Skip to main content
Data in Brief logoLink to Data in Brief
. 2016 Aug 4;9:673–675. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2016.07.061

Central visual field sensitivity data from microperimetry with spatially dense sampling

Andrew T Astle 1,, Iram Ali 1, Jonathan Denniss 1
PMCID: PMC5071537  PMID: 27790630

Abstract

Microperimetry, also referred to as fundus perimetry or fundus-driven perimetry, enables simultaneous acquisition of visual sensitivity and eye movement data. We present sensitivity data collected from 60 participants with normal vision using gaze-contingent perimetry. A custom designed spatially dense test grid was used to collect data across the visual field within 13° of fixation. These data are supplemental to a study in which we demonstrated a spatial interpolation method that facilitates comparison of acquired data from any set of spatial locations to normative data and thus screening of individuals with both normal and non-foveal fixation (Denniss and Astle, 2016) [1].

Keywords: Perimetry, Microperimetry, Visual field, Age-related macular degeneration


Specifications Table

Subject area Medicine, Neuroscience, Psychology
More specific subject area Ophthalmology, Vision Science
Type of data Table and figure
How data was acquired MAIA-2 microperimeter (CenterVue, Padova, Italy)
Data format Formatted
Experimental factors Measurement of visual field sensitivity within 13° of fixation in 60 human participants with normal vision using gaze-contingent microperimetry
Experimental features Sensitivity measured using a 4-2 test strategy at 237 test locations spaced 1° apart from fixation to an eccentricity of 5° and then 2° apart out to an eccentricity of 13°
Data source location Nottingham, United Kingdom
Data accessibility Data is within this article

Value of the data

  • The presented data provide a detailed baseline for the central region of the hill of vision.

  • They allow determination of between-participant variability of luminance increment sensitivity estimates across the central visual field [1].

  • The data may be used by those aiming to develop or test by simulation new analysis methods or test procedures for microperimetry.

1. Data

Microperimetry data for 60 healthy participants with normal vision and central (foveal) fixation are provided along separate rows in an annotated .csv file (see online version of this article). Columns contain information on: participant gender, age, eye tested, visual acuity (VA) of the tested eye, sensitivity (in dB) at each test location (specified by x,y coordinates) and fixation stability data in the form of the mean bivariate contour ellipse area (labeled a–d for the four tests) [2]. A summary of the data can be seen in Fig. 1, which shows the median and variability of sensitivity thresholds at each test location.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Median and interquartile range width of sensitivity data for all participants at each test location. The number at each location represents median sensitivity (dB). The diameter of the circle at each location represents the interquartile range width of sensitivity thresholds at that test location (dB, see key). Axis notation represents eccentricity in degrees. All data are presented as if they were acquired from right eyes only.

2. Experimental design, materials and methods

Data were collected from one eye each of 60 healthy participants with no ocular abnormalities and central (foveal) fixation using a MAIA-2 microperimeter (CenterVue, Padova, Italy). All participants were required to be 18 years or older, have spherical refractive error between −15.00 D and +10.00 D, astigmatism of less than 4.00 D, and visual acuity of 0.2 logMAR or better in the tested eye. If both eyes met the inclusion criteria one was randomly selected for testing. All participants completed at least one practice run on a “4-2 Expert” test before the experimental data were collected. All participants provided written informed consent to take part and for their data to be used anonymously in future studies.

Sensitivity was measured with a custom grid of 237 test locations within 13° of fixation using a 4-2 test strategy and Goldmann III (0.43°) stimuli. Test locations were placed on a square grid and spaced 1° apart from fixation to an eccentricity of 5° and then 2° apart to an eccentricity of 13°. Participants were required to fixate the standard 0.76° central annulus target. Data were collected during four randomly ordered test blocks, each containing evenly spaced test locations, with rest periods taken as required. Test blocks with fixation not classified as “stable” by the instrument were discarded and repeated. Due to the influence of the annulus fixation target we advise caution in interpreting the data from the central location (0°, 0°) [3]. In cases where the left eye was tested, data were flipped about the vertical midline such that all data are presented as if they were acquired from right eyes.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a College of Optometrists Postdoctoral Research Award awarded to JD and ATA. ATA was supported by a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Postdoctoral Fellowship (PDF-2013-06-057). This article presents independent research funded by the NIHR. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health.

Footnotes

Transparency document

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at doi:10.1016/j.dib.2016.07.061.

Appendix A

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at doi:10.1016/j.dib.2016.07.061.

Transparency document. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (28.4KB, docx)

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc2.csv (47.1KB, csv)

References

  • 1.Denniss J., Astle A.T. Spatial interpolation enables normative data comparison in gaze-contingent microperimetry. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2016;57 doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-20222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Steinman R.M. Effect of target size, luminance, and color on monocular fixation. J. Opt. Sci. Am. 1965;55:1158–1164. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Denniss J., Astle A.T. Central perimetric sensitivity estimates are directly influenced by the fixation target. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2016;36(4):453–458. doi: 10.1111/opo.12304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (28.4KB, docx)

Supplementary material

mmc2.csv (47.1KB, csv)

Articles from Data in Brief are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES