ADHD due to the efficacy of medications that regulate the levels of catecholamines [54]. A number of studies have investigated for possible association between COMT Val158Met and ADHD diagnosis, though a recent meta-analysis reported negative findings [55]. Nonetheless, many papers also examined whether Val158Met plays a role in conduct or aggressive behavior in ADHD children and adolescents.
Thapar et al. [56] first reported an association between the ValVal genotype in risk of comorbid conduct disorder in a sample of 240 ADHD children of European ancestry. Caspi et al. [57] carried out a large study to further support the relationship between Val158Met and aggressive behavior in three independent samples. Firstly, they examined conduct disorder symptoms in children with ADHD before treatment commencement. They found the ValVal homozygotes to score higher in conduct disorder symptoms than Met-allele carriers. When the authors followed up with a general population sample, they found that the association of ValVal with pervasive aggression, as measured by CBCL by multiple informants, was specific for children with ADHD. These association findings were not observed in children without ADHD. The authors further examined this relationship in a third cohort, where adolescents were followed to adulthood. Similar to the other two samples, higher scores on an antisocial behaviors index in ValVal genotype carriers in comparison to Met-allele carriers were observed only in individuals with ADHD. ADHD individuals with the ValVal genotype were also more likely to have been convicted of a crime at age 32. These replicated findings suggest that the association of COMT high-activity ValVal genotype with aggressive behavior may be dependent on underlying psychiatric diagnosis, as in this case ADHD.
Monuteaux et al. [58] attempted to further replicate the previous findings by examining conduct disorder symptoms in 444 ADHD patients extracted from four studies. Although COMT Val158Met was not associated with risk of conduct disorder, ValVal subjects reported more aggressive subtype symptoms than Met-allele carriers. Nonetheless, these findings are consistent with those reported by Caspi et al. [57]. Similarly, Qian et al. [59] looked at a sample of 171 Chinese ADHD boys and also found the ValVal genotypes to be observed more frequently in the group with comorbid oppositional defiant disorder than expected by chance. In another large study of 4365 children, the highest loading of antisocial behavior symptoms was found in ADHD children with the high-activity ValVal genotype (odds ratio: 2.82; 95% confidence interval: 2.02-3.94; P <0.001) [60]. Salatino-Oliveira et al. [61] studied on a sample of 516 Brazilian youths and adolescents with ADHD. They found the ValVal genotype to be at higher risk for conduct or oppositional defiant disorder (p=0.016; OR=1.58; CI95%=1.07–2.35).
In contrast, Palmason et al. [62] could not replicate the earlier findings when they analyzed symptom severity and risk for conduct disorder with Val158Met in a sample of 166 ADHD children. They reported the high-activity Met-carrying genotypes to be associated with higher ADHD symptom severity, and did not find Val158Met to be associated with lifetime history of comorbid conduct disorder in their sample. Nobile et al. [63] investigated 446 youths of mostly Italian ancestry using the CBCL/6-18 checklist. They found that children with the ValVal genotype in families of low socioeconomic status scored highest in the ADHD symptom severity compared to children of other genotypes and socioeconomic statuses. However, they did not find Val158Met to be associated with conduct behavioral problems. Future studies looking into specific aggressive symptoms within conduct disorder may be required to better address the role of Val158Met in aggressive behavior exhibited by ADHD children.
COMT and Suicidal Behavior (Reviewed in [64])
Aggression has been linked to suicidal behavior across psychiatric diagnoses (reviewed in [65]). Impulsive aggression, as measured by Barratt Impulsiveness Scale and the Brown-Goodwin History of Aggression, has been shown to be a predictor of suicide completion [66].
In a combined sample of Finnish and US schizophrenia/ schizoaffective disorder patients, the low-functioning Met allele was found more frequently in suicide attempters, especially in male attempters or violent attempters [67]. The low-activity MetMet genotype appeared to be over-represented in suicide attempters in a Croatian alcoholism sample [68]. A German study investigated constructs of aggression to test if the COMT gene could have more complex effects on aggression [69]. This study had a sample of 149 suicide attempters and 328 healthy control subjects. The low-activity Met allele was found to be over-represented in violent suicide attempters compared to the nonviolent attempters. In addition, the authors administered the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAI, [70, 71] ) for various components of aggressive behavior. After controlling for age, sex, educational levels, and suicide attempter versus healthy control status, the high-activity Val allele appeared to be associated with State Anger and Anger-in or inwardly directed anger, and the low-activity Met allele appeared to be associated with Anger-out or externalizing anger. There have been many other studies investigating Val158Met in suicidal behavior. The first meta-analysis of Val158Met in suicide utilizing earlier studies pointed to the low-activity Met variant to be associated with risk for suicidal behavior [72]. However, more recent meta-analyses with additional samples (e.g., [73-76]) failed to confirm this association [77, 78]. Interestingly, a number of studies reported the sex-specific association of Val158Met with suicidal behavior [67, 79], and sex ratios appeared to influence the overall findings [72, 77], warranting further studies involving sex-stratified analyses. As different associations have been observed for ADHD and schizophrenia, it would be prudent to conduct these suicide-related analyses within specific psychiatric diagnoses. In addition, as suicidal behavior is an outcome, examining aggression measures may be fruitful in this population.
