Skip to main content
. 2016 Apr 22;3:379–390. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2016.04.003

Table 1.

Studies on physical activity, motor skills and cognitive development.

Reference Study design/country Characteristics of study Physical activity measure Outcome measure(s) Main results
Physical activity
Becker et al. (2014) Cross-sectional single-group. USA
  • N = 51 children

  • Average age 4.8 years

  • 43.1% female

  • 49% enrolled in Head Start

  • 19.6% Spanish speakers

  • No SES information

  • ActiGraph GT1M accelerometer during 30 min of free outdoor play during preschool

  • Head–Toes–Knees–Shoulders task (HTKS) for self-regulation

  • The Letter-Word Identification subtest of the Woodcock–Johnson (to assess literacy)

  • The Applied Problems subtest of the Woodcock–Johnson (to assess math skills)

  • Active play positively correlated with self-regulation and positively correlated with math achievement

  • Active play did not significantly predict math or emergent literacy scores

  • Active play had a significant indirect effect with math scores as well as emergent literacy scores through HTKS

Kirk et al. (2014) Quasi-experimental. USA
  • N = 72 children

  • Intervention group, n = 51

  • average age 3.8 years

  • 56.9% female

  • Control group, n = 21

  • average age 3.9 years

  • 42.9% female

  • No race/ethnicity or SES information

  • Direct observation

  • System for Observing Fitness Instruction Time (SOFIT)

  • rating 3 times per week

  • assessed at 3 and 6 months

  • Intervention

  • Curriculum for 15 min of moderate physical activity 2 × 30 min total

  • Pre-school Literacy Individual Growth and Development Indicators (IGDIs)

  • Assessed at 3 and 6 months of intervention

  • Measured literacy and language

  • Picture Naming assessment

  • Rhyming assessment

  • Alliteration Assessment

  • Compared to control group, intervention group showed significant improvements in alliteration and picture naming between baseline and 3 to 6 months

Mavilidi et al. (2015) Cluster randomized-controlled rrial. Australia
  • N = 111 preschool children

  • Mean age 4.9 years

  • 15 child-care centers (4 centers in integrated condition, 4 centers in non-integrated, 4 centers in gesturing, and 3 centers in conventional control)

  • Actigraph acceleromter

  • 4 conditions administered over 4 weeks:

  • 1.

    Integrated physical exercise condition (vigorous exercise related to the word)

  • 2.

    Non-integrated physical exercise condition (vigorous exercise not related to the word)

  • 3.

    Gesturing condition (child seated, gesture related to word)

  • 4.

    Conventional condition (child seated and repeat the word)

  • Free-recall and cued recall of 14 Italian vocabulary words tested during intervention (at 2 weeks), immediately after intervention (at 4 weeks) and 6 weeks after intervention (10 weeks). [Free recall is asking child to name as many words as they can, cued recall is asking the child to use the Italian word for a picture.]

  • Children exposed to integrated physical exercise condition could freely recall more Italian words than children exposed to other conditions (non-integrated physical exercise, gesturing condition, and control condition) during, immediately after, and 6 weeks after the intervention

  • Children exposed to any movement (integrated physical activity, non-integrated exercise, or gesturing) outperformed the children in the sedentary control condition in the cued recall task.

Mierau et al. (2014) Cross-over design — subjects begin with either exercise or a control condition. Germany
  • N = 10 children

  • Average age 5.8 years

  • 100% male

  • Weight and height in the 10th and 90th percentile, respectively

  • No race/ethnicity or SES information

  • Assessed at ages 5 and 6 years

  • Heart rate was continuously measured

  • 45 min exercise sessions

  • 3 ten-minute movement games

  • 15 min of soccer

  • Assessed at ages 5 and 6 years of age

  • The determination test for children (DTC)

  • Measures accuracy and reaction speed to rapidly changing visual and acoustic stimuli

  • Electrical brain activity measured before and after exercise (or control condition) and continuously during the cognitive task

  • Reaction time decreased and correct responses increased from pre to post testing however, there was no significant effect by condition

Niederer et al. (2011) Cross-sectional and longitudinal. Switzerland
  • N = 312 children at baseline

  • N = 245 children at follow-up 9 months later

  • Average age at baseline 5.2 years

  • 49.4% female

  • 79% of parents born outside of Switzerland

  • 44% with low parental education

  • Direct observation at baseline and 9 month follow-up

  • 45–50 min assessment of 3 tests

  • Aerobic fitness

  • Agility

  • Dynamic balance

  • Assessed at baseline and 9 month follow-up

  • Intelligence and Development Scales – IDS

  • Spatial working memory

  • Konzentrations-Handlungverfahrn fur Volrschulkinder -KHV-VK

  • Attention

  • Cross-sectional analyses

  • Higher aerobic fitness was related to better attention, but not working memory

  • Greater agility was related to better working memory and attention

  • No outcome associations with dynamic balance.

