Abstract
Background
Use of cigarettes and cannabis frequently co-occurs. We examine the role of genetic and environmental influences on variation in and covariation between tobacco cigarette and cannabis use across European-American (EA) and African-American (AA) women.
Methods
Data on lifetime cannabis and cigarette use were drawn from interviews of 956 AA and 3,557 EA young adult female twins and non-twin same sex female full siblings. Twin modeling was used to decompose variance in and covariance between cigarette and cannabis use into additive genetic, shared, special twin and non-shared environmental sources.
Results
Cigarette use was more common in EAs (75.3%, 95% C.I. 73.8–76.7%) than AAs (64.2%, 95% C.I. 61.2–67.2%) while cannabis use was marginally more commonly reported by AAs (55.5%, 95% C.I. 52.5–58.8%) than EAs (52.4%, 95% C.I. 50.7–54.0%). Additive genetic factors were responsible for 43–66% of the variance in cigarette and cannabis use. Broad shared environmental factors (shared + special twin) played a more significant role in EA (23–29%) than AA (2–15%) women. In AA women, the influence of non-shared environment was more pronounced (42–45% vs. 11–19% in EA women). There was strong evidence for the same familial influences underlying use of bothsubstances (rA=0.82–0.89; rC+T=0.70–0.75). Non-shared environmental factors were also correlated but less so (rE=0.48–0.66). No racial/ethnic differences were apparent in these sources of covariation.
Conclusion
Heritability of cigarette and cannabis use is comparable across racial/ethnic groups. Differences in the contribution of shared and non-shared environmental influences indicate that different factors may shape substance use in EA and AA women.
Keywords: Cannabis, Tobacco, Cigarette, Initiation, Twin, Heritability
1. INTRODUCTION
According to the most recent estimates from the National Survey of Drug Use and Health, 86.8% of lifetime cannabis users aged 12 and older reported a lifetime history of tobacco cigarette use while 61.7% of cigarette smokers also reported smoking cannabis during their lifetime (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), 2014). Adolescents reporting dual use are more likely to experience problems with both drugs, including rapid escalation to more involved stages of use and difficulty quitting (Agrawal et al., 2008; Peters et al., 2012; Timberlake et al., 2007).
Contributors to the co-occurring use of cannabis and cigarettes include risk and protective influences that shape a general liability to experimentation with multiple substances (Hawkins et al., 1992) as well as influences specific to cigarette and cannabis co-use (e.g., shared route of administration; (Agrawal et al., 2012)). Both genetic and environmental influences play a role in the shared vulnerability to cannabis and cigarette use (Agrawal et al., 2010; Han et al., 1999; Young et al., 2006). One study suggested a genetic correlation as high as 0.75 (Agrawal et al., 2010) between cannabis and cigarette use while another suggested a more modest overlap of r = 0.31 (Young et al., 2006). Environmental contributions on these early stages of substance use can be further parsed into those that make members of twin and sibling pairs similar to each other (i.e., shared environment) and those that are individual-specific, with more robust evidence for the shared influences being correlated than the non-shared (Young et al., 2006). However, a study using a subset of the data from this study showed that in African American (AA) women, the relationship between timing of onset of cigarette smoking and cannabis use was prominently attributable to overlapping individual-specific environmental factors (r = 0.95) (Sartor et al., 2009).
The strong evidence for the heritability of and the co-heritability between lifetime use of cannabis and cigarettes comes almost entirely from international research conducted in twin samples of European origin. In U.S. populations, this is particularly problematic given significant variations in the rates of cannabis and cigarette use across race/ethnicity (Garrett et al., 2011; Griesler and Kandel 1998; Keyes et al., 2015; Wallace, Jr. et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2014). Racial/ethnic differences are also particularly pronounced in females with AA adolescent girls and young adult women appearing to be less likely than their European American (EA) counterparts to use cigarettes and cannabis (Garrett et al., 2011; Keyes et al., 2015; SAMHSA, 2014; Wallace, Jr. et al., 2003). In addition, although cigarette use typically predates cannabis use in EAs, reverse gateways (cannabis before cigarettes/alcohol) are somewhat more common in AAs than EAs (Sartor et al., 2013; Vaughn et al., 2008). Notably, these variations in prevalence and sequence may relate to differing societal attitudes towards cannabis and cigarette use, the relative availability and exposure opportunity of the two drugs as well as to putative differences in biological response to anticipation and receipt of drug-related rewards. For cigarette use, we are only aware of 3 studies, including two by us in the sample under study here, that show that additive genetic factors explain similar proportions of variance (40–50%) in AAs and EAs (Sartor et al., 2009, 2015; Whitfield et al., 2007). However, in a recent study by our group (Sartor et al., 2015), the remainder of the variance in cigarette use was solely attributable to individual-specific environmental factors (44%) in AA twins while in EA twins, substantial influence of both individual-specific (10%) and shared environmental factors (34%) was noted for cigarette use. Likewise, we have previously reported that timing to cannabis use is heritable in AA female twins (0.52) and that the role of shared environment is limited (Sartor et al., 2009). However, no study to date has examined the bivariate relationship between lifetime use of cannabis and cigarettes in AA and EA twins.
In the current study, we utilize a large, general population sample of adult female twins and non-twin siblings of self-described AA (n=956) and EA (n=3557) ancestry to examine the role of additive genetic, shared environmental and individual-specific environmental influences on the covariance between lifetime cigarette and cannabis use and the extent to which the magnitude of their contribution varies across race/ethnicity.
We leveraged a sample of females who are notably understudied in addiction research. Importantly, AA females appear to be at low risk for both cannabis and cigarette involvement, relative to EA females, both during adolescence (Keyes et al., 2015; Wallace, Jr. et al., 2003) and adulthood (SAMHSA, 2014). Thus, access to related individuals of AA ancestry is a unique aspect of the present study – we are also not aware of other datasets of this magnitude with AA twins. Further, by utilizing a young adult sample, we circumvented concerns regarding lack of adequate opportunity for experimentation with cannabis (Wagner and Anthony 2002).
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1 Participants
The sample was composed of female twins who completed the fourth wave of data collection for the Missouri Adolescent Female Twin Study MOAFTS and female participants from the Missouri Family Study (MOFAM). Data on male twins were not collected in MOAFTS, although male siblings did participate in MOFAM but were not included in the present study.
2.1.1 MOAFTS
The Missouri Adolescent Female Twin Study (MOAFTS; Heath et al., 2002; Knopik et al., 2005) is a population-based longitudinal study of female twin pairs born between July 1, 1975 and June 30, 1985 in Missouri to Missouri-resident parents The sample was demographically representative of the Missouri population at the time the twins were born, with nearly 15% of twins being African-American (AA) and the remainder being of European-American (EA) descent. A baseline interview was conducted with 3,258 twins beginning in 1995 (median age=15 years). All available twins were targeted for three waves of telephone interviews (Waves 1, 4, and 5, at median ages 15, 22, and 24 years, respectively). Between 2002 and 2005, all twins from the target cohort (excluding those who had withdrawn from the study or whose parents asked that the family not be re-contacted) were contacted for Wave 4 interviews. As all twins (N=3,787) were 18 years of age or older at the time of recruitment for Wave 4, sensitive questions regarding their illicit substance use was queried. Therefore, we limited the sample to MOAFTS participants who completed wave 4 interviews, but data from other waves (including the subsequent Wave 5, conducted from 2005 to 2008), which were available for over 95% of Wave 4 participants, were integrated as well. The Wave 4 sample consisted of 1,038 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs, 735 dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs and 241 twins whose co-twins did not participate.
The MOAFTS protocol was approved by the Washington University School of Medicine Human Research Protections Office. All twins 18 years old or older gave informed consent prior to study participation.
2.1.2 MOFAM
MOFAM is a longitudinal family study that included high-risk and low-risk subjects and was designed to investigate the impact of paternal alcoholism on offspring outcomes in an ethnically diverse sample of youth, with oversampling of AA families (55%) to increase the statistical power to detect differences in outcomes by race/ethnicity. As detailed elsewhere (Calvert et al., 2010), between 2003 and 2009, Missouri state birth records were used to identify families with at least one child aged 13, 15, 17 or 19 years (the same age range targeted in MOAFTS) and at least one full sibling aged 13 or older. Biological mothers completed brief telephone screening interviews to determine level of familial risk for alcoholism. Families in which the mother reported that the biological father had a history of excessive drinking were classified as “high risk.” All others were classified as “low risk.” An additional group of families was selected from men identified through driving records as having 2 or more drunk-driving convictions and classified as “very high risk.” Sample enrollment occurred over 6 years. A total of 731 females (of 1,461 offspring interviewed) completed at least one interview. For the current analyses, 163 full-sibling pairs, 30 full-sibling trios, and 315 individuals with no female sibling interview data were included – 81% of these women were interviewed at least twice. Of the 511 women who were recruited in the first 3 years and were interviewed at least once, 89% had 2 or more, and 73% had 3 or more follow up interviews. Rates are comparable across EA and AA women. These retention rates are quite good, particularly in light of the high risk nature of the families to which these women belonged.
The MOFAM study protocol was approved by the Washington University School of Medicine Human Research Protections Office and by the Ethics Board of the State Department of Health and Senior Services in accordance with regulations governing the use of vital records in research. All subjects aged 18 and older provided informed consent prior to interview, with parental consent and offspring assent obtained for those under age 18 prior to participation.
2.2 Procedure and Assessment Battery for MOAFTS and MOFAM
MOAFTS and MOFAM assessments were nearly identical by design to facilitate integration of data across studies (with adjustments for differences in ascertainment strategies). In both studies, data were collected via telephone interview by trained interviewers using a modified version of the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA; Bucholz et al., 1994). The SSAGA was designed to assess substance use history, psychiatric disorders, and other related psychosocial domains. Other than the wave 5 MOAFTS interview, which covered the 2 years between Wave 4 and 5 assessments, interviews queried lifetime psychiatric and psychosocial history.
Lifetime Cigarette use
Cigarette use was defined as ever having smoked one or more cigarettes. Participants who responded “Yes” to the question “Have you ever tried a cigarette?” at any wave of data collection were classified as positive for use.
Lifetime Cannabis use
Participants who responded “Yes” to the question “Have you ever used marijuana?” at any wave of data collection were classified as positive for use.
Those who reported a lifetime history of use were also asked about how old they were when they first used the substance.
2.3 Data Analysis: Twin-Sibling Modeling
Quantitative genetic analyses were used to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors to use of cigarettes and use of cannabis, and the overlap between them. Classical twin models were used to estimate the role of additive genetic factors (A), shared environmental factors (C; environmental influences that make twins similar to each other), and non-shared environmental factors (E; environmental influences not shared by twins, as well as error variance). Non-additive genetic influences (D) were not modeled as there was no evidence for them from inspection of the twin correlations (Neale and Cardon 1992). Correlations in DZ twin and non-twin siblings, who share their genetic material to the same extent, were compared and differences in these correlations were attributed to an additional special twin environment parameter (T; sources of twin similarity that do not relate to non-twin sibling resemblance; e.g., sharing a classroom, in-utero effects).
A bivariate model was fitted to raw categorical data from twins and siblings in the statistical software package Mx (Neale, 2004), which uses full information maximum likelihood estimation. A series of sub-models were tested to assess statistical significance of the A, C, T, and E influences and whether these influences could be equated across AA and EA subsamples. The difference between the -2 log likelihood fit of the full model and the nested sub-model, which is distributed as chi-square for the given degrees of freedom, was used to determine relative fit. All twin models were adjusted for age and study design variables (MOFAM low risk, high risk or very high risk).
3. RESULTS
3.1 Substance Use
Cigarette use was more common in EAs (75.3%) relative to AAs (64.2%) while cannabis use was marginally, but significantly, more commonly reported by the AA (55.5%) than the EA (52.4%) participants. Cigarette use (mean age at onset ~14 years) typically preceded cannabis use (mean age at onset ~17 years) in both racial/ethnic groups (see Table 1). As shown in Table 1, 50.3 and 46.4% of EA and AA participants respectively reported using both substances. While a fair proportion of women reported cigarette use alone (25.0 and 17.8% for EA and AA respectively), cannabis use in the absence of cigarette use was uncommon but more prevalent in AA (9.3%) relative to EA (2.1%) women. Reverse gateways (i.e., onset of cannabis use before the onset of cigarette use, by at least one year) were uncommon in general, however AA women (23.5%) were more likely than EA women (5.4%) to endorse this pattern of onsets.
Table 1.
African-American n=956 | European-American n=3557 | |
---|---|---|
MOFAM participant % (n) | 42.3% (N=405) | 9.2% (N=326) |
MOAFTS participant % (n) | 57.6% (N=551) | 90.8% (N=3231) |
Study Design Group | ||
MOFAM low risk | 17.6% (N=168) | 2.8% (N=98) |
MOFAM high risk | 12.8% (N=122) | 2.0% (N=70) |
MOFAM very high risk | 12.0% (N=115) | 4.4% (N=158) |
MOAFTS (general population) | 57.6% (N=551) | 90.8% (N=3231) |
MOAFTS: Monozygotic twin (MZ), % (n) | 24.6% (N=235, with 111 pairs) | 50.3% (N=1790, with 853 pairs) |
MOAFTS: Dizygotic twin (DZ), % (n) | 33.1% (N=316, with 143 pairs) | 40.5 (N=1441, with 663 pairs) |
MOFAM: Full Sibling (FS), % (n) | 42.4 (N=405, with 88 pairs and 13 trios) | 9.2 (N=326, with 75 pairs and 17 trios) |
Mean age at first interview (SD) | 18.0 (3.8) | 16.8 (3.3) |
MOFAM | 17.8 (3.7) | 17.3 (3.4) |
MOAFTS | 18.2 (3.9) | 16.7 (3.3) |