COMT and Aggression in other Psychiatric Disorders or Contexts
Aggressive behavior is also commonly observed in patients with borderline personality disorder (reviewed in [80]). With regard to personality disorders, several groups have investigated whether the low-activity Met allele or MetMet genotype is associated with antisocial (APD) or borderline personality disorders (BPD), both of which involve impulsive aggressive behaviors. The MetMet genotype was associated with risk for BPD when comparing between 161 BPD patients and 156 healthy controls in a study on a German sample [81]. In an Italian sample, Lazzaretti et al. [82] showed that COMT low-functioning MetMet genotype was more frequent in 19 BPD patients compared to 36 healthy controls (47.4% vs. 22.2%). However, these initial association findings between COMT Val158Met and BPD could not be replicated in a sample of young adults from low-income families in the US, a sample of Hungarian mood disorder patients [83], or a sample of female heroin-dependent patients in China [84]. Vevera et al. [85] examined a Caucasian sample of 47 male inmates convicted of violent crimes with high impulsivity. These participants had a diagnosis of APD. The authors did not find genotype frequencies of the Val158Met in these cases to differ significantly from those in 43 healthy control subjects. They, however, found a different polymorphism, Ala146Val to be significant, where the Val-carrying genotypes were over-represented in the violent antisocial cases. Cuartas Arias et al. [86] showed in a study in Colombia that the Met allele was more frequently observed in 310 male inmates with APD than in 200 male inmates without APD. With the few studies conducted on Val158Met in BPD or APD so far, it is premature to draw conclusions from the mixed findings reported thus far.
Alcohol use has been linked to violent and aggressive behavior at home [87, 88] and in the workplace [89]. Few studies have investigated specifically the role of Val158Met and aggression in alcohol dependence. Neither violent nor non-violent crimes were associated with Val158Met in two treatment-seeking alcoholic patient samples [34]. Moreover, Val158Met was found not to be associated with criminal or aggressive behavior as assessed using the Brown Goodwin Life History of Aggression, Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory, or the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) in a German sample and a Polish replication sample [34]. However, Kweon et al. [90] found the Met allele to be associated with history of violence in a Korean sample of 97 male alcoholics.
Bearden et al. [91] looked at behavioral problems in 38 children with 22q11.2 deletion; that is, with only one copy of the COMT gene. They found individuals carrying the Val allele to score significantly higher than Met allele carriers in various components of the CBCL, including externalizing and internalizing behaviors, delinquency, withdrawal, and social problems, but not in aggressive behavior. In a study on Russian delinquents, while the MetMet genotype was associated with the highest symptom counts for ADHD, the ValVal genotype was associated with the highest symptom counts for conduct disorder [92].
DISCUSSION
Overall, the findings from association studies between COMT Val158Met and aggression have been mixed. A few interesting trends were observed. The low-functioning Met allele appeared to be associated with aggression in schizophrenia patients, while the high-functioning Val allele appeared to be associated with conduct behavioral problems in a majority of ADHD samples thus far. These trends seemed to be congruent with some of the medications used for treating aggressive behaviors in these two populations, more specifically the use of dopamine D2 receptor-antagonizing antipsychotics in schizophrenia patients [93] and the use of dopamine-agonizing psychostimulants (e.g., methylphenidate) in ADHD patients [94]. These results may be reflective of the mechanism of the underlying psychiatric disorders. In some cases, the use of psychotropic medication could have confounded these findings. Unfortunately, methodological issues to be discussed below make genetic studies of aggression hard to interpret.
Gene-Gene and Gene-Environment Interaction in Aggression
The mixed findings for Val158Met with aggressive behavior could have been due to the fact that multiple gene variants likely contribute to the function of the COMT enzyme [95, 96]. It is COMT activity, which may be informed by the sum and interactions of its gene variants, may be associated with aggressive behavior. For example, one study found that a functional Alanine substitution for Serine (Ala72Ser, rs6267, [97]) was associated with history of homicides in a Korean schizophrenia sample [45]. The association was strengthened when the Ala72Ser and Val158Met were considered in haplotypes. These findings while requiring replications, suggest that various polymorphisms could lead to changes in enzymatic activity that can influence the risk for aggressive behavior. Additional work with other functional COMT polymorphisms may address some of the discrepancies in the association findings. In addition to studies based on single genes, multiple genes may additively or interactively contribute to the development of aggressive behavior. Tadic et al. [81] found the genotype combination of COMT MetMet and short/short or short/long at the SLC6A4 5HTTLPR to be over-represented in BPD patients. There is growing evidence that decreased serotonin and increased dopamine is associated impulsive aggression [98, 99]. Thus, studies looking into gene variants that regulate dopamine levels in addition to COMT Val158Met, and serotonin levels, as well as the interactions among these gene variants are warranted.
In some studies, investigators have investigated if individuals’ predisposition to aggressive behavior could be explained by the COMT gene in conjunction with environmental variables. In one study, Wagner et al. [100] aimed to see if aggressive behavior modulated the relationship between severe life events and BPD. In an exclusively female patient sample from Germany, aggression was assessed using the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI; [101]). The authors found an interaction between COMT Val158Met and stressful life events. More specifically, ValVal genotype carriers with history of childhood sexual abuse had the lowest average BDHI sum scores, while the ValVal genotype carriers without such history had the highest scores. One study found an interaction of low birth weight with COMT Val158Met in predicting antisocial behavior symptom severity in a European ADHD sample [56]. However, this finding failed to replicate in an independent sample of mostly Europeans [102]. Brennan et al. [103] investigated the effect of maternal prenatal cigarette smoking on psychopathology in the offspring in a large birth cohort study in Australia. They did not find Val158Met by itself to be associated with CBCL-rated aggressive behavior during adolescence. However, they found that ValVal carriers whose mothers smoked during pregnancy scored the highest in adolescent aggressive behavioral problems than other comparison groups. The interaction between Val158Met and maternal prenatal smoking in antisocial behavior was not observed in another study [56].
