Abstract
Social connection is a fundamental human need. As such, people’s brains are sensitized to social cues, such as those carried by language, and to promoting social communication. The neural mechanisms of certain key building blocks in this process, such as receptivity to and reproduction of social language, however, are not known. We combined quantitative linguistic analysis and neuroimaging to connect neural activity in brain regions used to simulate the mental states of others with exposure to, and re-transmission of, social language. Our results link findings on successful idea transmission from communication science, sociolinguistics and cognitive neuroscience to prospectively predict the degree of social language that participants utilize when re-transmitting ideas as a function of 1) initial language inputs and 2) neural activity during idea exposure.
Keywords: social language, natural language processing, social sharing, fMRI, mentalizing, word of mouth, re-transmission, neuroimaging
Social interaction and communication are fundamental needs for humans (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and as such, people are highly sensitized to multiple forms of social cues. One key tool facilitating social goals is language, which transmits both specific ideas as well as social cues through the words and patterns of words individuals select (Tomasello, 2000, 2008). Humans are particularly sensitive to words that are associated with instances of social interaction (or ‘social language’), and when exposed to ideas framed with such words people tend to use similar social language in response (e.g. in conversation) (Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002, 2009). In other words, social language begets more social language. This raises a number of questions regarding the mechanisms involved and to what extent they extend to instances of message propagation. The current study examines the mechanisms involved in the processing and re-transmission of social language in the novel context of word of mouth sharing.
Previous studies have leveraged neuroimaging methods such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) as a method to examine multiple processes simultaneously during idea exposure; fMRI can reveal implicit and explicit factors leading to successful communication that may not be apparent from self-report measures or other experimental methods alone (Falk et al., 2013; Hasson, Ghazanfar, Galantucci, Garrod, & Keysers, 2012). For example, Falk and colleagues (2013) characterized mechanisms associated with exposure to novel TV show ideas that are more likely to spread (a ‘buzz effect’) and the characteristics of individuals who are likely to be more successful at spreading an idea (a ‘salesperson effect’). In their investigation, neural activity explained variance in successful idea re-transmission beyond what was explained by participants’ self-reported intentions to retransmit the ideas, highlighting one value of applying neural methods to the examination of communicative processes (Falk et al., 2013).
One core finding was that individual differences in neural activity within the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) were associated with being a better idea salesperson. Although one of many possible explanations, the TPJ is a region strongly associated with considering the mental states of others (Decety & Lamm, 2007; Mars et al., 2012; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, 2010; Scholz, Triantafyllou, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Brown, & Saxe, 2009). Falk and colleagues suggested that individuals who engage in more consideration or simulation of others’ viewpoints during initial idea exposure may be better positioned to later successfully communicate ideas to such others. In line with this explanation, in a secondary analysis of the same dataset described above, Falk, O’Donnell and Lieberman (2012) found that activity within the TPJ during initial exposure to the TV show ideas predicted the later use of positive, evaluative language when participants subsequently described the shows to others. Neither analysis previously conducted by Falk and colleagues, however, directly addressed the type of social cues that might elicit activity within the mentalizing system or how such cues might elicit neural processing that predicts how the message would be re-transmitted.
In an effort to more deeply explore relationships between message features, neural processing and subsequent communication of messages, we leverage data collected from the same participants studied by Falk and colleagues during a new task in which the incoming ideas varied widely in terms of the social cues inherent in the incoming stimuli. The specific, quantifiable, social cue we focus on is the degree to which the language used was ‘social’—as measured using the LIWC dictionary (Pennebaker, Chung, Ireland, Gonzales, & Booth, 2007; Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010). Given the importance of social language for multiple outcomes ranging from depth of conversational engagement to success in cooperative problem solving (Dzindolet & Pierce, 2006; Gonzales, Hancock, & Pennebaker, 2010; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002, 2009), and our prior findings related to the use of neural systems implicated in mentalizing for the successful spread of ideas, we ask: What systems of the brain are activated by receiving social language? Does the resulting neural activity correlate with subsequent use of social language? Understanding how social language is processed by initial message recipients and then re-transmitted is one key component of understanding how ideas are propagated and the types of motivations and processes that reproduce not only content but also broader social consequences of sharing.
Building on the work described above, the present investigation examines whether brain systems that are engaged in considering and simulating the mental states of others are particularly engaged by social features of language (e.g. words associated with social interaction), and whether activity within the brain’s mentalizing system during exposure to ideas predicts the subsequent degree of social language employed in describing the ideas to others. We expect that language that calls to mind instances of social interaction to be associated with this process and will also activate neural regions most commonly associated with considering the mental states of others. This process, termed ‘mentalizing’, most commonly activates the bilateral temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) as well as the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex (PC/PCC) (Denny, Kober, Wager, & Ochsner, 2012; Lieberman, 2010; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, 2010; see Mars et al., 2012 for discussion of functional subdivisions within the TPJ).
Social language includes words that are widely used to describe instances of social interaction and engagement (e.g. you, your, them, friends, family, talk, share, people, call, etc.). Individuals then bring these words to mind when talking about social situations or when engaging others socially (Pennebaker, Chung, Ireland, Gonzales, & Booth, 2007), and as such they become associated with social interaction and social intent. Perhaps because of its social function, this type of language in particular has been shown to synchronize (i.e., speaker and hearer show matched frequencies of word category usage) during certain forms of successful communication (Gonzales et al., 2010; Ireland et al., 2011; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002). We extend these prior investigations to consider the context of idea re-transmission to new interaction partners.
Background
Language and Social Interaction
Social interaction and communication are fundamental needs for humans (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008; Eisenberger, 2012; Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004). Indeed, scholars have noted that “many of our cognitive faculties emerge from interpersonal interactions, and that a complete understanding of the cognitive processes within a single individual’s brain cannot be achieved without understanding the interactions among individuals” (Hasson & Honey, 2012, p. 1272). In addition, coupling between communicators has been observed on multiple levels, from non-verbal cues (Cappella, 1996; Cappella & Palmer, 1989; Clark, 2003; Giles & Smith, 1979; Richardson & Dale, 2005) to linguistic patterns (Branigan, Pickering, & Cleland, 2000; Dale & Spivey, 2006; Giles, Coupland, & Coupland, 1991; Gonzales et al., 2010; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002) to neural activity associated with producing and decoding narratives (Hasson et al., 2012; Stephens, Silbert, & Hasson, 2010).
We suggest that understanding neural mechanisms of social communication may provide a coherent link between observed behaviors and processes studied in other fields. This includes examining the coupling or synchronization on both the verbal and non-verbal levels that takes place in conversational dyads or offering insights about how the process of receptivity to linguistic social cues might tie in with broader understandings of social influence in social psychology, sociology and other fields concerned with influence. In addition, the use of communication paradigms can expand our understanding of the range of processes supported by specific brain systems implicated in social thought (e.g., through what pathways might these brain systems aid in preparing us to effectively signal social intent to others and re-transmit key pieces of cultural knowledge?).
Social Language Facilitates Social Communication
Previous studies have demonstrated how increased synchrony in dyadic communication of both verbal and nonverbal features is associated with more successful communication outcomes (Cappella, 1996; Cappella & Palmer, 1989; Cappella, 1997; Semin, 2007). Recent work has used language quantification, specifically word category counting, to measure the degree of synchrony across a range of language features and found the coordination of levels of ‘social language’ a prominent component (Goode & Robinson, 2013; Ireland et al., 2011; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002). In this context social language is understood and operationalized as words and patterns of words that are commonly used to describe instances of social interaction. Although patterns of linguistic usage are largely unconscious, it is possible that the use of ‘social language’ across communicators is especially important because it may signal affiliative or cooperative intent (Tomasello, 2008; Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, 2005). Indeed, it is well established that language form and function are intimately tied (Bybee, 2010; Croft & Cruse, 2004; Ellis & O’Donnell, 2012), with adult humans effortlessly transmitting and decoding meaning, even when they are not aware of the specific language forms they are utilizing to convey such meaning. In the present investigation, we extend past results to consider whether and how social language might not only synchronize between communication dyads, but might also influence idea re-transmission.