Mean age at last interview (SD) | 23.5 (3.9) | 24.2 (3.0) |
MOFAM | 21.6 (4.3) | 21.5 (4.1) |
MOAFTS | 24.8 (2.8) | 24.5 (2.7) |
Ever smoked a cigarette*, % | 64.2% (61.2 – 67.2) | 75.3% (73.8 – 76.7) |
MOFAM | 59.5% (54.7 – 64.3) | 66.6% (61.4 – 71.7) |
MOAFTS | 67.6% (63.7 – 71.6) | 76.1% (74.7 – 77.6) |
Mean age at cigarette use (SD) | 14.6 (3.5) | 14.1 (3.2) |
MOFAM | 14.9 (3.4) | 14.2 (3.4) |
MOAFTS | 14.4 (3.6) | 14.1 (3.2) |
Ever tried cannabis*, % | 55.5% (52.5 – 58.8) | 52.4% (50.7 – 54.0) |
MOFAM | 52.8% (48.0 – 57.7) | 50.6% (45.2 – 56.0) |
MOAFTS | 57.7% (53.6 – 61.8) | 52.5% (50.8 – 54.2) |
Mean age at cannabis use (SD) | 16.6 (2.7) | 16.7 (2.5) |
MOFAM | 16.0 (2.4) | 16.0 (2.5) |
MOAFTS | 17.1 (2.7) | 16.8 (2.5) |
Substance Use Profile | ||
Neither substance* | 26.5% (23.7 – 29.3) (N=253) | 22.7% (21.3 – 24.0) (N=806) |
Cigarettes only* | 17.8% (15.4 – 20.2) (N=170) | 25.0% (23.6 – 26.4) (N=889) |
Cannabis only* | 9.3% (7.5 – 11.2) (N=89) | 2.1% (1.6 – 2.6) (N=74) |
Both cigarettes and cannabis* | 46.4% (43.2 – 49.6) (N=443) | 50.3% (48.6 – 51.9) (N=1788) |
Substance use sequence | ||
Cigarettes first* | 61.3% (56.7 – 65.8) (N=269) | 80.3% (78.4 – 82.2) (N=1418) |
Both within same year | 15.3% (11.9 – 18.6) (N=67) | 14.3% (12.6 – 15.9) (N=252) |
Cannabis first* | 23.5% (19.5 – 27.4) (N=103) | 5.4% (4.4 – 6.5) (N=96) |
Significantly different between EA and AA women
3.2 Twin-Sibling Models
Twin and sibling pair correlations were lower in AA compared to EA women (Table 2). Details of the model-fitting procedure are presented in Supplementary Table 11. The model-fitting indicated that there were significant familial influences on both cigarette and cannabis use in AAs; however, we could not determine the extent to which this familiality was attributable to genetic, shared environmental, and/or twin specific environmental factors (either all A parameters could be dropped or all C+T parameters could be dropped, but a model dropping A, C, and T parameters simultaneously was rejected). For EAs, there were genetic influences on both cigarette and cannabis use and either source of familial environment, C or T, could be excluded from the model, but not both.
Table 2.
rMZ (MOAFTS) | rDZ (MOAFTS) | rFS (MOFAM) | |
---|---|---|---|
African Americans | N=111 pairs | N=143 pairs | N=127 pairsa |
Use of Cigarettes | 0.59 (0.35 – 0.82) | 0.28 (0.02 – 0.54) | 0.33 (0.08 – 0.58) |
Use of Cannabis | 0.63 (0.42 – 0.84) | 0.30 (0.06 – 0.54) | 0.53 (0.28 – 0.77) |
European Americans | N=853 pairs | N=663 pairs | N=126 pairsb |
Use of Cigarettes | 0.90 (0.86 – 0.93) | 0.64 (0.54 – 0.73) | 0.53 (0.31 – 0.75) |
Use of Cannabis | 0.80 (0.75 – 0.85) | 0.57 (0.47 – 0.66) | 0.61 (0.40 – 0.81) |
NOTE: all correlations significant at p < 0.05
includes 88 families with two siblings interviewed, and 13 families with three siblings interviewed (yielding three pairs of respondents each)
includes 75 families with two siblings interviewed, and 17 families with three siblings interviewed (yielding three pairs of respondents each)
Based on this pattern of results, for both racial/ethnic groups and substances, we proceeded with a model that allowed for A and E and combined the sources of familial environment (C+T; broad family environment) into a single parameter (Table 3). Using this model, we noted that, for each substance, while either A or C+T could be equated across racial/ethnic groups, the total extent of familial variance (A+C+T) was substantially greater in EA relative to AA women (i.e., for cigarettes: 54% in AA vs. 89% in EA; for cannabis: 58% in AA vs. 81% in EA). Consequently, the role of individual-specific environment was significantly less pronounced in EA women, particularly for cigarette use. Sensitivity analyses revealed that estimates of E for cigarette use in AA women might be 2.15 to 7.35 times greater than those for their EA counterparts (Supplemental Figure 12), and that for cannabis use, the E in AA women might be 1.10 to 3.70 times greater than the EA estimate (Supplemental Figure 23).
Table 3.
Additive Genetic | Broad Family Environment (C+T) | Non-shared Environment | |
---|---|---|---|
African-Americans | |||
Use of Cigarettes | 0.52 (0.00 – 0.73) | 0.02 (0.00 – 0.47) | 0.45* (0.27 – 0.76) |
Use of Cannabis | 0.43 (0.00 – 0.78) | 0.15 (0.00 – 0.55) | 0.42* (0.22 – 0.67) |
European Americans | |||
Use of Cigarettes | 0.66* (0.46 – 0.88) | 0.23* (0.03 – 0.43) | 0.11* (0.07 – 0.15) |
Use of Cannabis | 0.52* (0.32 – 0.72) | 0.29* (0.09 – 0.45) | 0.19* (0.15 – 0.25) |
Indicates variance component significant at p < 0.05
Finally, we estimated the extent of genetic (rA), broad familial environmental (rC+T) and individual-specific environmental (rE) correlations for both racial/ethnic groups (Table 4). We were unable to resolve the extent to which rA or rC+T contributed to the covariance between cigarette and cannabis use in AA twins. However both genetic and family environmental sources of covariance could not be simultaneously constrained to zero, indicating overlapping sources of familial influence with insufficient power to determine the source of the familial overlap. For the EA twins, both rA and rC+T were significant and substantial; confidence limits indicated the possibility of complete overlap across substances in both sources of variance. Despite racial/ethnic differences in the magnitude of E for each substance, rE was moderate for both AAs and EAs, and could be equated in magnitude across the racial/ethnic groups.
Table 4.
Cross-substance rA | Cross-substance rC | Cross-substance rE | |
---|---|---|---|
African-Americans | 0.82 (−1.00 – 0.91) | 0.75 (−1.00 – 1.00) | 0.48* (0.20 – 0.79) |
European Americans | 0.89* (0.71 – 1.00) | 0.70* (0.26 – 1.00) | 0.66* (0.41 – 0.83) |
Note: rA=additive genetic correlation, rC=family and twin-specific environmental correlation, rE=non-shared environmental correlation.
indicates p < 0.05
4. DISCUSSION
To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the role of genetic and environmental influences on cannabis use, and on covariation between cannabis use and cigarette use, separately in EA and AA women. Our study also includes the largest number of AA twins currently available for the study of substance use. We broadly replicated existing racial/ethnic trends in cannabis and cigarette use with one exception in that rates of cannabis use in our sample were comparable, if not marginally higher in AA than EA women and this difference was more pronounced in MOAFTS, which is a general population twin sample. As we relied on multiple longitudinal reports of cannabis use, it is possible that our study design allowed participants greater opportunity to admit to a potentially illicit behavior, particularly during adulthood.
We have previously reported on univariate estimates for cigarette use in the same sample (Sartor et al., 2015); that study found that while genetic influences made similar contributions in both racial/ethnic groups, EAs and AAs diverged in the extent to which environmental factors shaped their cigarette use. While the influence of familial environmental effects were quite pronounced for EA women in the univariate analysis, non-genetic sources of variance in AA women were almost entirely individual-specific in nature. The current study reveals similar racial/ethnic differences exist for cannabis use; familial environment was somewhat important in EA twins while individual-specific environmental factors dominated in AA pairs.
The relative contribution of individual-specific environmental factors varied, both across substances and racial/ethnic groups. In EA women, factors specific to individual members of a twin pair were modest but nearly twice as prominent (and statistically different) for cannabis (19%) as for cigarette use (11%). The role of E on both substances was also considerably greater in AA (42–45%) versus EA (11–19%) women. Estimation of E arises from the deviation of the MZ twin correlation from unity (i.e., rMZ≠1; (Evans et al., 2002)) and reflects person-specific factors as well as measurement error, although the latter is unlikely to be a major concern for simple binary indices of lifetime substance use in an adult population. Such differences in twin concordance across substances and ethnicities (i.e., overall: AA > EA; in EA: Cannabis > Cigarette) might be due to variations in social attitudes towards and relative availability of substances across ethnic groups, and within the EA twins, to differences in the legal status of the drugs (Boardman et al., 2010; Shanahan and Hofer 2005). For instance, while cannabis is more socially accessible in AA populations (Wallace and Muroff 2002), AA girls (but not boys) are less likely than their EA counterparts to report lifetime and recent cannabis use (Schepis et al., 2011). Another possible contributor to reduced familial and increased E variance in AA twins and siblings may be exposure to an authoritarian form of parental monitoring (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008), which has been shown to be more associated with reduction in substance involvement in AA than in EA youth. Interestingly, studies have shown that the relative importance of familial sources of variance is attenuated in the presence of increased parental supervision (Dick et al., 2007). An alternate explanation is reduced power associated with the notably smaller number of AA pairs (e.g., for MZ: 111 vs. 853, Table 2). Despite this limitation, confidence limits on the correlations suggest that the reduced AA rMZ and rDZ are meaningful.
Despite differences in the relative magnitude of E influences on variance in cannabis and cigarette use and, importantly, across racial/ethnic groups, the degree to which these factors influenced covariance between the two substances did not vary across EA and AA women. About 23–44% (Table 4) of the individual specific environmental variance in cannabis and cigarette use was shared. Thus, while the magnitude of person-specific influences differed for each substance, the qualitative nature of those influences was similar across them.