  • Longitudinal analyses

  • Higher baseline aerobic fitness showed better attention over time

  • Better dynamic balance at baseline showed better spatial working memory over time

  • No other longitudinal associations

Palmer et al. (2013) Cross-sectional within-subjects study cohort. USA
  • N = 16 children

  • 18.7% female

  • Average age 4.1

  • Average income $12,000 above national median

  • No race or ethnicity information

  • Acti-Graph GT3X + accelerometer

  • Picture Deletion Task for Preschoolers (PDTP)

  • Measures response inhibition

  • Preschoolers had significantly improved ability to sustain attention after the physical activity condition compared to the sedentary condition




Motor skill
Davis et al. (2011) Cross-sectional. England
  • N = 248 children

  • Subsample: 4 year olds n = 30, 5 year olds n = 30

  • 50% female

  • No race/ethnicity or SES information

  • Assessed at age 4 or 5

  • Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency -2nd ed (BOT-2) to test motor ability

  • Assessed at age 4 or 5

  • Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children -2nd ed (KABC-II)

  • Measures short-term memory

  • Visual processing

  • Long-term storage and retrieval

  • Fluid reasoning

  • Crystallized ability

  • Positive correlation between overall cognitive score and overall motor score

  • In 4 year olds, stronger correlation for females than males

Draper et al. (2012) Quasi-experimental. Pre/post-test with a control group. South Africa
  • N = 83 children

  • Intervention group, n = 43

  • average age 4.75 years

  • 52.5% female

  • Control group, n = 40

  • average age 4.67 years

  • 32.5% female

  • From disadvantaged and low-SES settings in South Africa

  • No race/ethnicity information

  • No quantitative measurement of PA

  • Intervention

  • Little Champs: once/week 45–60 min physical activity program led by a trained coach

  • Herbst test for fine/gross motor skills administered

  • Herbst early childhood development criteria test

  • Assessed cognitive functions for school readiness in 3 to 6 year old children

  • Intervention group showed significant improvement in cognitive scores

Livesey et al. (2006) Cross-sectional. Australia
  • N = 36 children

  • 58.3% female

  • Average age 6.25 years

  • No race/ethnicity or SES information

  • Assessed at ages 5 and 6 years

  • Movement assessment battery for children (MABC - Henerson and Sugden, 1992)

  • Assessed at 5 and 6 years of age

  • Three Tasks

  • Modified stop-signal task (SST)

  • Response inhibition

  • Modified Day-night Stroop task

  • Response inhibition

  • Rowe behavior rating inventory (RBRI)

  • Lower RBRI scores were associated with better ball skills

  • Stroop task performance was associated with better fine motor skills

  • The relationship between motor performance and the SST performance trended in the positive direction but was not significant

  • Partial correlation showed moderate correlation between go-signal reaction time and fine motor and ball skills

Piek et al. (2008) Longitudinal cohort. Australia
  • N = 33 children

  • Age at testing 4 months–4 years; age at re-testing 6–12 years

  • 48.5% female

  • No race/ethnicity or SES information

  • Ages in Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) for motor skills

  • completed at 4, 6, 8, 12, 16, 18, 20, 24, 30, 36, and 48 months

  • Assessed at school age (between ages 6–12 years)

  • Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV fourth edition)

  • Verbal comprehension (VCI)

  • Working memory (WMI)

  • Processing speed (PSI)

  • Perceptual reasoning (PRI)

  • Gross motor trajectory (controlling for SES) did not predict VCI or PRI but was significant for WMI and PSI

  • The ASQ gross motor trajectory set of predictors accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in cognitive performance once SES was controlled

Rhemtulla and Tucker-Drob (2011) Longitudinal survey. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). USA
  • N = 10,201 children

  • n = 8300 at age 4

  • n = 6800 at age 5

  • n = 1850 at age 6

  • 51.1% female

  • Race/Ethnicity: 41.4% white, 15.9% African-American, 20.5% Hispanic, 11.3% Asian, and 10.8% other

  • Assessed at age 4, 5 and 6

  • Gross motor skills

  • Jumping

  • Balancing

  • Hopping

  • Skipping

  • Walking backwards

  • Catching a bean bag

  • Assessed at age 4, 5, and 6

  • Reading

  • Preschool Language and Assessment Scale – preLAS

  • Preschool Comprehensive Test of Phonological & Print Processing

  • Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

  • Mathematics

  • Measures number sense

  • Oral language

  • Let's tell stories task from preLAS

  • The rates of developmental change in every domain (motor and cognitive) were positively inter-correlated with a common factor, which accounted for an average of 42% of individual differences in change

  • Findings suggest significant relationships between the development of several cognitive and motor skills in childhood

Rosey et al. (2010) Cross-sectional within-subjects' design. USA
  • N = 61 children

  • Age groups 3–5 years old

  • 3-year olds, n = 16 (62.5% female, average age, 3.4 years)

  • 4-year olds, n = 25 (44% female, average age, 4.25 years)

  • 5-year olds, n = 20 (75% female, average age, 5.25 years)

  • No race or ethnicity information

  • Three fundamental movement skills measured

  • Unipedal balance

  • Overarm throw

  • Hopping

  • Tasks analyzed on film

  • Day-Night

  • verbal inhibition

  • Hand-Candle

  • motor inhibition

  • Tap-Once/Tap-Twice

  • motor inhibition

  • Go-No Go task

  • Delayed inhibition

  • Trail Making Test

  • 5-year old children only

  • Inhibition task performances were correlated with coordination level for the three motor skills for the 3–4 year old children only

  • Non-verbal inhibition was a coordination level predictor more than the verbal or delayed inhibition