Tools for integrating our understanding of social communication
Given that individuals may not be aware of the mechanisms that lead them to imbue their communications with broader social meaning (e.g., via patterns of language), the tools needed to examine these mechanisms need to be able to access and measure implicit processes. We propose a methodological combination of quantitative linguistics and neuroimaging as one approach to investigating the psychological processes associated with effective communication and the successful transmission of ideas, norms and behaviors (O’Donnell & Falk, Submitted). Both linguistic and neuroimaging tools can be used to indirectly measure information about psychological processes that unfold during different stages of the communication process (Falk, Morelli, Welborn, Dambacher, & Lieberman, 2013; Lieberman, 2010; Pennebaker & King, 1999; Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010).
More specifically, quantitative linguistic analysis can pick up on features of communication such as social orientation that contextualize specific information being delivered (Everett, 2012; Halliday, 1978; Hymes, 1974). Thus, during a communicative interaction language carries not only content information but also social cues. Through instances of associative learning (i.e., fast (System 1) thinking (Evans, 2003; Lieberman, 2003; McLaren et al., 2014; Oaksford & Chater, 2012) and implicit learning (Ellis, 2008; Shanks, 2010)) language users build up an inventory of word-to-social function mappings that they use to interpret the communicative intent of speakers (Hoey, 2005). Further, by this view, meaning in language is a result of the negotiation between language users, interacting in various social contexts, using language for various functions (Halliday, 1977; Halliday et al., 2004), which by extension must be encoded, decoded and planned and executed by the brain.
In parallel, neuroscience investigations have characterized brain systems associated with considering the mental states of others (Denny et al., 2012; Lieberman, 2010; Saxe, 2010; Saxe & Powell, 2006; Scholz, Triantafyllou, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Brown, & Saxe, 2009)—termed mentalizing—and have established that the mentalizing system is engaged in successful speaker/listener coupling (Hasson et al., 2012; Stephens et al., 2010) as well as in the successful spread of ideas from person to person (Falk et al., 2013, 2012). Broadly, tools such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are able to interrogate multiple processes simultaneously, without the need to ask what types of mechanisms people think they are using (Lieberman, 2010). In past studies, this has offered novel insight into the mechanisms underlying a wide range of social psychological processes (Lieberman, 2010) and allowed researchers to predict variance in outcomes not explained by self-reports and other available measures (Berkman & Falk, 2013).
The Present Study
The present investigation builds on and extends prior findings by examining the neurocognitive mechanisms associated with exposure to one key vehicle for social communication—social language, and subsequently how neural activity within the brain’s mentalizing system may prime or prepare communicators to later employ social language. In particular, in this study we combine fMRI analysis of neural activity in the mentalizing system during exposure to descriptions of novel products with linguistic categorical word scores (using LIWC (Pennebaker et al., 2007)) of the language in the descriptions. We then examine whether mentalizing activity during initial idea exposure predicts subsequent uses of social language, beyond that contained in the initial idea descriptions. Our goal is to better understand how stimulus features (language) and individual features (neural response) are implicated in core components of social sharing and effective communication. Previous work has demonstrated how dyad synchrony, including similar rates of categorical word use (Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002), improves communicative effectiveness. However, we anticipate that neural responses to social language in the initial stimuli and their association to social language usage in subsequent description will reveal processes that are engaged beyond encoding and recoding specific wording. Instead, we suggest that social language, in particular, may prime broader social cognition that goes beyond mere reproduction of language features and extends as well to social motivation and simulation of others’ mental states.
Specifically, we hypothesized that: 1. activity in the mentalizing system, especially subregions of the TPJ previously implicated in successful idea propagation, would show greater activation when participants were exposed product descriptions high in social language; 2. activity in the mentalizing system during idea exposure would predict the usage of social language in post-scan product descriptions.
Method
Participants
Twenty undergraduate participants were recruited for a larger fMRI study in exchange for course credit or financial compensation. One participant’s data was not used due to technical difficulties, leaving n=19 (11 female, mean age = 20.55, SD = 6.17). Participants were right-handed, spoke English fluently and met the following criteria related to fMRI safety: 1) were not claustrophobic; 2) had no metal in their bodies (other than tooth fillings); 3) were not pregnant/breast-feeding; 4) were not currently taking psychoactive medication. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects in accordance with the policies of the UCLA Institutional Review Board.1
Stimuli
A set of 24 stimuli were created by asking undergraduate students not involved in the fMRI study to describe novel products they were familiar with, framed as personal reviews. The descriptions were selected for novelty (in 2008), edited for length (mean words 94.21, SD=6.53 words) and consistency in terms of language complexity and reading level, but the original framing was not altered. Thus, the 24 product descriptions all consisted of positive recommendations for the product they described but varied in terms of their use of social language according to the tendency of the original communicator (see Figure 2 and Appendix A).
Procedure
During an fMRI session participants were shown descriptions of the 24 different products recommended by their peers and asked to indicate whether they would in turn recommend each product to a friend using a four point scale (prompt: ‘Would you tell a friend about this product?’, rating: 1=Definitely not, 2=Unlikely, 3=Likely and 4=Definitely; see Figure 1). They completed this task across four separate runs alongside a second task, the TV Show Task (Falk et al. 2013) that consisted of three separate runs. All the runs from each task were completed contiguously within task but their orders within task were randomized and the order in which the tasks were performed was counter-balanced, i.e. whether a participant completed the Product or TV Show task first (see Falk et al., 2013 for more details of TV Show Task).
After the scanning session participants were videotaped describing each of the 24 products, aided by a cue card that contained the name and pictures of each of the products. These video descriptions were then transcribed into standard orthography following rules of written text (e.g. sentences divisions) but keeping hesitations, and repetitions, and marking the length of significant pauses in parentheses.
Linguistic measures
The social processes category from the English 2007 LIWC dictionary (Pennebaker et al., 2007) was used in the analysis. It consists of 455 words and captures both references to individuals who may be engaged in a social interaction (e.g. people, friends, someone, mother, father, they, you, etc.) and words used to describe these interactive processes (e.g. talk, share, write, etc). Words from the Social Processes category accounted for 8.32% of words in the LIWC sample corpora used to determine base word usage rates (Pennebaker et al., 2007). Each of the original product descriptions used as stimuli and each participant’s verbal description of each product were scored using LIWC. LIWC normalizes these scores according to text length. The 24 product descriptions used as experimental stimuli had a mean score of 8.10 (SD=5.36) for the Social Processes category, which accounts for overall word count. These scores are referred to as ‘social language in’. Table 1 shows two descriptions of products, which objectively both involve the potential for social interaction (video taping others; showing others photos), though in the stimulus set one is high (Digital Photo Key Chain) and the other low (DVR Pen) on social category words.
Table 1.
DVR Pen | Digital Photo Key Chain |
---|---|
The DVR pen is awesome and perfect for lectures. It’s a pen that is a digital video recorder capable of capturing video at 30 frames a second. It even has a microphone for capturing sound. It records to flash memory or micro-SD, and has a Bluetooth wireless transfer capability for videos that must go straight to the computer or mobile device. It was also designed to operate in low-light, and has motion detection capability. It even has five different motion detection activities. | I found this one cool key chain where you can actually store up to 60 pictures in it. I thought it was interesting because you can input pictures of your friends and family or whatever and carry it with you at all times. So when you’re bored, you can just take it out for a trip down memory lane. Or you could share it with people you know or meet or show off your dog or something like that. And if someone doesn’t remember who you are talking about, you can show them a picture of that person too, no problem. |
LIWC Social processes: 0 | LIWC Social processes: 21.0 |
It should be clear from these extracts, which represent the extremes of the LIWC Social Processes category scores in our initial stimuli, how the words in this category bring to mind instances of social interaction and frame the information about the product within that context. It would be possible, for instance, to talk about the DVR Pen in a way that would incorporate many words associated with social interaction and about the Digital Photo Key Chain with no or very few such words (see Table 2). The variation found across the 24 products (see Appendix A) is a result of framing choices made by the 24 individuals who generated the initial product descriptions. We capitalize on this variation here to examine how such natural variation is encoded by subsequent message recipients. In addition, for discriminant validity, we examined scores on two other LIWC categories: 1. Cognitive processes (e.g., cause, know, ought) and 2. Affective processes (e.g., happy, cried, abandon) (Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010).
