Skip to main content
. 2016 Apr 27;11(4):e0153686. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153686

Table 7. Speculation on Cause of Word Aversion.

Experiment Aversion Sound Connotation K χ2(3) χ2(1)
1 (N = 400) Averse (n = 82) 40.2% 39.0% .803 43.462 26.646
Non-averse 11.3% 52.8% V = .571 V = .315
2 (N = 377) Averse (n = 57) 38.6% 49.1% .710 16.694 13.555
Non-averse 15.6% 66.9% V = .498 V = .231
3 (N = 572) Averse (n = 108) 51.9% 37.0% .831 54.175 40.553
Non-averse 19.0% 60.3% V = .533 V = .296
4 (N = 370) Averse (n = 49) 51.0% 38.8% .778 39.501 28.465
Non-averse 15.0% 66.4% V = .566 V = .305
5 (N = 688) Averse (n = 139) 47.5% 43.9% .841 47.743 31.207
Non-averse 19.9% 58.7% V = .415 V = .293

Speculation on the cause of moist aversion in Experiments 1–5 grouped by self-reported moist-aversion. The “sound” column reflects the proportion of participants who identified the sound alone or the sound and connotation as aversive; the “connotation” column reflects the proportion of participants who identified the connotation alone as aversive. K refers to Cohen’s Kappa, a measure of inter-rater reliability. Two chi-square tests of independence are reported for each experiment: one in which all four categories of responses were taken into account, and one in which two categories were taken into account (sound, which collapsed over sound and connotation, and connotation alone; the “other” category was excluded for this second test). All ps < .001. Cramer’s V is reported as a measure of effect size.