Skip to main content
. 2016 Apr 25;7:560. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560

Table 1.

Summary of studies examining bilingual benefit using a task-switching paradigm (task-cuing paradigm) in adults.

Study Participants Mean age Number of trials Cue Response mappinga RCb Task-switching outcome
Garbin et al., 2010 Bilinguals (N = 19)
Monolinguals (N = 21)
Young adult (20.9) 60 trials (30 mixed-switch and 30 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 50% Switch-cost advantages in bilinguals
Prior and MacWhinney, 2010 Bilinguals (N = 47)
Monolinguals (N = 45)
Young adult (19.1) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) sandwich design Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% Switch-cost advantages in bilinguals
Prior and Gollan, 2011 Spanish-English bilinguals (N = 41)
Mandarin-English bilinguals (N = 43)
Monolinguals (N = 47)
Young adult (19.9) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% Relative switch-cost advantages after controlling for parents' educational level in Spanish-English bilinguals, but not in Chinese-English bilinguals
Soveri et al., 2011 Finnish-Swedish bilinguals (N = 38) Older adult (52.8) 144 trials (64 pure-repeat, 32 mixed-switch, and 48 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal overlapping 50% Mixing-cost advantages in high language switching bilinguals
Hernández et al., 2013; experiment 3 Bilinguals (N = 38)
Monolinguals (N = 39)
Young adult (19.9) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% No bilingual advantages
Paap and Greenberg, 2013 Bilinguals (N = 109)
Monolinguals (N = 144)
Young adult (NA) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% No bilingual advantages
Gold et al., 2013; experiment 1 Older adult bilinguals (N = 15)
Older adult
monolinguals (N = 15)
Older adult (63.7) 240 trials (80 pure-repeat, 80 mixed-switch, 80 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 50% Mixing-cost advantages in bilinguals
Gold et al., 2013; experiment 2 Older adult bilinguals (N = 20)
Younger adult bilinguals (N = 20)
Older adult
monolinguals (N = 20)
Young adult bilinguals (N = 20)
Young adult (31.9) and Older adult (64.2) 240 trials (80 pure-repeat, 80 mixed-switch, 80 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 50% No bilingual advantages (p = 0.056 trend toward bilingual advantages in mixing costs in older adults)
Paap and Sawi, 2014 Bilinguals (N = 58)
Monolinguals (N = 62)
Young adult (24.6) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% No bilingual advantages
Wiseheart et al., 2016 Bilinguals (N = 31)
Monolinguals (N = 37)
Young adult (19.1) 150 trials (50 pure-repeat, 50 mixed-switch and 50 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal overlapping 0% Mixing-cost advantages in bilinguals
Mor et al., 2015 Bilinguals with ADHD (N = 20)
Bilinguals control (N = 20)
Monolingual with ADHD (N = 20)
Monolingual control (N = 20)
Young adult (24.6) 288 trials (144 pure-repeat, 72 mixed-switch and 72 mixed-repeat) sandwich design Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% No bilingual advantages
Qu et al., 2015; ScAc conditionc Chinese-English bilinguals (N = 32)
Chinese> monolinguals (N = 32)
Young adult (21.1) 118 trials (40 pure-repeat, 10 mixed-switch, and 68 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 0% Bilingual advantages in switch costs and monolingual advantages in mixing costs
Qu et al., 2015; ScAc conditionc Chinese-English bilinguals (N = 32)
Chinese> monolinguals (N = 32)
Young adult (21.1) 118 trials (40 pure-repeat, 10 mixed-switch, and 68 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 0% Bilingual advantages in switch costs
Qu et al., 2015; ScAc conditionc Chinese-English bilinguals (N = 32)
Chinese monolinguals (N = 32)
Young adult (21.1) 118 trials (40 pure-repeat, 10 mixed-switch, and 68 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 0% Bilingual advantages in switch costs
Qu et al., 2015; ScAc conditionc Chinese-English bilinguals (N = 32)
Chinese monolinguals (N = 32)
Young adult (21.1) 118 trials (40 pure-repeat, 10 mixed-switch, and 68 mixed-repeat) Verbal overlapping 0% No bilingual advantages
Houtzager et al., 2015 Dutch-Frisian bilinguals (N = 50)
German bilinguals (N = 50)
Older adult (60.2) 192 trials (96 pure-repeat, 48 mixed-switch, and 48 mixed-repeat) Non-verbal non-overlapping 100% Bilingual advantages in switch costs

CSI, cue-to-stimulus interval; RCI, response-to-cue interval; RC, response compatibility; SCL, single-language context; DCL, dual-language context.

a

Overlapping response mapping occurs when each response key is assigned to two responses (e.g., “green” and “triangle”) on the color vs. shape tasks, while non-overlapping response mapping occurs when each response key is assigned to only one response (e.g., “green”).

b

RC (response compatibility) indicates the proportion of trials in which the stimulus and response are compatible in the color-shape switching task. For instance, on compatible trials, the bivalent stimulus (e.g., “green triangle”) correctly matches the response associated with “green” and “triangle.”

c

Qu et al. (2015) manipulated the cognitive demands of suppression and activation across 4 different task-switching tasks. ScAc, suppress one set of conflicting responses while simultaneously activating another set of conflicting responses; ScAϵ, suppress one set of conflicting responses while simultaneously activating another set of non-conflicting responses; SϵAc, suppress one set of non-conflicting responses while simultaneously activating another set of conflicting responses; SϵAϵ, suppress one set of non-conflicting responses while simultaneously activating another set of non-conflicting responses.