Table 2.
Digital Photo Key Chain | Digital Photo Key Chain |
---|---|
Another product was the digital photo keychain. And you could store up to sixty pictures of friends and family. Whatever you like to look at and carry around with you all the time. Um, if you’re having a conversation with a friend and they didn’t remember about someone you were talking about you could just pull out the keychain and show them. Or if you’re bored you could reminisce through pictures of your life. | Uh the digital photo key chain, that was also something that’s- is very cool just to- to be able to have tons of digital er sixty or something digital images to carry around and look at, um whenever just your free-time. It’s kinda a cool little thing to have on your key chain it’s really different. |
LIWC Social processes: 22.7 | LIWC Social processes: 3.45 |
DVR Pen | DVR Pen |
---|---|
The DVR pen is a pen that not only fulfills the task of writing but it um, it contains video and voice recorder so if you use it in lecture so when you use it in a lecture you can not only write your notes but can also record your professor and so that way you can um, review what your professor said for an upcoming exam so I thought it was really an essential product, um, because it enhances the life of a college student and helps them um, economically. | Um the DVR pen was kinda interesting it was- it’s a little pen that jus- that has the ability to capture video and audio and um, it just seems it seems like something very high tech and very um, I don- I don’t understand how they would be able to make a product like that now but it seems like something very interesting |
LIWC Social processes: 14.4 | LIWC Social processes: 1.56 |
After our fMRI participants were exposed to each of these ideas, they also recorded their own recommendations of the products. The post-scan product descriptions were transcribed from the video segments produced by the participants and scores were computed for the LIWC Social processes category. We refer to these scores as ‘social language out’. Table 2 provides an example of the language used by two different participants when describing the Digital Photo Key Chain that vary between high (22.7) and low (3.45) on LIWC social process scores. This demonstrates individual differences in the use of these linguistic features given that both participants are responding to same stimulus (the initial Digital Photo Key Chain description has a LIWC Social Processes score of 21.0).
fMRI Acquisition and Analysis
fMRI Data Acquisition
Imaging data were acquired using a Trio 3 Tesla head-only MRI scanner at the UCLA Ahmanson-Lovelace Brainmapping Center. Head motion was minimized using foam padding and surgical tape; goggles were also fixed in place using surgical tape connecting to the head coil and scanner bed. A set of high-resolution structural T2-weighted echo-planar images were acquired coplanar with the functional scans (spin-echo; TR = 5000 ms; TE = 34ms; matrix size = 128 × 128; 33 interleaved slices; FOV = 220mm; slice thickness = 4 mm; voxel size = 1.7 × 1.7 × 4.0mm; flip angle = 90°). A high resolution T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) scan was also acquired in the coronal plane (TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.47 ms; matrix size = 64 × 64; FOV = 256 mm; slice thickness = 1.0 mm; 160 slices; voxel size = 1.3 ×. 1.3 × 1.0 mm; flip angle = 8°). Four functional runs were recorded for each participant (echo-planar T2-weighted gradient-echo, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 75°, matrix size = 64 × 64, 33 axial slices, FOV = 220 mm, 4mm thick; voxel size = 3.4 × 3.4 × 4.0 mm). Each run consisted of 6 blocks (one product was described and rated in each block). The first two volumes from each run were discarded to allow the scanner to equilibrate. The data were analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK).
fMRI Preprocessing
Preprocessing steps were done using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK) apart from the despiking of the functional images that was carried out using the default options of AFNI 3dDespike (Cox, 1996). Using SPM8, despiked functional images were corrected for slice acquisition timing differences within volumes (slice order interleaved), realigned within and between runs to correct for residual head motion. These were then coregistered using a two stage process in which the mean functional volume was coregistered with the matched-bandwidth structural scan, and the matched-bandwidth structural scan was coregistered with the MPRAGE, using 6-parameter rigid body transformations. To ensure accurate skull stripping the coregistered MP-RAGE images where segmented and were then normalized into Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard stereotactic space (using the MNI152_T1_1mm template). The resulting parameters were applied to all segmented, coregistered, functional images. Finally the functional images were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel (8 mm full width at half maximum).
fMRI data analysis
We constructed individual models for each subject in which the description periods for each product were treated as separate regressors in the design matrix (i.e., an item-based model) with a single boxcar regressor for each 3-second rating period using Statistical Parametric Modeling (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK). Response periods were all modeled using one regressor of no interest. Fixation periods served as an implicit baseline. Corresponding random effects models were constructed at the group level, averaging across subject first-level models for each product.
ROI data were extracted for each product at the group level, representing the mean activation across all voxels in the ROI during exposure to the product minus mean activation during the implicit baseline (this is the item-based contrast) divided by mean activity during the baseline/rest period (to give a percent signal change). We constructed two separate sets of regions of interest (ROIs). The first focused on specific functionally-defined regions of the mentalizing system within the bilateral TPJ, which have been previously associated with being a good “idea salesperson”. This effect was established according to individual differences in people’s ability to successfully convince others of the value of the idea salesperson’s preferred ideas (Falk et al., 2013). Brain masks for functional ROIs were identified using xjview, and MarsBar (Brett, Anton, Valabregue, & Poline, 2002) was used to convert these image masks to ROIs (Figure 4). The second set of ROIs focused more broadly on a wider range of brain regions most commonly implicated in mentalizing (Lieberman, 2010; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, 2010; Saxe & Powell, 2006), including the bilateral temporal parietal junction (TPJ), the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (Figure 3). Anatomical ROIs were constructed in Wake Forest University Pickatlas toolbox within SPM (Maldjian, Laurienti, Burdette, & Kraft, 2003), combining gross definitions from the Automated Anatomical Labeling Atlas (AAL; (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) Brodmann areas. MarsBar (Brett et al., 2002) was used to convert these anatomical images to ROIs.
Combining fMRI and linguistic data
We combined neuroimaging data with computational linguistic quantification of 1. the linguistic input (i.e., the product descriptions) and 2. the post-scan language output, i.e. fMRI subjects’ descriptions of the products, both using the standard word counting approach described above, namely LIWC (Pennebaker et al., 2007; Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010).
Statistical analysis combining our a priori hypothesized ROI data with quantitative linguistic output was carried out in R (R Core Team, 2013) using the lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2013) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2014) packages to perform linear mixed effects modeling.
We first ran a regression specifying fixed effects of LIWC Social Processes score for product stimuli predicting neural activity extracted as percent signal change from each of our hypothesized mentalizing network ROIs. Participants were treated as random effects with slopes and intercepts allowed to vary randomly, and accounting for non-independence in the data from these two sources.
Next, we ran a regression specifying fixed effects of neural activity extracted as percent signal change from each of our mentalizing network ROIs, predicting LIWC Social Processes scores for each participant’s post-scan descriptions of each product. Participants were treated as random effects with slopes and intercepts allowed to vary randomly.
Whole brain search
In addition to these ROI analyses, we also examined whole brain parameter maps to uncover any regions outside of the mentalizing network that might be associated with social language in and social language out. The LIWC Social Processes scores from the product descriptions (social language in) and from participants’ post-scan descriptions for each product (social language out) were used as parametric modulators of neural activity in two separate analyses of neural activity during exposure to the 24 product descriptions. To do so, for each participant we modeled neural activity associated with exposure to social language (‘social language in’) within a first-level fixed effects model using SPM8. More specifically, we modeled LWIC scores for each of the initial product stimulus descriptions within the LIWC Social Processes category as a parametric modulator of neural activity during exposure to the corresponding initial product ideas. This analysis identified voxels throughout the brain whose activation levels covaried with exposure to product descriptions that initially contained more social language within each subject. These subject-level models were combined in a random effects model to produce a whole brain activation map of neural regions associated with exposure to more social language across participants.
Second, using a parallel procedure, for each participant we modeled neural activity associated with subsequent social language use (social language out) as a parametric modulator of neural activity during exposure to the initial product ideas within a fixed effects model in SPM8. These subject-level models were combined using a random effects model to produce whole brain activation map of neural regions associated with subsequently using more social language to describe products across participants. Results were thresholded at p = .005, K = 36, corresponding to p < .05, corrected, based on a Monte Carlo Simulation implemented using AlphaSim in the software package AFNI (Ward, 2000).
Finally, we examined whether social language in the product descriptions was associated with greater subsequent use of social language to describe products, and whether neural activity within the mentalizing system remained predictive of this subsequent social language controlling for social language scores in the initial product descriptions.
Results
Behavioral Data & Association Between Social Language In and Social Language Out
The LIWC Social Processes scores for product descriptions in the stimuli (social language in) are shown in Appendix A next to each description (with words from the category underlined) (M=8.08 SD=5.37). Summary scores from participants post-scan descriptions are also shown for each product (M=8.44 SD=4.67). The stimuli LIWC scores (social language in) predicted the LIWC scores for Social Processes in the transcribed language produced by participants describing what they had seen in the scanner (social language out) (t=7.46, p<.001, n_subjects=19, n_descriptions = 24). That is, the degree of social language in was systematically, positively related to the degree of social language out (the language used by participants when they subsequently describe the stimuli to others), accounting for the nested structure of descriptions within products and participants.
Neural activity associated with social language
Neural correlates of social language in
We first examined neural activity during our scanned participants’ exposure to the initial product descriptions as a function of social language in within a priori defined regions of interest. Neural activity within the functionally defined bilateral TPJ ROI previously associated with the successful transmission of ideas to others (the Salesperson Effect in (Falk et al., 2013); Figure 4) was strongly associated with exposure to social language in (t=3.96, p<.001).
In order to examine whether a broader range of ROIs within the mentalizing system would be associated with social language in, we examined activity within anatomically defined right and left TPJ, DMPFC and PCC (Figure 3; Table 3). This ROI analysis suggested that the association between social language in and neural activity in the mentalizing system is most strongly focused in the bilateral TPJ and is particularly strongly associated with the left TPJ. This association was only significant for the Social Processes LIWC category and not for the two other LIWC categories, Cognitive Processes and Affective Processes, we examined.
Table 3.
ROI | t | p |
---|---|---|
DMPFC + lTPJ + rTPJ + PCC | 1.352 | 0.181 |
DMPFC | −0.006 | 0.995 |
PCC | 1.288 | 0.198 |
rTPJ | 1.691 | 0.100 |
π lTPJ | 3.910 | 0.000*** |
Functional TPJ (Falk et al 2013) | 3.961 | 0.000*** |
Functional rTPJ | 2.615 | 0.014* |
Functional lTPJ | 4.84 | 0.000*** |
Finally, we followed up this targeted ROI analysis, with a whole brain analysis to ascertain if other regions outside of the mentalizing network, or more targeted sub-regions within our ROIs, might also be involved in individuals’ response to language containing cues to situations of social interaction (Figure 5). This analysis confirmed that activity within bilateral TPJ was robustly associated with exposure to higher levels of social language. In addition, activity within subregions of the mentalizing more broadly, including a sub-portion of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and temporal pole, but little else in the brain, were associated with initial exposure to more social language (see Table 4).
Table 4.
Region | Local Max | K | t-stat | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
TPJ (right) | 53 | −60 | 28 | 213 | 7.08 |
TPJ (left) | −44 | −67 | 34 | 508 | 6.61 |
TPJ (left) | −50 | −57 | 28 | 5.39 | |
TPJ/IPL (left) | −54 | −50 | 46 | 5.36 | |
DLPFC | −33 | 29 | 55 | 87 | 4.32 |
MPFC | −9 | 63 | 16 | 181 | 4.07 |
DMPFC | −13 | 60 | 25 | 3.89 | |
DMPFC/Superior frontal gyrus | −16 | 39 | 40 | 3.87 | |
Temporal Pole (left) | −47 | 1 | −29 | 66 | 3.87 |
Posterior cingulate | 1 | −50 | 28 | 52 | 3.43 |
Precuneus (right) | 11 | −50 | 22 | 3.1 |
Neural correlates of social language out
We next examined whether neural activity within the mentalizing system as participants were exposed to the initial product descriptions predicted the degree of social language used post-scan. More specifically, we fitted a linear mixed effect model using parameter estimates for percent signal change extracted from the same ROIs described above (see Figures 3 and 4) to predict LIWC Social Processes scores in language output.
We found a significant relationship between levels of activity both within our functionally defined and anatomically defined mentalizing ROIs during exposure to product ideas and the subsequent level of social language usage when participants were asked to describe the product to others (Table 5). Consistent with the data reported above in which TPJ activity was particularly strongly associated with social language in, we found that when controlling for social language in, the effects of TPJ on social language out were no longer significant, whereas effects in the mentalizing system overall (t=2.02, p=0.053) and specifically in the DMPFC (t=2.01, p=0.046) remained significantly associated with social language out. This association was only significant for the Social Processes LIWC category and not for the two other LIWC categories we examined: Cognitive Processes (p=0.296) and Affective Processes (p=0.789).
Table 5.
Model A | Model B | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
ROI | t | p | t | p |
DMPFC + lTPJ + rTPJ + PCC | 2.410 | 0.019* | 2.072 | 0.053 |
DMPFC | 1.716 | 0.088 | 2.013 | 0.046* |
PCC | 1.691 | 0.094 | 1.470 | 0.143 |
rTPJ | 2.165 | 0.035* | 1.360 | 0.190 |
lTPJ | 3.151 | 0.008** | 1.151 | 0.150 |
Functional TPJ (Falk et al 2013) | 3.108 | 0.007** | 1.573 | 0.133 |
Functional rTPJ | 2.378 | 0.021* | 1.358 | 0.192 |
Functional lTPJ | 3.589 | 0.029** | 1.579 | 0.134 |
To examine whether neural regions outside of our hypothesized mentalizing network were also associated with social language out, we ran a whole brain search. Figure 6 shows the neural regions associated with higher LIWC Social Processes scores. In this analysis, the only brain regions significantly associated with social language used in post-scan descriptions are within the mentalizing system, including LTPJ and DMPFC (Table 6). Taken together, these results demonstrate that higher levels of activity in regions associated with processes of mentalizing during idea exposure are positively associated with greater usage of words from the LIWC Social Processes category.
Table 6.
Region | Local Max | K | t-stat | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
DMPFC (left) | −13 | 56 | 19 | 61 | 4.42 |
−2 | 50 | 19 | 3.18 | ||
TPJ (left) | −57 | −64 | 40 | 69 | 3.96 |
BA40 | −61 | −60 | 31 | 3.76 |
Discussion
The present study demonstrates that social cues in language can activate neural systems implicated both in understanding the mental states of others and successfully re-transmitting ideas. Furthermore, neural activity within such brain regions is also predictive of subsequent uses of social language when re-transmitting the ideas, beyond what is predicted from features of the initial language stimulus. These effects were specific to the ‘social processes’ category of language, suggesting that the neural effects observed go beyond mere semantic priming, such that social language in particular may call to mind instances of social interaction and prime further consideration of the mental states of others. Our findings add to a growing body of literature examining not only the mechanisms of successful communication in dyads (Hasson et al., 2012; Stephens et al., 2010) but also how simulation of others’ mental states may facilitate effective idea re-transmission (Falk et al., 2013, 2012).
