Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Oct 25.
Published in final edited form as: Adv Exp Med Biol. 2016;894:457–465. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-25474-6_48

Individual differences in behavioural decision weights related to irregularities in cochlear mechanics

Jungmee Lee 1, Inseok Heo 1, An-Chieh Chang 1, Kristen Bond 1, Christophe Stoelinga 1, Robert Lutfi 1, Glenis Long 2
PMCID: PMC5079619  NIHMSID: NIHMS823767  PMID: 27080687

Abstract

An unexpected finding of previous psychophysical studies is that listeners show highly replicable, individualistic patterns of decision weights on frequencies affecting their performance in spectral discrimination tasks – what has been referred to as individual listening styles (Doherty & Lutfi, 1996; Lutfi & Liu, 2007; Jesteadt et al., 2014). We, like many other researchers, have attributed these listening styles to peculiarities in how listeners attend to sounds, but we now believe they partially reflect irregularities in cochlear micromechanics modifying what listeners hear. The most striking evidence for cochlear irregularities is the presence of low-level spontaneous otoacoustic emissions (SOAEs) measured in the ear canal and the systematic variation in stimulus frequency otoacoustic emissions (SFOAEs), both of which result from back-propagation of waves in the cochlea. SOAEs and SFOAEs vary greatly across individual ears and have been shown to affect behavioural thresholds, behavioural frequency selectivity and judged loudness for tones (Long and Tubis, 1988; Lee & Long, 2012; Baiduc, Lee & Dhar, 2012; Mauermann, Long & Kollmeier, 2004). The present paper reports pilot data providing evidence that SOAEs and SFOAEs are also predictive of the relative decision weight listeners give to a pair of tones in a level discrimination task. In one condition the frequency of one tone was selected to be near that of an SOAE and the frequency of the other was selected to be in a frequency region for which there was no detectable SOAE. In a second condition the frequency of one tone was selected to correspond to an SFOAE maxima, the frequency of the other tone, an SFOAE minima. In both conditions a statistically significant correlation was found between the average relative decision weight on the two tones and the difference in OAE level.

Keywords: behavioural decision weights, level discrimination, spontaneous otoacoustic emissions, stimulus frequency otoacoustic emission

1 Introduction

People with normal hearing acuity usually can follow a conversation with their friends at a noisy party, a phenomenon known as the “cocktail party effect” (Cherry, 1953). This remarkable ability to attend to target sounds in background noise deteriorates with age and hearing loss. Yet, people who have been diagnosed in the clinic as having a very mild hearing loss or even normal hearing based on their pure tone audiogram (the clinical gold standard for identifying hearing loss) still often report considerable difficulty communicating with others in such noisy environments (King & Stephens, 1992). The conventional pure tone audiogram, the cornerstone of hearing loss diagnosis, is not always the best predictor for these kinds of difficulties.

Perturbation analysis has become a popular approach in psychoacoustic research to measure how listeners hear out a target sound in background noise (cf. Berg, 1990; Lutfi,, 1995; Richards, 2002). Studies using this paradigm show listeners to have highly replicable, individualistic patterns of decision weights on frequencies affecting their ability to hear out specific targets in noise – what has been referred to as individual listening styles (Doherty & Lutfi 1996; Lutfi & Liu, 2007; Jesteadt et al., 2014; Alexander & Lutfi, 2004). Unfortunately this paradigm is extremely time-consuming, rendering it ineffective for clinical use. Finding a quick and an objective way to measure effective listening in noisy environments would provide a dramatic improvement in clinical assessments, potentially resulting in better diagnosis and treatment.

In the clinic, otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) provide a fast, noninvasive means to assess auditory function. Otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) are faint sounds that travel from the cochlea back through the middle ear and are measured in the external auditory canal. Since their discovery in the late 1970s by David Kemp (1978), they have been used in the clinic to evaluate the health of outer hair cells (OHC) and in research to gain scientific insight into cochlear mechanics. Behaviorally they have been shown to predict the pattern of pure-tone quiet thresholds (Long & Tubis, 1988; Lee & Long, 2012; Dewey & Dhar, 2014), auditory frequency selectivity, (Baiduc et al. 2014), and loudness perception (Mauermann et al., 2004).

The effect of threshold microstructure (as measured by OAEs) on loudness perception is particularly noteworthy because relative loudness is also known to be one of the most important factors affecting the decision weights listeners place on different information-bearing components of sounds (Berg, 1990; Lutfi & Jesteadt, 2006; Epstein & Silva, 2009; Thorson 2012; Rasetshwane et al., 2013). This suggests that OAEs might be used to diagnose difficulty in target-in-noise listening tasks through their impact on decision weights. OAEs may be evoked by external sound stimulation (EOAEs) or may occur spontaneously (SOAEs). Stimulus frequency OAEs (SFOAEs), which are evoked using a single frequency sound, are one of the most diagnostic OAEs regarding cochlear function. They show a highly replicable individualistic pattern of amplitude maxima and minima when measured with high enough frequency resolution, a pattern called SFOAE fine structure. The level difference between maxima and minima can be as large as 30 dB. Usually SOAEs occur near the maxima of SFOAEs fine structure (Bergevin et al., 2012; Dewey & Dhar, 2014). Given that loudness varies with SFOAE maxima and minima and that loudness is a strong predictor of listener decision weights, it is possible that both SFOAEs and SOAEs may be used to predict individual differences in behavioural decision weights.

2 Methods

2.1 Listeners

Data is presented from seven individuals (mean age: 27.42 yrs) with pure tone air-conduction hearing thresholds better than 15 dB HL at all frequencies between 0.5 and 4 kHz, normal tympanograms, and no history of middle ear disease or surgery.

2.2 Measurement and Analysis of Otoacoustic Emissions

SOAEs were evaluated from 3 minute recordings of sound in the ear canal obtained after subjects were seated comfortably for 15 minutes in a double-walled, Industrial Acoustics, sound-attenuation chamber. The signal from the ER10B+ microphone was amplified by an Etymotic preamplifier with 20 dB gain before being digitized by a Fireface UC (16bit, 44100 samples/sec). The signal was then segmented into 1-sec analysis windows (1-Hz frequency resolution) with a step size of 250 ms. Half of the segments with the highest power were discarded in order to reduce the impact of subject generated noise. Then an estimate of the spectrum in the ear canal was obtained by converting the averages of FFT magnitude in each frequency bin to dB SPL. SOAE frequencies were identified as a very narrow peak of energy at least 3 dB above an average of the background level of adjacent frequencies.

SFOAE measurements were obtained using an adaptation of a swept-tone paradigm (cf. Long & Talmadge, 2007, 2008). A probe tone was swept logarithmically at a rate of 2 seconds per octave from 500Hz to 8000Hz at 35 dB SPL (8 sec per sweep). The stimuli were delivered to the subjects’ ear canal through ER2 tube phones attached to an ER10B+ probe microphone used to record the OAEs. The sweep was repeated 32 times to permit reduction of the noise and intermittent artifacts. Each measurement was multiplied by a half-second-long sliding window with a step size of 25ms, which gives successive windowed signal segments xmn where n = 1,2,…,32 and m are the indices of repeated number and segments respectively. An artifact-reduced segment xm was obtained by taking a temporal average of the 32 segments having power falling within 75% of the median, then the magnitude coefficients Xm (at frequency Fm = 500·2(0.025/2)m) were evaluated by LSF procedure to the delayed probe stimulus (Long & Talmadge, 1997; Talmadge et al., 1999; Naghibolhosseini et al., 2014). Real time data processing was controlled by a Fireface UC, using Matlab and Psychtoolbox (Kleiner et al., 2007) in a Windows OS programing environment. The SFOAE fine structure obtained was high-pass filtered with a cutoff frequency of 1 cycle/octave to reduce the impact of any slow changes in overall SFOAEs level (Figure 1). The levels of Max and Min frequency were determined by the filtered SFOAE fine structure.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

An example of raw (light blue) and the filtered (blue) SFOAEs. The raw SFOAE fine structure was high-pass filtered with a high-pass frequency of 1 cycle/octave to reduce the impact of any slow changes in overall SFOAEs level on estimates of fine structure depth. The red line represents SOAEs and the grey line represents the noise floor.

2.3 Behavioural task: Two-tone level discrimination

A two-interval, forced-choice procedure was used: two-tone complexes were presented in two intervals, standard and target, on each trial. All stimuli were presented monaurally at a duration of 300 (in SOAEs experiment) or 400 ms (SFOAEs experiment) with cosine-squared, 5-ms rise/fall ramps. In the target interval, the level of each tone was always 3 dB greater than that in the standard interval. Small independent and random perturbations in level of the individual tones were presented from one presentation to the next. The level perturbations were normally distributed with sigma (σ) = 3dB. The order of standard and target intervals was selected at random on each trial. Listeners were asked to choose the interval in which the target (higher level) sound occurred by pressing a mouse button. Correct feedback was given immediately after the listener’s response. Decision weights on the tones for each listener were then estimated from logistic regression coefficients in a general linear model for which the perturbations were predictor variables for the listener’s trial-by-trial response (Berg, 1990). In experiment 1, the frequencies of the two-tone complex were chosen from SOAE measures from each listener: one at the frequency of an SOAE and the other at a nonSOAE frequency, either lower or higher than the chosen SOAE frequency. The level of each tone in the standard interval was 50 dB SPL. SOAEs usually occur near a maxima of SFOAEs fine structure (Bergevin et al., 2012; Dewey & Dhar, 2014), but SOAEs are not always detectable at such maxima. Thus, we decided also to measure SFOAE fine structure and select frequencies at the maxima (Max) and minima (Min) of the fine structure for the behavioural level discrimination task. In experiment 2, two frequencies were chosen from the measured SFOAE fine structure for each listener: one at a maxima of the fine structure and the other at a minimum. The level of the standard stimuli was 35 dB SPL During a testing session, SOAEs and SFOAEs were recorded prior to and after the behavioural task.