In contrast to the variability in estimates of E on individual differences in and between cannabis and cigarette use, the role of genetic influences was comparable across substances and racial/ethnic groups. Heritability estimates (43–66%; Table 3) were similar across racial/ethnic groups and approximated reports from other studies of European twin cohorts (Madden et al., 2004; Maes et al., 2006; Pergadia et al., 2006; Verweij et al., 2010; Vink et al., 2005). Results from the bivariate model suggested that these genetic influences were highly and possibly, perfectly correlated across the substances (Table 4). Such shared genetic factors could include predisposition to a third, heritable trait, such as a general liability to disinhibited behaviors (Hicks et al., 2011) or a shared vulnerability to the use of drugs that utilize combustion/inhalation as the main route of administration (Agrawal and Lynskey 2009; van Leeuwen et al., 2011). Alternatively, genes related to cigarette use may also be linked to onset of cannabis use, such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). The BDNF variant rs6265 has been linked to cigarette use at p = 1.8 × 10−8 (Tobacco and Genetics Consortium 2010). A recent study also related this variant to cannabis use (Agrawal et al., 2015). Another study examining polygenic scores derived from a genomewide association study of tobacco smoking found these scores to predict a modest but significant proportion of variance in cannabis use as well (Vink et al., 2014).
While we could not disentangle shared and special twin environmental influences from each other, likely due to low power, there was evidence that broad shared environmental factors were more significant for EA than AA women. This observation aligns well with multiple studies showing that AA youth are less vulnerable to peer effects (Conn and Marks 2014; Mason et al., 2014; Wallace and Muroff 2002) and deviant sibling influences (Catalano et al., 1992) and that, in fact, EA women are most susceptible to peer attitudes towards substance use (Mason et al., 2014). Even though religious attendance is more common in AA youth, its protective association with substance use is more pronounced in EA youth (Wallace and Muroff 2002). As these factors are commonly shared by twin and sibling pairs, we anticipate that they contribute to variance in EA but not AA women.
Other notable limitations of our study include the possibility of retrospective recall bias; however as the sample is relatively young, we anticipate the effects of recall bias to be minimal. Second, as our sample consists of young women, results may not extrapolate to other demographic groups. Third, the binary indices used in this study reflect initiation and there is significant variability in the extent of cigarette and cannabis use that is not captured by them (i.e., used once or twice versus daily/problem users). Thus, we are uncertain about where in the spectrum of liability these race/ethnic differences may be occurring. Fourth, we did not assess whether participants were also smoking products that combined cannabis and tobacco. While the practice of adding tobacco to cannabis joints is uncommon in the present population (and rare in the United States; Ream et al., 2008), its role should be carefully examined in international samples where the practice is common (Belanger et al., 2011) even in individuals who do not report cigarette smoking (Belanger et al., 2013; Gage et al., 2014). We also did not query participants about blunt smoking (i.e., rolling marijuana in cigar wrappers, which may contain a small, residual amount of tobacco). The practice of blunt smoking is more common in AA populations but less so in women (Fairman 2015; Timberlake 2013). Fifth, rates of cigarette and cannabis use were somewhat lower in MOFAM, which might have impacted our estimation of C+T as MOFAM was the sole source of nontwin siblings. MOFAM women were somewhat younger than MOAFTS women at their last assessment (22 vs. 25 years, Table 1) and may have been marginally less likely to have surpassed the full risk period for onset of cigarette and cannabis use. This age and sample effect was accounted for in all twin modeling, nonetheless combining a general population cohort of twins with a sample with overrepresentation of high-risk families may have influenced our findings.
Our study indicates that while heritable variation in cigarette and cannabis use is comparable across racial/ethnic groups, the impact of familial versus individual-specific sources of environmental influence vary markedly across EA and AA women. Further unpacking the substance use trajectory to identify precisely when these racial/ethnic differences emerge will be critical for future studies.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Heritability of cigarette and cannabis use is similar across ethnic groups.
No ethnic differences in genetic or environmental influences on covariance.
Shared environment more important in European-Americans.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at http://dx.doi.org and by entering doi:…
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at http://dx.doi.org and by entering doi:…
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at http://dx.doi.org and by entering doi:…
Conflict of interest: AA received peer-reviewed funding and a travel honorarium from ABMRF/Foundation for Alcohol Research prior to 12/31/12. Other authors have no interests to declare.