More specifically, we have demonstrated: 1. Individuals vary in the way they choose to use social language when asked to provide positive personal recommendations of products. 2. To the extent that these cues can be measured using a quantitative linguistic tool, such as LIWC, there is an association between the use of social language and levels of neural activity in regions connected to processes of social cognition or mentalizing that is specific to the social processes category (i.e., not an exclusive effect of semantic priming or parroting). 3. Neural activity in these same regions during idea exposure prospectively predicts the use of social language in subsequent description of the idea, and 4. does so above and beyond the linguistic features of the initial seed idea.
Consistent with past work examining verbal synchrony or linguistic style matching (Ireland et al., 2011; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002), in this study we observed a significant relationship between use of social language by one communicator (social language in) and that by the initial listener who subsequently becomes a re-transmitter (social language out). Our results go beyond the effects of mere matching, however, and illustrate one strength of adding a neuroimaging perspective. We found that cues represented through the use of social language patterns (but not other types of language patters) are associated with neural activity in hypothesized regions of TPJ as indexed by a region of interest analysis, as well as a whole brain search. TPJ is a brain region commonly associated with mentalizing (Denny et al., 2012; Lieberman, 2010; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, 2010; Saxe & Powell, 2006) and has also been associated with individual differences in successfully spreading one’s preferred ideas (Falk et al., 2013, 2012). To strengthen our confidence in our theorized link from social language in to mentalizing processes, we also compared the region of TPJ associated with ‘social language in’ in our study to prior studies of mentalizing using Neurosynth (Yarkoni, Poldrack, Nichols, Van Essen, & Wager, 2011). This analysis suggested that the probability of mentalizing given the coordinate activations observed is high.
Furthermore, activity within the TPJ during exposure to the initial product ideas predicted the degree of social language used when participants later described the products, above and beyond the language contained in the initial seed descriptions. In other words, although neural activity within the TPJ was strongly associated with both exposure to social language in and production of social language out, when controlling for social language in, the effects of TPJ were diminished, however overall effects within the hypothesized mentalizing network ROI (bilateral TPJ, posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus), and within DMPFC in particular, were strongly positively predictive of social language out, controlling for social language in. Effects within DMPFC and left TPJ were also reflected in a whole brain search for regions associated with subsequent use of social language during idea re-transmission.
We interpret these findings in the context of our hypothesis that social language, in particular, may prime broader social cognition that goes beyond mere reproduction of language features and extends as well to social motivation and simulation of others’ mental states. In past work, DMPFC has been implicated in considering others’ attributes and motivations (Lieberman, 2010; Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2006; Spunt, Falk, & Lieberman, 2010). Lieberman (2010) argues that DMPFC, perhaps more than other subregions of the mentalizing system, may be associated with motivational states and it is possible that the continued prediction of social language out by DMPFC after controlling for initial social language in reflects that social language is not merely reproduced, but also serves specific motivational ends for the speaker (e.g., to bond or look good by communicating good ideas in a compelling way to others).
More generally, our analysis ntrolling for social language in, suggests that the brain goes beyond merely processing and reproducing low-level features of social language, i.e. lexical and semantic processing. Individuals respond to social language cues in a novel idea description by engaging systems associated with social cognition, which are then subsequently associated with social language out. Social framing of the idea, using words that have a shared association with instances of social interaction, appears to enhance processes of mentalizing as individuals are exposed to the idea. In turn, increased mentalizing while being exposed to and encoding a novel idea makes it more likely that in subsequently describing the idea to others, individuals will use social words to frame their description.
These data are consistent with the idea that in taking in and processing social cues speakers may automatically process social intent, but that additional types of social cognition may be involved in successful preparation for communicating social intent to others. The present investigation builds on prior work suggesting that neural activity within the TPJ and mentalizing system more broadly are associated with the successful spread of ideas (Falk et al., 2013). Future research that brings together the results observed here with metrics of successful propagation to future idea recipients will be of interest in confirming whether the activity within hypothesized mentalizing regions observed here is directly linked to being later prepared to understand the motivations of others and re-transmit ideas accordingly.
Understanding the mechanisms underlying how social language is retransmitted may also contribute to a better understanding of the role of social language patterns in promoting social bonds and cooperation and successful communication more broadly. This could occur either directly through the reproduction of social language patterns, or through broader engagement of social cognition that facilitates a range of future behaviors.
At the level of language processing and reproduction, recent studies have found neural activity in the TPJ related to higher linguistic levels and functions (e.g. pragmatic aspects of communication and speech acts) (Egorova, Pulvermuller, & Shtyrov, 2014; Egorova, Shtyrov, & Pulvermuller, 2013; Sassa et al., 2007). Exposure to linguistic cues that prime social connection (e.g., the social processes category of LIWC) may make understanding linguistic input and generation of linguistic output to connect with others all the more salient. Future work is needed to isolate and clarify the roles of specific linguistic features and task and contextual factors (i.e., idea sharing) in relation to the activity in the neural regions reported here. Such work could also link specific language inputs and outputs to real-world relevant behaviors known to covary with higher levels of theory of mind and with higher levels of activity within the brain’s mentalizing system (Dietvorst et al., 2009). In addition, there is some overlap with the mentalizing regions focused on in the current investigation, particularly in the left hemisphere, and with regions associated with a range of lower level linguistic phenomena (i.e., phonological, lexical, syntactic and semantic) (Friederici, 2011; Vigneau et al., 2006). Although we have focused on higher level social motivations, it is likely that the physical proximity between regions involved in mentalizing and language processing could facilitate the types of effects observed here, and future work should engage such possibilities (Arbib, 2012).
At the level of broader motivations, we have conceptualized the LIWC category social processes not simply as a list of independent words which at the semantic level contain social elements but as markers that represent a part of an associative network of commonly co-occurring words in instances of language used describing social interaction. That is, our experience of communicative events describing instances of social interaction is partially organized and encoded around the frequent words and language patterns particularly associated with these discourses. This is the basis for our hypotheses concerning the mentalizing system and the idea that social language may not only beget more social language, but also broader social cognition. Although this suggestion is speculative beyond the scope of our current data, it is consistent with the cited linguistic models, data collected through collocational analysis of large-scale corpora, and links to work on associative learning and involved neural systems (e.g. Bar, 2007; Bar, Aminoff, Mason, & Fenske, 2007).
Links between social language and social cognition may be especially important in the context of preparing to re-transmit ideas to others. The regions of TPJ that we observed to be associated with social language in, and predictive of social language out, have been previously implicated in successful message propagation (Falk et al. 2013). We suggest that mentalizing may be key in initially evaluating ideas and in preparing for future successful social interactions (Dietvorst et al., 2009). Future research that combines past findings regarding synchronization of socially oriented words and linguistic features in the communication literature (Cappella, 1996; Giles & Smith, 1979; Giles et al., 1991; Goode & Robinson, 2013; Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002; Semin, 2007) with neuroscientific investigations of speaker-listener synchronization (Hasson et al., 2012; Stephens et al., 2010) will provide more robust evidence for a conceptual model that links language, neural activity, behavior and relational outcomes. Future investigations that further compare such synchrony in the context of dynamic dyads with retransmission to specific and general others will also inform our understanding on the extent to which the mechanisms observed underpin successful communication in general, or are used selectively according to social goals.
Each of the discussion points above should be interpreted within the bounds of specific limits, however. Most notably with respect to the language variables, the variation in the amount of social language in the product descriptions was naturally occurring in different undergraduates—although this language captures a high degree of external validity, we did not experimentally manipulate the language features. A future study could manipulate this variation and create different versions of the product descriptions, which are high and low in the frequency of these words. This would control for the possibility that some types of products could be, by design and function, more social than others. Given the large number of products, and the variation in subsequent language used to describe products that were initially high and low in social language in, however, we believe the results will remain robust. In addition, although it has substantial advantages for parsimony, the use of a dictionary based approached to linguistic quantification of social and psychological categories has some limitations in terms of domain coverage and the simplicity of the model of language usage (O’Donnell & Falk, Submitted). Future studies might also make use of other types of linguistic quantification (such as supervised machine learning classification) in combination with neuroimaging. With respect to the neuroimaging component of our work, as is characteristic of fMRI studies we report findings based on a relatively small number (n=19) of subjects. Likewise, as with most neuroimaging work, the psychological functions ascribed to the neural regions of interest represent only one of several possible interpretations— the usual caution with respect to reverse inference that has been leveled at brain mapping studies applies equally to this work (Poldrack, 2006).
Despite these considerations, our findings make a novel methodological contribution, illustrating the combination of neural and linguistic tools to understand psychological responses to persuasive communication, message processing, and the spread of ideas. They highlight the importance of considering the role of social cues encoded in language patterns and the role of neurocognitive mechanisms associated with social processes for the discussion of successful communication.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge Karl Dambacher, Sylvia Morelli, Locke Welborn, Sarah Paje, Chu Kim, Amber Haney, Danielle Whalen, Aliss Markosian, Heather Mak, Tomoyo Kuriyama, and Julia Tian for assistance with data collection, and Kirsten DiGiacomi and Brandon Fix for assistance with data coding. We thank Josh Carp for expert assistance and discussions regarding data analysis and Joe Cappella, Jason Coronel and Bob Hornik for helpful feedback on ideas contained in this manuscript. We are grateful to the editor of this special issue and the reviewer of this paper for their detailed feedback and suggestions for improvements. This work was generously supported by the UCLA Department of Psychology; analysis was partially supported by NIH-1 DP2 DA035156-01 (PI: Falk).
Biographies
Matthew Brook O’Donnell, Ph.D., is a research assistant professor within the Annenberg School for Communication at the University of Pennsylvania. His research combines tools and insights from computational linguistics with neuroimaging and experimental and large-scale descriptive methods, to examine how ideas and influence spread through online and offline social networks.
Emily Falk, Ph.D., is an assistant professor within the Annenberg School for Communication at the University of Pennsylvania. Her research integrates cognitive neuroscience, psychology, and communication science to understand media effects and social influence at the individual, group, and population levels. Falk’s lab examines the spread of messages, social norms, values, and culture through social networks.
Matthew D. Lieberman, Ph.D., is a professor of psychology, psychiatry, and biobehavioral sciences at the University of California at Los Angeles. His research focuses on social cognitive neuroscience and uses neuroimaging (fMRI) to examine how we make sense of others, ourselves, and the relation between the two. His work also focuses on persuasion and the spread of ideas, empathy, emotion regulation, and self-knowledge.
Appendix A - LIWC Social Processes category scores for product stimuli (in category words underlined)
Product | Description | LIWC Social score | Mean Social score in post-scan descriptions |
---|---|---|---|
Staircase Storage | I have a really small apartment, even though it’s two floors. Storage under staircases has been around for years, but recently I read online about this new system that takes it one step further. It’s a simple yet obvious idea. It’s under-the-staircase drawer solution that uses each step as a storage drawer. The space underneath each stair is used as a drawer which is a great way to utilize a generally overlooked space. Step up and open the drawer which can be used for many and varied purposes with the only problem being remembering which drawer held what. | 0 | 5.60 (SD=3.20) |
DVR Pen | The DVR pen is awesome and perfect for lectures. It’s a pen that is a digital video recorder capable of capturing video at 30 frames a second. It even has a microphone for capturing sound. It records to flash memory or micro-SD, and has a Bluetooth wireless transfer capability for videos that must go straight to the computer or mobile device. It was also designed to operate in low-light, and has motion detection capability. It even has five different motion detection activities. | 0 | 5.48 (SD=4.09) |
Periodic Element Rings | I’m sort of a geek, but I like to think of myself as a fashionable geek. I recently got one of the Periodic Rings, which are literal rings of chemical elements from the periodic table. I got the “Ag” one, which is silver. It looks exactly like the element box from the periodic table. These rings are also made of exactly what they say – so the platinum version is really expensive, but the silver and gold ones are a little more reasonable. I’m hoping someday that I can collect all three to become some sort of science teacher superhero. | 2 | 4.24 (SD=3.46) |
Doggy Treadmill | Sometimes I have so much to do that I don’t have time to walk my dog. But I found out about this new doggy treadmill that’s like a regular treadmill for adults except it has special features designed just for dogs. The treadmill is enclosed by a coated metal gate so that your pet will be safe while working out on the treadmill. It also comes with a safety leash that stops the treadmill when pulled. Now my dog can work out at home to keep in shape and I can finish my own work. | 2.11 | 8.59 (SD=4.96) |
Flatwire | I don’t like my room being cluttered with wires, but so far, I haven’t found anything like a wireless power source. I recently found the next best thing though, flat wires. Since its first patent in 1995, FlatWire Ready has been winning awards and converts a unique approach to electrical wiring. The technology seems, pretty easy, even for someone like me to set up. I think their motto goes something like: Map it, Stick It, Click It, and Make it Disappear. At least I don’t have to worry about tripping over any wires anymore. | 2.15 | 6.58 (SD=4.92) |
STRIDA Bike | I want the STRIDA bike for going around campus and keeping in my apartment. This bike is ideal for short daily commutes because all the useful and practical features it has. The seating position and the mounted handlebars provide the rider with a good vantage point in traffic. The disc brakes provide a quick and smooth stop, not to mention that the bike can be folded in a matter of seconds. The STRIDA is completely collapsible and can be stored or transported in the small storage bag that comes with it. | 3.3 | 4.32 (SD=3.87) |
Beard Cap | I’m from the East Coast, so I’m always looking for stuff to keep warm when I go visit my parents, or just when I go skiing on vacation or whatever. If you’re somewhere cold, or if you just have a thing for funny-looking headgear, check out the Beard Cap. It’s made from Icelandic wool, and it’s handmade, which I like. It’s a woven cap that covers the entire head except for the eyes and nose, and features an awesome beard-like shape, complete with a woven faux mustache. I think it’s unique looking and it makes me laugh. | 4.08 | 6.17 (SD=2.92) |
Nightlight Airbed | The Nightlight Airbed by Coleman is great if you’re having a guest over or for people who love to camp like me. The mattress is really comfortable. The 8-inch thick air coil design provides full body support and allows for a good night’s rest. The Nightlight Airbed is also covered with soft suede to ensure maximum comfort. This mattress even has a built in flashlight that lasts up to 8 hours and is powered by only three AAA batteries. This product was made specifically for the outdoors so it is very durable, leak proof and puncture-resistant. | 5.15 | 6.16 (SD=3.90) |
Wall Decals | You can’t really paint the apartment or dorm walls but Wall Decals are an instant way to glam up your room without damaging the paint or walls and getting charged for it. There are many cool styles and varieties like shirt designs or even chandeliers. They are re-moveable and re-useable, so you can change things around when you get bored or feel like redecorating. It’s simple and takes only minutes. Wall decals make the walls really unique while avoiding the messiness of wall-painting. Its great for dorms, apartments, studios, houses, or the office. | 5.21 | 7.71 (SD=4.70) |
Powerstick | So I was thinking Wouldn’t it be great to have a portable power source for my gadgets that didn’t take up much space and was easy to charge?” So I looked around some and found “The Powerstick”, it’s a thumb-sized accessory that charges via the built-in USB connector and it can provide extra juice for lots of different devices, including my cell phone and iPod, and others that charge via mini-USB. I also like it because it has a simple fuel gauge graphic that lets you know how the charging is going and how much power you have left. It’s genius. | 5.77 | 4.10 (SD=4.16) |
Belkin Mini Surge Protector | I kind of can’t believe that no one has really made something like this before, but the Belkin Mini Surge Protector with USB Charger is really a pretty neat product that fullfills a need that I hadn’t been able to fill before. It’s basically a little power strip that expands one power outlet into three, but the surge bar also offers two USB ports for charging up your gadgets. So you can recharge everything at once, including your laptop, phone, ipod; whatever. It’s pretty sleek looking and if you’re a frequent traveler, this can be pretty useful. | 7.22 | 5.93 (SD=4.46) |
Green Cell Battery | The Green Cell Battery is the perfect solution to the waste caused by discarded batteries. It’s a single battery that is designed to fit all types of personal electronic devices. No more searching for chargers or buying different size batteries for each device. The Green Cell Battery is made without harmful chemicals and it’s eco-friendly. The batteries can be replaced or recharged at your local vending machine. So it’s so convenient. This battery is the solution that we all have been waiting for, so go out and help make our world a better place. | 8.42 | 7.26 (SD=3.91) |
Nokia Morph Phone | I found out about the concept phone Nokia was working on called Morph online. It’s really advanced and uses exciting new nanotechnology. It comes in several cool, bright transparent colors and it can morph shape. You can stretch it out to get a full keyboard and touch pad or fold it up and wear it around your wrist. It also features self-cleaning surfaces and saves energy as well by harvesting energy from the local environment. I can’t wait for these phones to come out! They said this technology would be available soon. | 8.6 | 7.10 (SD=5.04) |
Flowbee | Flowbee is awesome, you can stay at home and cut your hair exactly the way you want. It’s simple, easy, and precise. Flowbee uses a vacuum to suction hair up and uses a spacer to cut the desired length. The spacer makes it impossible to cut the hair shorter than the length it is set for. So you can’t mess up or get a bad hair cut. They even have a specially designed spacer for a tapered cut. So now everyone in the family can cut their hair at home, even the family pet. | 9.57 | 6.44 (SD=3.90) |
FPS Vest | I am really into video games. But I’m over holding those awkward controllers. So I was real excited to find out about the FPS Vests. They’re strap-on vests that stimulate all the action from your game. Being hit by bullets, kicks and punches, explosions, and crashing into stuff. They’re making more and more games that are compatible with the vests too. It really takes your gaming to a whole another level. | 9.72 | 9.90 (SD=4.85) |
LCD Keyboard | There’s a new keyboard in development that doesn’t have any keys to it; it just consists of several large LCD screens. It’s basically a rectangular LCD screen that’s touch sensitive and can display any image you want to interact with on it. You can design your own keyboard layout or have different keys on each button specific to your need or wants. You can customize it any way you want to fit your personality or just to change it up when you are bored. It’s capabilities are extremely different and new. | 9.89 | 6.53 (SD=4.24) |
Nike Pedometer | I like how treadmills keep tracks of how long you’ve run and the calories you’ve burned, but I’d rather run outside. I’ve tried some pedometers but they were too bulky and big. However, Nike has come out with a small, discrete chip that you can stick on your shoes to transmit the distance you’ve ran, calories you’ve burned, and the time to your iPod. It’s great because it doesn’t impede your running at all. Now I can run outside and get my numbers without the extra weight. | 10.34 | 11.20 (SD=4.43) |
Clocky | There’s an alarm clock that actually gets me out of bed called Clocky. It’s not like regular alarm clocks that go on your night stand, you can keep hitting the snooze button, keep on sleeping, and miss classes, flights, and appointments. After the first snooze, Clocky goes off and randomly rolls around, forcing you to get up to stop the noise. It’s a good way to get you out of bed, it gets your blood pumping, chasing it around, it also helps you wake up and start your day. It’s perfect for people who have trouble waking up. | 11.22 | 6.33 (SD=2.92) |
Tap Project | I recently heard about a cool new program called the “Tap Project.” Basically, while we’re over here buying $2 bottles of water, more than a billion people all over the world have little or no access to clean water. The Tap Project lets us change that a bit, just by going out to dinner with friends. At participating restaurants, you can donate $1 for each glass of water served. The donations will be used to provide safe drinking water to poor areas. In fact, one dollar provides a child in need with 40 days of clean water, which is pretty cool. | 11.22 | 6.20 (SD=3.77) |
Chatterbowl | I feel bad when I go to work and leave my pet home alone. But I found these new “talking” pet bowls called Chatterbowls that are the perfect gadgets for my pets who get lonely and anxious when left alone. These bowls are very simple to operate. The voice box is located underneath the bowl and can hold up to 10 messages from the pet’s owner. When the pet approaches the bowl the voice box plays back the owner’s message. Now you can leave your pet home alone having the peace of mind that they have company. | 11.46 | 13.47 (SD=6.43) |
ATP Photofinder | The ATP Photo Finder is really cool. Basically, it’s like having a geo-tagging feature for your digital camera. The ATP Photo Finder adds the necessary tags to your photos when you take them. Pretty much, you turn the Photo Finder on while you’re shooting, then insert your memory card into the device when you’re finished. It matches the time stamps on the pics with your location at the time, tagging each photo with your position so you can use tools like Google Earth to pinpoint exactly where you were when you saw that guy. | 14.74 | 9.35 (SD=5.41) |
ONElink Smoke Detector | My friend has the ONELink Carbon Monoxide and Smoke Detector system at her house and it’s pretty cool. It takes your smoke detector wireless and tells you exactly what the problem is that you are dealing with. They have voiceovers which tell you both the location and type of danger occurring in your home. Also, this detector can be customized to your home so that it tells you the quickest route to get to safety. Every home or office needs the safety and security that the ONELink Carbon Monoxide and Smoke Detector can provide. | 14.89 | 8.13 (SD=3.94) |
Kiddo Alarm | I always feel bad for the kids put on a leash. It looks silly. I think they should use the Kiddo alarm instead. It’s from Smart Target and allows parents to keep track of wandering children, without the leash. The adult carries the receiver and the transmitter is worn by the child. You program a proximity range and the adult is automatically alerted by visual signal on the receiver whenever the child has stepped out of the invisible boundaries set up by the parent. You can keep your child safe with the Kiddo alarm system. | 15.79 | 13.59 (SD=6.19) |
Digital Photo Key Chain | I found this one cool key chain where you can actually store up to 60 pictures in it. I thought it was interesting because you can input pictures of your friends and family or whatever and carry it with you at all times. So when you’re bored, you can just take it out for a trip down memory lane. Or you could share it with people you know or meet or show off your dog or something like that. And if someone doesn’t remember who you are talking about, you can show them a picture of that person too, no problem. | 21 | 11.41 (SD=6.42) |
M=8.08 (SD=5.37) | M=8.44 (SD=4.67) (for 405 descriptions) |
Footnotes
The data reported in this study come from the Products Task and were collected from the same participants and during the same scanning sessions as the TV Shows Task described elsewhere (Falk, Morelli, Welborn, Dambacher, & Lieberman, 2013; Falk, O’Donnell, & Lieberman, 2012).
Contributor Information
Dr. Matthew Brook O’Donnell, Email: mbod@asc.upenn.edu, Communication Neuroscience Lab, Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, PA, USA
Dr. Emily B. Falk, Email: falk@asc.upenn.edu, Communication Neuroscience Lab, Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, PA, USA
Dr. Matthew D. Lieberman, Email: lieber@ucla.edu, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
References
- Arbib MA. How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Bar M. The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11(7):280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar M, Aminoff E, Mason M, Fenske M. The units of thought. Hippocampus. 2007;17(6):420–428. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. 2013 Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
- Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. 1995;117:497–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branigan HP, Pickering MJ, Cleland AA. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition. 2000;75(2):B13–25. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00081-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brett M, Anton J, Valabregue R, Poline J. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. Presented at the The 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain.2002. [Google Scholar]
- Bybee . Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Patrick W. Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Cappella JN. Dynamic coordination of vocal and kinesic behavior in dyadic interaction: Methods, problems, and interpersonal outcomes. In: Watt J, VanLear C, editors. Dynamic patterns in communication processes. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Cappella JN. The development of theory about automated patterns of face-to-face human interaction. In: Philipsen G, Albrecht T, editors. Developing Communication theories. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Cappella JN, Palmer MT. The Structure and Organisation of Verbal and Nonverbal Behaviour: Data for Models of Reception. Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 1989;8(3–4):167–192. doi: 10.1177/0261927X8983002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clark HH. Pointing and placing. In: Kita S, editor. Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 2003. pp. 243–268. [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croft W, Cruse DA. Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Dale R, Spivey MJ. Unraveling the Dyad: Using Recurrence Analysis to Explore Patterns of Syntactic Coordination Between Children and Caregivers in Conversation. Language Learning. 2006;56(3):391–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00372.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J, Lamm C. The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition. The Neuroscientist. 2007;13(6):580–593. doi: 10.1177/1073858407304654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denny BT, Kober H, Wager TD, Ochsner KN. A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self- and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24:1742–52. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietvorst RC, Verbeke WJM, Bagozzi RP, Yoon C, Smits M, van der Lugt A. A Sales Force–Specific Theory-of-Mind Scale: Tests of Its Validity by Classical Methods and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Marketing Research. 2009;46(5):653–668. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.46.5.653. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dzindolet MT, Pierce LG. Using linguistic analysis to identify high performing teams. In Proceedings of the Command and Control Research Technology Symposium; San Diego, CA: CCRTS; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Egorova N, Pulvermuller F, Shtyrov Y. Neural Dynamics of Speech Act Comprehension: An MEG Study of Naming and Requesting. Brain Topography. 2014;27:375–392. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0329-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egorova N, Shtyrov Y, Pulvermuller F. Early and parallel processing of pragmatic and semantic information in speech acts: neurophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberger NI. The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychosom Med. 2012;74:126–35. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD. Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:294–300. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis NC. Implicit and Explicit Knowledge about Language. In: Hornberger NH, editor. Encyclopedia of Language and Education. Springer US; 2008. pp. 1878–1890. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-0-387-30424-3_143. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis NC, O’Donnell MB. Statistical construction learning: Does a Zipfian problem space ensure robust language learning? In: Rebuschat P, Williams J, editors. Statistical Learning and Language Acquisition. Walter de Gruyter; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Evans JSBT. In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(10):454–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everett DL. Language: The Cultural Tool. New York, NY: Vintage; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Falk EB, Morelli SA, Welborn BL, Dambacher K, Lieberman MD. Creating buzz: the neural correlates of effective message propagation. Psychological Science. 2013;24(7):1234–1242. doi: 10.1177/0956797612474670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falk EB, O’Donnell MB, Lieberman MD. Getting the word out: neural correlates of enthusiastic message propagation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:313. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD. The Brain Basis of Language Processing: From Structure to Function. Physiological Reviews. 2011;91(4):1357–1392. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giles H, Coupland J, Coupland N. Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics. Cambridge University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Giles H, Smith PM. Accomodation Theory: Optimal levels of convergence. In: Giles H, St Clair R, editors. Language and social psychology. Oxford: Blackwell; 1979. pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
- Gonzales AL, Hancock JT, Pennebaker JW. Language Style Matching as a Predictor of Social Dynamics in Small Groups. Communication Research. 2010;37(1):3–19. doi: 10.1177/0093650209351468. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goode J, Robinson JD. Linguistic Synchrony in Parasocial Interaction. Communication Studies. 2013;64(4):453–466. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2013.773923. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Halliday MAK. Learning How to Mean: Explorations in the Development of Language. London: Edward Arnold; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Halliday MAK. Language as a Social Semiotic: The Social Interpretation of Language and Meaning. University Park Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Halliday MAK, Cermáková A, Teubert W, Yallop C. Lexicology and Corpus Linguistics. Bloomsbury; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Ghazanfar AA, Galantucci B, Garrod S, Keysers C. Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(2):114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Honey CJ. Future trends in Neuroimaging: Neural processes as expressed within real-life contexts. NeuroImage. 2012;62(2):1272–1278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoey M. Lexical Priming: A New Theory of Words and Language. London, UK: Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Hymes DH. Foundations in Sociolinguistics: An Ethnographic Approach. University of Pennsylvania Press; 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Ireland ME, Slatcher RB, Eastwick PW, Scissors LE, Finkel EJ, Pennebaker JW. Language Style Matching Predicts Relationship Initiation and Stability. Psychological Science. 2011;22(1):39–44. doi: 10.1177/0956797610392928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package) 2014 Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest.
- Lieberman MD. Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. In: Forgas JP, Williams KR, von Hippel W, editors. Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2003. pp. 44–67. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience. In: Fiske S, Gilbert D, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of Social Psychology. 5. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2010. pp. 143–193. [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian J, Laurienti P, Burdette J, Kraft R. An Automated Method for Neuroanatomic and Cytoarchitectonic Atlas-based Interrogation of fMRI Data Sets. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars RB, Sallet J, Schüffelgen U, Jbabdi S, Toni I, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity-Based Subdivisions of the Human Right “Temporoparietal Junction Area”: Evidence for Different Areas Participating in Different Cortical Networks. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22(8):1894–1903. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren IPL, Forrest CLD, McLaren RP, Jones FW, Aitken MRF, Mackintosh NJ. Associations and propositions: The case for a dual-process account of learning in humans. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2014;108:185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron. 2006;50:655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niederhoffer KG, Pennebaker JW. Linguistic Style Matching in Social Interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 2002;21(4):337–360. doi: 10.1177/026192702237953. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Niederhoffer KG, Pennebaker JW. Sharing one’s story: On the benefits of writing or talking about emotional experience. In: Lopez SJ, Snyder CR, editors. Oxford handbook of positive psychology. 2. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 621–632. [Google Scholar]
- Oaksford M, Chater N. Dual processes, probabilities, and cognitive architecture. Mind & Society. 2012;11(1):15–26. doi: 10.1007/s11299-011-0096-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donnell MB, Falk EB. Linking neuroimaging with functional linguistic analysis to understand processes of successful communication. Communication Methods and Measures. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2014.999751. (Submitted) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, Chung CK, Ireland M, Gonzales A, Booth RJ. The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2007. Austin, TX: LIWC. net; 2007. Retrieved February 1, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, King LA. Linguistic styles: language use as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;77(6):1296–1312. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.77.6.1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson DC, Dale R. Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science. 2005:1045–1060. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sassa Y, Sugiura M, Jeong H, Horie K, Sato S, Kawashima R. Cortical mechanism of communicative speech production. NeuroImage. 2007;37(3):985–992. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R. The right temporo-parietal junction: a specific brain region for thinking about thoughts. In: Leslie A, German T, editors. Handbook of Theory of Mind. 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R, Kanwisher N. People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1835–1842. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00230-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R, Powell LJ. It’s the Thought That Counts Specific Brain Regions for One Component of Theory of Mind. Psychological Science. 2006;17(8):692–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholz J, Triantafyllou C, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Brown EN, Saxe R. Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semin GR. Grounding communication: Synchrony. In: Kruglanski A, Higgins ET, editors. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. 2. New York: Guilford Publications; 2007. pp. 630–649. [Google Scholar]
- Shanks DR. Learning: From association to cognition. 2010;61 doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spunt RP, Falk EB, Lieberman MD. Dissociable neural systems support retrieval of how and why action knowledge. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1593–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797610386618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephens GJ, Silbert LJ, Hasson U. Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(32):14425–14430. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008662107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tausczik YR, Pennebaker JW. The Psychological Meaning of Words: LIWC and Computerized Text Analysis Methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 2010;29(1):24–54. doi: 10.1177/0261927X09351676. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. The Social-Pragmatic Theory Of Word Learning. Pragmatics. 2000;10(4) Retrieved from http://elanguage.net/journals/pragmatics/article/view/302. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28(5):675–691. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129. discussion 691–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houdé O, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage. 2006;30(4):1414–1432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward BD. Simulateneous Inference for FMRI Data (AlphaSim) (Version AlphaSim: AFNI version=AFNI_2011_12_21_1014 (Jan 28 2012)) Biophysics Research Institute, Medical College of Wisconsin; 2000. Retrieved from http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/manual/AlphaSim.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature Methods. 2011;8(8):665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]