In order to further elucidate how the environment can contribute towards aggressive behavior, epigenetic studies could be included to see if methylation patterns can explain any changes in aggressive behavior. Thus far, the COMT gene has not been studied epigenetically at great depth. However, one study looked at the potential role of DNA methylation in the COMT gene in relation to PTSD. Norrholm et al. (2013) looked at a sample of patients with a history of PTSD from the inner cities of Atlanta Georgia. The study wanted to see if the environment can confer an impact on PTSD and fear response, specifically if the COMT promoter region methylation can have an effect on it. What they found was that the Met/Met genotype had higher fear response to a signal than the Val-allele carrying genotypes. This pattern also persisted in the Met/Met genotype with a diagnosis of PTSD. In addition, the Met/Met genotype was associated with methylation patterns at four CpG sites of which two sites also had an association with impaired fear inhibition to a safety signal [104]. These results highlight the complexity of the contribution of environmental variables to genetic studies of aggression, and encourage the inclusion of epigenetic analyses to the studies of COMT.
It is further likely that both gene-gene and gene-environment interactions will have to be considered in concert in studies of aggressive behavior. For example, van IJzendoorn et al. [105] studied a group of mother-toddler pairs where the children scored at least 75th percentile on the externalizing problem scale of the CBCL. They examined hassles experienced by the mothers and their sensitivity towards their toddlers during a series of problem-solving tasks. Mothers who carried a Val allele as well as a 7R allele of the DRD4 exon-3 variable-number tandem repeat marker were more sensitive to their toddlers when they reported less hassle. The mothers with the same genotypes were less sensitive when they reported more hassle. These observations, though preliminary, demonstrate the presence of both gene-gene and gene-environment interactions in aggressive behavior.
Defining and Measuring Aggression
An important issue in summarizing the literature on the association findings of genetic variants with aggression is the variety of ways used to define aggressive behavior. Firstly, aggression has been studied in the context of various psychiatric disorders. For instance, ADHD children exhibiting aggressive behavior are commonly diagnosed with either Oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) or Conduct Disorder (CD). However, even in individuals diagnosed with either of the ODD or CD, there is considerable heterogeneity between them. ADHD comorbid with ODD appeared to be associated with lower risk of learning disability, while ADHD comorbid with CD appeared to be associated with increased anxiety, aggression, and maternal psychopathology [106]. In a ten-year follow-up study, boys with ADHD and ODD were at higher risk of depression, while boys with ADHD and CD were at higher risk of bipolar disorder, antisocial personality disorder, and smoking [107].
Another issue comes in distinguishing between antisocial and aggressive behaviors. Aggressive behavior can be defined as behavior that is meant to harm other people and/or objects or it can be self-directed. Antisocial behaviors can be defined as behaviors where those affected are disadvantaged due to a violation of social norms and values. An example of antisocial behavior is stealing. By definition, aggressive behaviors represent one of the dimensions of antisocial behaviors but not all antisocial behaviors are acts of aggression. A longitudinal population-based twin study in Sweden reported difference in heritability of antisocial behavior between the sexes [108]. More specifically, while the genetic contribution to aggressive behavior was similar for both sexes, genes explain more of the variance in delinquency in females than in males. Moreover, while the genetic contribution to aggressive antisocial behavior appears to be stable from childhood to adolescence, its contribution to delinquent behavior fluctuates through this developmental period [109]. Conducting genetic analyses on aggressive and non-aggressive delinquent antisocial behaviors separately may help reduce the noise associated with heterogeneity of antisocial behavior.
Aggressive behaviors also fall under two broad subtypes, reactive and proactive aggression [110]. Reactive aggression can be defined as an impulsive act which is generally uncontrolled and is related to high levels of arousal and emotions. It is an angry, hostile reaction to a stimulus. An example is striking someone after being insulted. Proactive aggression directed at achieving an end. It can be seen as a means to use aggression instrumentally in order to attain a goal. Proactive aggression is characteristic of psychopaths as their behaviors are labeled as callous and unemotional. This distinction has been mostly ignored in past studies of Val158Met in aggression. A twin study using the Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire from multiple informants has shown that genetic factors appear to explain a significant variance in proactive aggression and reactive aggression, with larger variance explained for proactive aggression [6]. This genetic contribution also appears to be larger in males, warranting sex-stratified analyses of aggressive behavior.
Measuring aggression has been a challenge due to its heterogeneity. In the case of children, a common questionnaire that is used is the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; [25]). There are three versions, the parent- or guardian-rated CBCL, the Teacher Report Form, and the Youth Self Report. Some aggression studies discussed above were conducted only with single informants (only mothers or only participants) [58, 63], thus the aggressive phenotype might not have been pervasive across different settings and might have been more dependent on specific environmental stressors.
In this context it is worth noting that aggressive behavior can occur as a “state” or a “trait”. In the former circumstances aggressive behavior occurs within a limited context, for example, within the family home when limits are placed on the child or adolescent. In contrast, when aggression is a trait, aggressive behavior will be evident in multiple situations and contexts, at home, at school, with friends and so on. In these circumstances the aggression would be considered pervasive. The intensity of the aggressive behaviors may fluctuate depending on the status of the underlying psychiatric illness, such as manic episodes in bipolar disorder [111, 112], or psychotic episodes in schizophrenia. None of the studies reviewed have explicitly dealt with this issue, and some of the contradictory findings may be attributed to the failure to differentiate between aggression as a trait versus aggression as a state.
The Overt Aggression Scale (OAS [40]), which is used for the assessment of aggressive behavior in adults, consists of subscales for verbal aggression, aggression against objects, physical aggression against self, and physical aggression against other people. It is interesting to note that OAS scores could be influenced by other personality traits such as sensation seeking and impulsivity, as well as environmental factors such as smoking [113]. Moreover, unlike BDHI and LHA, OAS assesses aggressive behavior occurring during the past week, thus it does not capture aggressive behavior across lifetime. To further complicate matter, one meta-analysis of genetic and environmental influence on aggression showed that self-reported and parent-rated aggression yielded different heritability measures from observational data [114], warranting the use of multiple tools of aggression assessment.