References
- Agrawal A, Budney AJ, Lynskey MT. The co-occurring use and misuse of cannabis and tobacco: a review. Addiction. 2012;107:1221–1233. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03837.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. Tobacco and cannabis co-occurrence: does route of administration matter? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;99:240–247. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Lynskey MT, Kapoor M, Bucholz KK, Edenberg HJ, Schuckit M, Brooks A, Hesselbrock V, Kramer J, Saccone N, Tischfield J, Bierut LJ. Are genetic variants for tobacco smoking associated with cannabis involvement? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;150:183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.02.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Heath AC, Lynskey MT. Transitions to regular smoking and to nicotine dependence in women using cannabis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;95:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Silberg JL, Lynskey MT, Maes HH, Eaves LJ. Mechanisms underlying the lifetime co-occurrence of tobacco and cannabis use in adolescent and young adult twins. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;108:49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belanger RE, Akre C, Kuntsche E, Gmel G, Suris JC. Adding tobacco to cannabis–its frequency and likely implications. Nicotine Tob Res. 2011;13:746–750. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belanger RE, Marclay F, Berchtold A, Saugy M, Cornuz J, Suris JC. To what extent does adding tobacco to cannabis expose young users to nicotine? Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15:1832–1838. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boardman JD, Blalock CL, Pampel FC. Trends in the genetic influences on smoking. J Health Soc Behav. 2010;51:108–123. doi: 10.1177/0022146509361195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bucholz KK, Cadoret RJ, Cloninger RC, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock V, Nurnberger JI, Reich T, Schmidt I, Schuckit MA. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies. J Stud Alcohol. 1994;55:149–158. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvert WJ, Keenan BK, Steger-May K. Early drinking and its association with adolescents’ participation in risky behaviors. J Am Psychiatr Nurses Assoc. 2010;16:239–25. doi: 10.1177/1078390310374356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catalano RF, Morrison DM, Wells EA, Gillmore MR, Iritani B, Hawkins JD. Ethnic differences in family factors related to early drug initiation. J Stud Alcohol. 1992;53:208–217. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1992.53.208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conn BM, Marks AK. Ethnic/racial differences in peer and parent influence on adolescent prescription drug misuse. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35:257–265. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dick DM, Viken R, Purcell S, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ. Parental monitoring moderates the importance of genetic and environmental influences on adolescent smoking. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:213–218. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans DM, Gillespie NA, Martin NG. Biometrical genetics. Biol Psychol. 2002;61:33–51. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00051-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairman BJ. Cannabis problem experiences among users of the tobacco-cannabis combination known as blunts. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;150:77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gage SH, Hickman M, Heron J, Munafo MR, Lewis G, Macleod J, Zammit S. Associations of cannabis and cigarette use with psychotic experiences at age 18: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Psychol Med. 2014;44:3435–3444. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett BE, Dube SR, Trosclair A, Caraballo RS, Pechacek TF. Cigarette smoking – United States, 1965–2008. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2011;60(Suppl):109–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griesler PC, Kandel DB. Ethnic differences in correlates of adolescent cigarette smoking. J Adolesc Health. 1998;23:167–180. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(98)00029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han C, McGue MK, Iacono WG. Lifetime tobacco, alcohol and other substance use in adolescent Minnesota twins: univariate and multivariate behavioral genetic analyses. Addiction. 1999;94:981–993. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9479814.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:64–105. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath AC, Howells W, Bucholz KK, Glowinski AL, Nelson EC, Madden PA. Ascertainment of a mid-western US female adolescent twin cohort for alcohol studies: assessment of sample representativeness using birth record data. Twin Res. 2002;5:107–112. doi: 10.1375/1369052022974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hicks BM, Schalet BD, Malone SM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Psychometric and genetic architecture of substance use disorder and behavioral disinhibition measures for gene association studies. Behav Genet. 2011;41:459–475. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9417-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes KM, Vo T, Wall MM, Caetano R, Suglia SF, Martins SS, Galea S, Hasin D. Racial/ethnic differences in use of alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana: is there a cross-over from adolescence to adulthood? Soc Sci Med. 2015;124:132–141. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.11.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knopik VS, Sparrow EP, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Hudziak JJ, Reich W, Slutske WS, Grant JD, McLaughlin TL, Todorov A, Todd RD, Heath AC. Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk: a female twin study. Psychol Med. 2005;35:625–635. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704004155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden PA, Pedersen NL, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo MJ, Martin NG. The epidemiology and genetics of smoking initiation and persistence: crosscultural comparisons of twin study results. Twin Res. 2004;7:82–97. doi: 10.1375/13690520460741471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maes HH, Neale MC, Kendler KS, Martin NG, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Genetic and cultural transmission of smoking initiation: n extended twin kinship model. Behav Genet. 2006;36:795–808. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9085-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason MJ, Mennis J, Linker J, Bares C, Zaharakis N. Peer attitudes effects on adolescent substance use: the moderating role of race and gender. Prev Sci. 2014;15:56–64. doi: 10.1007/s11121-012-0353-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neale MC. Statistical Modeling with Mx. Dept. of Psychiatry, Box # 980710, Richmond VA 23298 2004 [Google Scholar]
- Neale MC, Cardon LR. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Kluwer Academic Publishers; Netherlands: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Pergadia ML, Heath AC, Agrawal A, Bucholz KK, Martin NG, Madden PA. The implications of simultaneous smoking initiation for inferences about the genetics of smoking behavior from twin data. Behav Genet. 2006;36:567–576. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9042-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters EN, Budney AJ, Carroll KM. Clinical correlates of co-occurring cannabis and tobacco use: a systematic review. Addiction. 2012;107:1404–1417. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03843.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ream GL, Benoit E, Johnson BD, Dunlap E. Smoking tobacco along with marijuana increases symptoms of cannabis dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;95:199–208. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartor CE, Agrawal A, Lynskey MT, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Heath AC. Common genetic influences on the timing of first use for alcohol, cigarettes, and cannabis in young African-American women. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;102:49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartor CE, Agrawal A, Lynskey MT, Duncan AE, Grant JD, Nelson EC, Madden PA, Heath AC, Bucholz KK. Cannabis or alcohol first? Differences by ethnicity and in risk for rapid progression to cannabis-related problems in women. Psychol Med. 2013;43:813–823. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartor CE, Grant JD, Agrawal A, Sadler B, Madden PAF, Heath AC, Bucholz KK. Genetic and environmental contributions to initiation of cigarette smoking in young African-American and European-American women. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;157:54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schepis TS, Desai RA, Cavallo DA, Smith AE, McFetridge A, Liss TB, Potenza MN, Krishnan-Sarin S. Gender differences in adolescent marijuana use and associated psychosocial characteristics. J Addict Med. 2011;5:65–73. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181d8dc62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shanahan MJ, Hofer SM. Social context in gene-environment interactions: retrospect and prospect. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005;60(Spec No 1):65–76. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.special_issue_1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. National Survey on Drug Use and Health, 2013. ICPSR35509-V1. United States Department of Health and Human Services. Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor]; Ann Arbor, MI: 2014. http://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR35509.v1. [Google Scholar]
- Tamis-LeMonda CS, Briggs RD, McClowry SG, Snow DL. Challenges to the Study of African American Parenting: Conceptualization, Sampling, research approaches, measurement, and design. Parent Sci Pract. 2008;8:319–358. [Google Scholar]
- Timberlake DS. The changing demographic of blunt smokers across birth cohorts. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;130:129–134. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timberlake DS, Haberstick BC, Hopfer CJ, Bricker J, Sakai JT, Lessem JM, Hewitt JK. Progression from marijuana use to daily smoking and nicotine dependence in a national sample of U.S. adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88:272–281. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tobacco and Genetics Consortium. Genome-wide meta-analyses identify multiple loci associated with smoking behavior. Nat Genet. 2010;42:441–447. doi: 10.1038/ng.571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Leeuwen AP, Verhulst FC, Reijneveld SA, Vollebergh WA, Ormel J, Huizink AC. Can the gateway hypothesis, the common liability model and/or, the route of administration model predict initiation of cannabis use during adolescence? A survival analysis–the TRAILS study. J Adolesc Health. 2011;48:73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaughn M, Wallace J, Perron B, Copeland V, Howard M. Does marijuana use serve as a gateway to cigarette use for high-risk African-American youth? Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2008;34:782–791. doi: 10.1080/00952990802455477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verweij KJ, Zietsch BP, Lynskey MT, Medland SE, Neale MC, Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Vink JM. Genetic and environmental influences on cannabis use initiation and problematic use: a meta-analysis of twin studies. Addiction. 2010;105:417–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02831.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vink JM, Hottenga JJ, de Geus EJ, Willemsen G, Neale MC, Furberg H, Boomsma DI. Polygenic risk scores for smoking: predictors for alcohol and cannabis use? Addiction. 2014;109:1141–1151. doi: 10.1111/add.12491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vink JM, Willemsen G, Boomsma DI. Heritability of smoking initiation and nicotine dependence. Behav Genet. 2005;35:397–406. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-1327-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner FA, Anthony JC. Into the world of illegal drug use: exposure opportunity and other mechanisms linking the use of alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, and cocaine. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155:918–925. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.10.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace JM, Jr, Bachman JG, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Cooper SM, Johnston LD. Gender and ethnic differences in smoking, drinking and illicit drug use among American 8th, 10th and 12th grade students, 1976–2000. Addiction. 2003;98:225–234. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00282.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace JM, Muroff JR. Preventing substance abuse among African American children and youth: race differences in risk factor exposure and vulnerability. J Prim Prev. 2002;22:235–261. [Google Scholar]
- Whitfield KE, King G, Moller S, Edwards CL, Nelson T, Vandenbergh D. Concordance rates for smoking among African-American twins. J Natl Med Assoc. 2007;99:213–217. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu LT, Brady KT, Mannelli P, Killeen TK. Cannabis use disorders are comparatively prevalent among nonwhite racial/ethnic groups and adolescents: a national study. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;50:26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.11.010. epub;%2013 Dec 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young SE, Rhee SH, Stallings MC, Corley RP, Hewitt JK. Genetic and environmental vulnerabilities underlying adolescent substance use and problem use: general or specific? Behav Genet. 2006;36:603–615. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9066-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.