In summary, there have been many studies on COMT Val158Met in aggressive behavior. However, its role in aggression is still unclear. Aggressive behaviors have been observed in mood disorder patients, with bipolar I disorder appearing to be associated with higher levels of aggression than bipolar II disorder or unipolar depression [112]. Future studies of COMT aggression should include these populations, as well as intellectual disability, Alzheimer’s disease, stroke victims, and other dementias. Future studies will also need to consider many approaches to help to resolve ininconsistencies in previous studies of COMT and aggression. They include testing additional genetic markers in COMT and other dopamine system genes, looking at genetic markers in other neurotransmitter systems (e.g., serotonin), and exploring gene-gene interactions. Environmental variables also need to be considered in conjunction with epigenetic changes. More importantly, there needs to be consensus on the assessment and characterization of the different facets of aggressive behavior, including proactive or reactive, state or trait, self-reported or multi-informant. Overcoming these challenges will help to consolidate the various genetic findings and elucidate the biological mechanism underlying the development of aggressive behavior, leading to improved outcome through evidence-based prevention and intervention strategies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank Dr. Sheng Chen and Andrea Smart for their assistance in the preparation of this review.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors confirm that this article content has no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Shepherd J.P., Shepherd I., Newcombe R.G., Farrington D. Impact of antisocial lifestyle on health: chronic disability and death by middle age. J. Public Health (Oxf.) 2009;31(4):506–511. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdp054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hodgins S., Cree A., Alderton J., Mak T. From conduct disorder to severe mental illness: associations with aggressive behaviour, crime and victimization. Psychol. Med. 2008;38(7):975–987. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hodgins S., Alderton J., Cree A., Aboud A., Mak T. Aggressive behaviour, victimization and crime among severely mentally ill patients requiring hospitalisation. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2007;191:343–350. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.06.029587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mendes D.D., Mari Jde.J., Singer M., Barros G.M., Mello A.F. [Study review of biological, social and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior]. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2009;31(Suppl. 2):S77–S85. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462009000600006. [Study review of biological, social and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Veenema A.H. Early life stress, the development of aggression and neuroendocrine and neurobiological correlates: what can we learn from animal models? Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30(4):497–518. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Baker L.A., Raine A., Liu J., Jacobson K.C. Differential genetic and environmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008;36(8):1265–1278. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bezdjian S., Tuvblad C., Raine A., Baker L.A. The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 8.Brendgen M., Vitaro F., Boivin M., Dionne G., PA(c)russe D. Examining genetic and environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression. Dev. Psychol. 2006;42(6):1299–1312. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tuvblad C., Raine A., Zheng M., Baker L.A. Genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 10.Rhee S.H., Waldman I.D. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. 2002. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Craig I.W., Halton K.E. Genetics of human aggressive behaviour. Hum. Genet. 2009;126(1):101–113. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0695-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Grossman M.H., Emanuel B.S., Budarf M.L. Chromosomal mapping of the human catechol-O-methyltransferase gene to 22q11.1----q11.2. Genomics. 1992;12(4):822–825. doi: 10.1016/0888-7543(92)90316-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tenhunen J., Salminen M., LundstrA m K., Kiviluoto T., Savolainen R., Ulmanen I. Genomic organization of the human catechol O-methyltransferase gene and its expression from two distinct promoters. Eur. J. Biochem. 1994;223(3):1049–1059. doi: 10.1111/j.1432-1033.1994.tb19083.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tunbridge E.M., Bannerman D.M., Sharp T., Harrison P.J. Catechol-o-methyltransferase inhibition improves set-shifting performance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2004;24(23):5331–5335. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1124-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hong J., Shu-Leong H., Tao X., Lap-Ping Y. Distribution of catechol-O-methyltransferase expression in human central nervous system. 1998. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 16.Chen J., Lipska B.K., Halim N., Ma Q.D., Matsumoto M., Melhem S., Kolachana B.S., Hyde T.M., Herman M.M., Apud J., Egan M.F., Kleinman J.E., Weinberger D.R. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2004;75(5):807–821. doi: 10.1086/425589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lotta T., Vidgren J., Tilgmann C., Ulmanen I., MelA(c)n K., Julkunen I., Taskinen J. Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O-methyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry. 1995;34(13):4202–4210. doi: 10.1021/bi00013a008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Tidey J.W., Miczek K.A. Social defeat stress selectively alters mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 1996;721(1-2):140–149. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00159-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Miczek K.A., Fish E.W., De Bold J.F., De Almeida R.M. Social and neural determinants of aggressive behavior: pharmacotherapeutic targets at serotonin, dopamine and gamma-aminobutyric acid systems. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2002;163(3-4):434–458. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1139-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ossowska G., Klenk-Majewska B., Zebrowska-Lupina I. Acute effect of dopamine agonists and some antidepressants in stress-induced deficit of fighting behavior. Pol. J. Pharmacol. 1996;48(4):403–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pucilowski O., Kostowski W., Bidzinski A., Hauptmann M. Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine-induced lesions of A10 dopaminergic neurons on aggressive behavior in rats. . Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1982;16:547–551. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90412-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kuperman S., Kramer J., Loney J. Enzyme activity and behavior in hyperactive children grown up. Biol. Psychiatry. 1988;24(4):375–383. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90173-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gogos J.A., Morgan M., Luine V., Santha M., Ogawa S., Pfaff D., Karayiorgou M. Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95(17):9991–9996. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.17.9991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Albaugh M.D., Harder V.S., Althoff R.R., Rettew D.C., Ehli E.A., Lengyel-Nelson T., Davies G.E., Ayer L., Sulman J., Stanger C., Hudziak J.J. COMT Val158Met genotype as a risk factor for problem behaviors in youth. 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 25.Achenbach T.M. Manual for the child behavior checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 profile, Burlington, VT; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Avramopoulos D., Stefanis N.C., Hantoumi I., Smyrnis N., Evdokimidis I., Stefanis C.N. Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy young males carrying the COMT high activity allele. Mol. Psychiatry. 2002;7(7):706–711. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hirata Y., Zai C.C., Nowrouzi B., Beitchman J.H., Kennedy J.L. Study of the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene with high aggression in children. Aggress. Behav. 2013;39(1):45–51. doi: 10.1002/ab.21448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Vassos E., Collier D.A., Fazel S. Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies of violence and aggression. Mol. Psychiatry. 2014;19(4):471–477. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pappadopulos E., Macintyre Ii J.C., Crismon M.L., Findling R.L., Malone R.P., Derivan A., Schooler N., Sikich L., Greenhill L., Schur S.B., Felton C.J., Kranzler H., Rube D.M., Sverd J., Finnerty M., Ketner S., Siennick S.E., Jensen P.S. Treatment recommendations for the use of antipsychotics for aggressive youth (TRAAY). Part II. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2003;42(2):145–161. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200302000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schur S.B., Sikich L., Findling R.L., Malone R.P., Crismon M.L., Derivan A., Macintyre Ii J.C., Pappadopulos E., Greenhill L., Schooler N., Van Orden K., Jensen P.S. Treatment recommendations for the use of antipsychotics for aggressive youth (TRAAY). Part I: a review. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2003;42(2):132–144. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200302000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gallagher D., Herrmann N. Agitation and aggression in Alzheimer ?(tm)s disease: an update on pharmacological and psychosocial approaches to care. Neurodegener. Dis. Manag. 2015;5(1):75–83. doi: 10.2217/nmt.14.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Krakowski M.I., Czobor P., Citrome L., Bark N., Cooper T.B. Atypical antipsychotic agents in the treatment of violent patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. 2006. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 33.Strous R.D., Bark N., Parsia S.S., Volavka J., Lachman H.M. Analysis of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism in schizophrenia: evidence for association with aggressive and antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Res. 1997;69(2-3):71–77. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(96)03111-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. (a) Soyka M., Zill P., Koller G., Samochowiec A., Grzywacz A., Preuss U.W. Val158Met COMT polymorphism and risk of aggression in alcohol dependence. Addict. Biol. 2015;20(1):197–204. doi: 10.1111/adb.12098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; (b) Volavka J., Kennedy J.L., Ni X., Czobor P., Nolan K., Sheitman B., Lindenmayer J.P., Citrome L., McEvoy J., Lieberman J.A. COMT158 polymorphism and hostility. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2004;127B(1):28–29. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lachman H.M., Nolan K.A., Mohr P., Saito T., Volavka J. Association between catechol O-methyltransferase genotype and violence in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1998;155(6):835–837. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.6.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kotler M., Barak P., Cohen H., Averbuch I.E., Grinshpoon A., Gritsenko I., Nemanov L., Ebstein R.P. Homicidal behavior in schizophrenia associated with a genetic polymorphism determining low catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) activity. Am. J. Med. Genet. 1999;88(6):628–633. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19991215)88:6<628::AID-AJMG10>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Strous R.D., Nolan K.A., Lapidus R., Diaz L., Saito T., Lachman H.M. Aggressive behavior in schizophrenia is associated with the low enzyme activity COMT polymorphism: a replication study. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2003;120B(1):29–34. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Coccaro E.F., Berman M.E., Kavoussi R.J. Assessment of life history of aggression: development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Res. 1997;73(3):147–157. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(97)00119-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Han D.H., Park D.B., Na C., Kee B.S., Lee Y.S. Association of aggressive behavior in Korean male schizophrenic patients with polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter promoter and catecholamine-O-methyltransferase genes. Psychiatry Res. 2004;129(1):29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Yudofsky S.C., Silver J.M., Jackson W., Endicott J., Williams D. The Overt Aggression Scale for the objective rating of verbal and physical aggression. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1986;143(1):35–39. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Han D.H., Kee B.S., Min K.J., Lee Y.S., Na C., Park D.B., Lyoo I.K. Effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism on the cognitive stability and aggression in the first-onset schizophrenic patients. Neuroreport. 2006;17(1):95–99. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000192740.38653.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tosato S., Bonetto C., Di Forti M., Collier D., Cristofalo D., Bertani M., Zanoni M., Marrella G., Lazzarotto L., Lasalvia A., De Gironcoli M., Tansella M., Dazzan P., Murray R., Ruggeri M. Effect of COMT genotype on aggressive behaviour in a community cohort of schizophrenic patients. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;495(1):17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jones G., Zammit S., Norton N., Hamshere M.L., Jones S.J., Milham C., Sanders R.D., McCarthy G.M., Jones L.A., Cardno A.G., Gray M., Murphy K.C., Owen M.J. Aggressive behaviour in patients with schizophrenia is associated with catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2001;179:351–355. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.4.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zammit S., Jones G., Jones S.J., Norton N., Sanders R.D., Milham C., McCarthy G.M., Jones L.A., Cardno A.G., Gray M., Murphy K.C., O ?(tm)Donovan M.C., Owen M.J. Polymorphisms in the MAOA, MAOB, and COMT genes and aggressive behavior in schizophrenia. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2004;128B(1):19–20. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hong J.P., Lee J.S., Chung S., Jung J., Yoo H.K., Chang S.M., Kim C.Y. New functional single nucleotide polymorphism (Ala72Ser) in the COMT gene is associated with aggressive behavior in male schizophrenia. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2008;147B(5):658–660. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Liou Y.J., Tsai S.J., Hong C.J., Wang Y.C., Lai I.C. Association analysis of a functional catechol-o-methyltransferase gene polymorphism in schizophrenic patients in Taiwan. Neuropsychobiology. 2001;43(1):11–14. doi: 10.1159/000054858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Koen L., Kinnear C.J., Corfield V.A., Emsley R.A., Jordaan E., Keyter N., Moolman-Smook J.C., Stein D.J., Niehaus D.J. Violence in male patients with schizophrenia: risk markers in a South African population. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2004;38(4):254–259. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2004.01338.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kim Y.R., Kim J.H., Kim S.J., Lee D., Min S.K. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism in relation to aggressive schizophrenia in a Korean population. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18(11):820–825. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Koh K.B., Choi E.H., Lee Y.J., Han M., Choi S.S., Kim S.W., Lee M.G. The relation of serotonin-related gene and COMT gene polymorphisms with criminal behavior in schizophrenic disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2012;73(2):159–163. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bhakta S.G., Zhang J.P., Malhotra A.K. The COMT Met158 allele and violence in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 51.Singh J.P., Volavka J., Czobor P., Van Dorn R.A. A meta-analysis of the Val158Met COMT polymorphism and violent behavior in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):e43423. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kutcher S., Aman M., Brooks S.J., Buitelaar J., van Daalen E., Fegert J., Findling R.L., Fisman S., Greenhill L.L., Huss M., Kusumakar V., Pine D., Taylor E., Tyano S. International consensus statement on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disruptive behaviour disorders (DBDs): clinical implications and treatment practice suggestions. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004;14(1):11–28. doi: 10.1016/S0924-977X(03)00045-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Thapar A., van den Bree M., Fowler T., Langley K., Whittinger N. Predictors of antisocial behaviour in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2006;15(2):118–125. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0511-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wigal S.B. Efficacy and safety limitations of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder pharmacotherapy in children and adults. CNS Drugs. 2009;23(Suppl. 1):21–31. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200923000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Sun H., Yuan F., Shen X., Xiong G., Wu J. Role of COMT in ADHD: a systematic meta-analysis. Mol. Neurobiol. 2014;49(1):251–261. doi: 10.1007/s12035-013-8516-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Thapar A., Langley K., Fowler T., Rice F., Turic D., Whittinger N., Aggleton J., Van den Bree M., Owen M., O'Donovan M. Catechol O-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. . Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:1275–1278. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Caspi A., Langley K., Milne B., Moffitt T.E., O ?(tm)Donovan M., Owen M.J., Polo Tomas M., Poulton R., Rutter M., Taylor A., Williams B., Thapar A. A replicated molecular genetic basis for subtyping antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):203–210. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Monuteaux M.C., Biederman J., Doyle A.E., Mick E., Faraone S.V. Genetic risk for conduct disorder symptom subtypes in an ADHD sample: specificity to aggressive symptoms. 2009. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 59.Qian Q.J., Liu J., Wang Y.F., Yang L., Guan L.L., Faraone S.V. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder comorbid oppositional defiant disorder and its predominately inattentive type: evidence for an association with COMT but not MAOA in a Chinese sample. Behav. Brain Funct. 2009;5:8. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Langley K., Heron J., O ?(tm)Donovan M.C., Owen M.J., Thapar A. Genotype link with extreme antisocial behavior: the contribution of cognitive pathways. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2010;67(12):1317–1323. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Salatino-Oliveira A., Genro J.P., GuimarA es A.P., Chazan R., Zeni C., Schmitz M., Polanczyk G., Roman T., Rohde L.A., Hutz M.H. Cathechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158)Met polymorphism is associated with disruptive behavior disorders among children and adolescents with ADHD. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2012;119(6):729–733. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0766-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.PA lmason H., Moser D., Sigmund J., Vogler C., HA nig S., Schneider A., Seitz C., Marcus A., Meyer J., Freitag C.M. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder phenotype is influenced by a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase variant. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2010;117(2):259–267. doi: 10.1007/s00702-009-0338-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nobile M., Rusconi M., Bellina M., Marino C., Giorda R., Carlet O., Vanzin L., Molteni M., Battaglia M. COMT Val158Met polymorphism and socioeconomic status interact to predict attention deficit/hyperactivity problems in children aged 10-14. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;19(7):549–557. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Zai C.C., de Luca V., Strauss J., Tong R.P., Sakinofsky I., Kennedy J.L. Genetic Factors and Suicidal Behavior. In: Dwivedi Y., editor. The Neurobiological Basis of Suicide. Boca Raton, FL: 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Gvion Y., Apter A. Aggression, impulsivity, and suicide behavior: a review of the literature. Arch. Suicide Res. 2011;15(2):93–112. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2011.565265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Dumais A., Lesage A.D., Alda M., Rouleau G., Dumont M., Chawky N., Roy M., Mann J.J., Benkelfat C., Turecki G. Risk factors for suicide completion in major depression: a case-control study of impulsive and aggressive behaviors in men. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 67.Nolan K.A., Volavka J., Czobor P., Cseh A., Lachman H., Saito T., Tiihonen J., Putkonen A., Hallikainen T., Kotilainen I., RA sA nen P., Isohanni M., JA rvelin M.R., Karvonen M.K. Suicidal behavior in patients with schizophrenia is related to COMT polymorphism. Psychiatr. Genet. 2000;10(3):117–124. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200010030-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nedic G., Nikolac M., Sviglin K.N., Muck-Seler D., Borovecki F., Pivac N. Association study of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val108/158Met polymorphism and suicide attempts in patients with alcohol dependence. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;14(3):377–388. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rujescu D., Giegling I., Gietl A., Hartmann A.M., MA ller H.J. A functional single nucleotide polymorphism (V158M) in the COMT gene is associated with aggressive personality traits. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54(1):34–39. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01831-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Schwenkmezger P., Hodapp V. [A questionnaire for assessing anger and expression of anger]. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychopathol. Psychother. 1991;39(1):63–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Spielberger C.D., Gorssuch R.L., Lushene P.R., Vagg P.R., Jacobs G.A. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kia-Keating B.M., Glatt S.J., Tsuang M.T. Meta-analyses suggest association between COMT, but not HTR1B, alleles, and suicidal behavior. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2007;144B(8):1048–1053. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Baud P., Courtet P., Perroud N., Jollant F., Buresi C., Malafosse A. Catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism (COMT) in suicide attempters: a possible gender effect on anger traits. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2007;144B(8):1042–1047. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.De Luca V., Tharmalingam S., Muller D.J., Wong G., de Bartolomeis A., Kennedy J.L. Gene-gene interaction between MAOA and COMT in suicidal behavior: analysis in schizophrenia. 2006. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 75.De Luca V., Tharmalingam S., Sicard T., Kennedy J.L. Gene-gene interaction between MAOA and COMT in suicidal behavior. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 76.Zalsman G., Huang Y.Y., Oquendo M.A., Brent D.A., Giner L., Haghighi F., Burke A.K., Ellis S.P., Currier D., Mann J.J. No association of COMT Val158Met polymorphism with suicidal behavior or CSF monoamine metabolites in mood disorders. Arch. Suicide Res. 2008;12(4):327–335. doi: 10.1080/13811110802324912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Calati R., Porcelli S., Giegling I., Hartmann A.M., Moller H.J., De Ronchi D., Serretti A., Rujescu D. Catechol-o-methyltransferase gene modulation on suicidal behavior and personality traits: review, meta-analysis and association study. 2011. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 78.Tovilla-Zarate C., Juarez-Rojop I., Ramon-Frias T., Villar-Soto M., Pool-Garcia S., Medellin B.C., Genis Mendoza A.D., Narvaez L.L., Humberto N. No association between COMT val158met polymorphism and suicidal behavior: meta-analysis and new data. 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 79.Ono H., Shirakawa O., Nushida H., Ueno Y., Maeda K. Association between catechol-O-methyltransferase functional polymorphism and male suicide completers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(7):1374–1377. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Allen A., Links P.S. Aggression in borderline personality disorder: evidence for increased risk and clinical predictors. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2012;14(1):62–69. doi: 10.1007/s11920-011-0244-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Tadi A., Victor A., Ba Ykaya O., von Cube R., Hoch J., Kouti I., Anicker N.J., HA ppner W., Lieb K., Dahmen N. Interaction between gene variants of the serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) and catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) in borderline personality disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2009;150B(4):487–495. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Lazzaretti M., Fabbro D., Sala M., Del Toso K., de Vidovich G., Marraffini E., Morandotti N., Gambini F., Barale F., Balestrieri M., Damante G., Caverzasi E., Brambilla P. Association between low-activity allele of cathecolamine-O-methyl-transferase (COMT) and Borderline Personality Disorder in an Italian population. Behav. Med. 2013;39(2):25–28. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2012.722134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Nemoda Z., Lyons-Ruth K., Szekely A., Bertha E., Faludi G., Sasvari-Szekely M. Association between dopaminergic polymorphisms and borderline personality traits among at-risk young adults and psychiatric inpatients. Behav. Brain Funct. 2010;6:4. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Yang M., Mamy J., Wang Q., Liao Y.H., Seewoobudul V., Xiao S.Y., Hao W. The association of 5-HTR2A-1438A/G, COMTVal158Met, MAOA-LPR, DATVNTR and 5-HTTVNTR gene polymorphisms and borderline personality disorder in female heroin-dependent Chinese subjects. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;50:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Vevera J., Stopkova R., Bes M., Albrecht T., Papezova H., Zukov I., Raboch J., Stopka P. COMT polymorphisms in impulsively violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 2009;30(6):753–756. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Cuartas Arias J.M., Palacio Acosta C.A., Valencia J.G., Montoya G.J., Arango Viana J.C., Nieto O.C., FlA3rez A.F., Camarena Medellin B.E., Montoya W.R., Lopez Jaramillo C.A., Achury J.G., Fuentes C.C., Berrio G.B., Ruiz-Linares A. Exploring epistasis in candidate genes for antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatr. Genet. 2011;21(3):115–124. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283437175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Foran H.M., O ?(tm)Leary K.D. Alcohol and intimate partner violence: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2008;28(7):1222–1234. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Quigley B.M., Leonard K.E. Alcohol and the continuation of early marital aggression. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2000;24(7):1003–1010. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb04643.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.McFarlin S.K., Fals-Stewart W., Major D.A., Justice E.M. Alcohol use and workplace aggression: an examination of perpetration and victimization. J. Subst. Abuse. 2001;13(3):303–321. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3289(01)00080-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Kweon Y.S., Lee H.K., Lee C.T., Pae C.U. Association study of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism in Korean male alcoholics. Psychiatr. Genet. 2005;15(2):151–154. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200506000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bearden C.E., Jawad A.F., Lynch D.R., Monterossso J.R., Sokol S., McDonald-McGinn D.M., Saitta S.C., Harris S.E., Moss E., Wang P.P., Zackai E., Emanuel B.S., Simon T.J. Effects of COMT genotype on behavioral symptomatology in the 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11(1):109–117. doi: 10.1080/09297040590911239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.DeYoung C.G., Getchell M., Koposov R.A., Yrigollen C.M., Haeffel G.J., af Klinteberg B., Oreland L., Ruchkin V.V., Pakstis A.J., Grigorenko E.L. Variation in the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val 158 Met polymorphism associated with conduct disorder and ADHD symptoms, among adolescent male delinquents. Psychiatr. Genet. 2010;20(1):20–24. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32833511e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Goedhard L.E., Stolker J.J., Heerdink E.R., Nijman H.L., Olivier B., Egberts T.C. Pharmacotherapy for the treatment of aggressive behavior in general adult psychiatry: A systematic review. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2006;67(7):1013–1024. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v67n0702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Pappadopulos E., Woolston S., Chait A., Perkins M., Connor D.F., Jensen P.S. Pharmacotherapy of aggression in children and adolescents: efficacy and effect size. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2006;15(1):27–39. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Meyer-Lindenberg A., Nichols T., Callicott J.H., Ding J., Kolachana B., Buckholtz J., Mattay V.S., Egan M., Weinberger D.R. Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. 2006. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 96.Nackley A.G., Shabalina S.A., Tchivileva I.E., Satterfield K., Korchynskyi O., Makarov S.S., Maixner W., Diatchenko L. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase haplotypes modulate protein expression by altering mRNA secondary structure. Science. 2006;314(5807):1930–1933. doi: 10.1126/science.1131262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Lee S.G., Joo Y., Kim B., Chung S., Kim H.L., Lee I., Choi B., Kim C., Song K. Association of Ala72Ser polymorphism with COMT enzyme activity and the risk of schizophrenia in Koreans. Hum. Genet. 2005;116(4):319–328. doi: 10.1007/s00439-004-1239-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Engelborghs S., Vloeberghs E., Le Bastard N., Van Buggenhout M., Marien P., Somers N., Nagels G., Pickut B.A., De Deyn P.P. The dopaminergic neurotransmitter system is associated with aggression and agitation in frontotemporal dementia. 2008. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 99.Seo D., Patrick C.J., Kennealy P.J. Role of Serotonin and Dopamine System Interactions in the Neurobiology of Impulsive Aggression and its Comorbidity with other Clinical Disorders. 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 100.Wagner S., Baskaya O., Anicker N.J., Dahmen N., Lieb K., Tadi A. The catechol o-methyltransferase (COMT) val(158)met polymorphism modulates the association of serious life events (SLE) and impulsive aggression in female patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010;122(2):110–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Buss A.H., Durkee A. An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. J. Consult. Psychol. 1957;21(4):343–349. doi: 10.1037/h0046900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Sengupta S.M., Grizenko N., Schmitz N., Schwartz G., Ben Amor L., Bellingham J., de Guzman R., Polotskaia A., Ter Stepanian M., Thakur G., Joober R. COMT Val108/158Met gene variant, birth weight, and conduct disorder in children with ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2006;45(11):1363–1369. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000251212.44491.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Brennan P.A., Hammen C., Sylvers P., Bor W., Najman J., Lind P., Montgomery G., Smith A.K. Interactions between the COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism and maternal prenatal smoking predict aggressive behavior outcomes. Biol. Psychol. 2011;87:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Norrholm S.D., Jovanovic T., Smith A.K., Binder E., Klengel T., Conneely K., Mercer K.B., Davis J.S., Kerley K., Winkler J., Gillespie C.F., Bradley B., Ressler K.J. Differential Genetic and Epigenetic Regulation of catechol-O-methyltransferase is Associated with Impaired Fear Inhibition in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 105.van IJzendoorn M.H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., Mesman J. Dopamine system genes associated with parenting in the context of daily hassles. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7(4):403–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00362.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Kuhne M., Schachar R., Tannock R. Impact of comorbid oppositional or conduct problems on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 1997;36(12):1715–1725. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Biederman J., Petty C.R., Dolan C., Hughes S., Mick E., Monuteaux M.C., Faraone S.V. The long-term longitudinal course of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder in ADHD boys: findings from a controlled 10-year prospective longitudinal follow-up study. Psychol. Med. 2008;38(7):1027–1036. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Tuvblad C., Eley T.C., Lichtenstein P. The development of antisocial behaviour from childhood to adolescence. A longitudinal twin study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2005;14(4):216–225. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0458-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Burt S.A., Neiderhiser J.M. Aggressive versus nonaggressive antisocial behavior: distinctive etiological moderation by age. Dev. Psychol. 2009;45(4):1164–1176. doi: 10.1037/a0016130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Siever L.J. Neurobiology of aggression and violence. 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 111.Ballester J., Goldstein B., Goldstein T.R., Yu H., Axelson D., Monk K., Hickey M.B., Diler R.S., Sakolsky D.J., Sparks G., Iyengar S., Kupfer D.J., Brent D.A., Birmaher B. Prospective longitudinal course of aggression among adults with bipolar disorder. 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 112.Dervic K., Garcia-Amador M., Sudol K., Freed P., Brent D.A., Mann J.J., Harkavy-Friedman J.M., Oquendo M.A. Bipolar I and II versus unipolar depression: clinical differences and impulsivity/aggression traits. Eur. Psychiatry. 2015;30(1):106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Lejoyeux M., Nivoli F., Basquin A., Petit A., Chalvin F., Embouazza H. An Investigation of Factors Increasing the Risk of Aggressive Behavior among Schizophrenic Inpatients. Front. Psychiatry. 2013;4:97. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Miles D.R., Carey G. Genetic and environmental architecture of human aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997;72(1):207–217. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Gu Y., Yun L., Tian Y., Hu Z. Association between COMT gene and Chinese male schizophrenic patients with violent behavior. Med. Sci. Monit. 2009;15(9):CR484–CR489. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Isir A.B., Dai A.I., Nacak M., Gorucu S. Study: the lack of significant association of the catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT) gene polymorphism in violent offenders with mental retardation. J. Forensic Sci. 2010;55:225–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2009.01205.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Flory J.D., Xu K., New A.S., Finch T., Goldman D., Siever L.J. Irritable assault and variation in the COMT gene. Psychiatr. Genet. 2007;17(6):344–346. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3281c8f126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Vinkers D.J., Bogaerts S., Van Schaik R.H. MAOA, COMT and 5-HTTLPR frequencies in convicted and never convicted Afro-Caribbeans in the Netherlands. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health. 2014;24(5):365–367. doi: 10.1002/cbm.1908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]