3 Results

Figure 2 shows the mean relative decision weight for the tone at an SOAE frequency as a function of the mean level-difference of the OAE at the SOAE and non-SOAE frequency for six different ears. There was a statistically significant positive correlation between the relative decision weights and the OAE level difference (r2 = 0.759, p=0.0148).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The mean relative decision weight for the tone at an SOAE frequency as a function of the mean OAE level difference measured at the SOAE and nonSOAE (~SOAE) frequency. Each decision weight was based on at least 400 trials. The relative decision weight was calculated as WSOAE tone / (WSOAE tone + WnonSOAE tone), where W is the coefficient from the logistic regression. Each different colour represents a different ear.

The relative decision weights were compared with the level differences between tones at the maxima and tones at the minima of the fine structure for two listeners (Figure 3). These listeners have detectable SOAEs, which occur near SFOAE maxima.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The relative decision weights from two listeners responding to tones at either SFOAE maxims or minima as a function of the level difference between the maxima and minima. Each decision weight was based on 1000 trials. The relative decision weight was calculated as WMax freq / (WMax freq + WMin freq).

The SFOAEs levels obtained at the beginning of the session are associated with the decision weights from the first half of the behavioural trials (filled symbols), and those at the end of session are associated with the decision weights from the second half of the behavioural trials (open symbols). The correlation between the relative decision weight and the level difference between tones near fine-structure maxima and tones near minima is statistically significant (r2 = 0.48, p=0.000014). This outcome suggests that the association between decision weights and OAEs does not depend on detection of SOAEs.

The position of the frequency pairs used for the behavioural task and the decision weight analysis for subject 1 and 2 are shown in Figure 4 and 5 respectively. The relative weights were transformed into level differences using the fitted function in the right panel and plotted the transformed level differences (an indication of fine structure depth) in the right panel as a function of SFOAE frequency.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

The left panel represents the relative weight of the tone at SFOAE fine structure maxima as a function of the level difference between SFOAE maxima and minima for subject 1. The frequencies used in each comparison are represented by number pairs (right panel). The level difference between the maxima and minima (represented by the same numbers in a pair) is a direct transformation of the relative weights into the level difference using the fitted regression line in the left panel. The rank of the relative weights of frequency pairs is represented by the font size of the numbers in the right panel. Weights from the first half (black) and second half (orange) of the behavioural trials are both presented.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

The same as in Figure 4 for subject 2

4 Discussion

Given that loudness varies with SFOAE maxima and minima and that loudness is a strong predictor of listener decision weights, we hypothesized that both SFOAEs and SOAEs may be used to predict individual differences in decision weights in a level discrimination task. As expected, the data showed a significant positive correlation between the level difference of the OAE at the SOAE and non-SOAE frequency and the relative decision weights on level discrimination task of two-tone complex (see Figure 1). Also there was a similar positive correlation with the difference between SFOAE maxima and minima (see Figure 2). The results suggest that OAE levels might be used to predict individual differences in more complex target-in-noise listening tasks, even possibly in the diagnosis of speech understanding in specific noise backgrounds. For clinical applications swept frequency SFOAEs might provide a better measure of cochlear fine structure inasmuch as they are less time consuming than SOAE measurements and provide a clearer indications of regions of threshold microstructure and variations in loudness.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIDCD grant R01 DC001262-21. Authors thank Simon Henin and Joshua Hajicek for providing MATLAB code for the LSF analysis.

References

  1. Alexander JM, Lutfi RA. Informational masking in hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners: sensation level and decision weights. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004;116(4):2234–2247. doi: 10.1121/1.1784437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baiduc RR, Lee J, Dhar S. Spontaneous otoacoustic emissions, threshold microstructure, and psychophysical tuning over a wide frequency range in humans. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014;135(1):300–314. doi: 10.1121/1.4840775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berg BG. Observer efficiency and weights in a multiple observation task. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1990;88:149–158. doi: 10.1121/1.399962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bergevin C, Fulcher A, Richmond S, Velenovsky D, Lee J. Interrelationships between spontaneous and low-level stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissions in humans. Hearing Research. 2012;285:20–28. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cherry EC. Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1953;25(5):975–979. [Google Scholar]
  6. Dewey JB, Dhar S. Comparing behavioral and otoacoustic emission fine structures. Forum Acousticum. 2014 [Google Scholar]
  7. Doherty KA, Lutfi RA. Spectral weights for overall discrimination in listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1996;99(2):1053–1058. doi: 10.1121/1.414634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Epstein M, Silva I. Analysis of parameters for the estimation of loudness from tone-burst otoacoustic emissions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009;125(6):3855–10. doi: 10.1121/1.3106531. doi:10.1121/1.3106531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Jesteadt W, Valente DL, Joshi SN, Schmid KK. Perceptual weights for loudness judgments of six-tone complexes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014;136(2):728–735. doi: 10.1121/1.4887478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Kemp DT. Stimulated otoacoustic emissions from within the human auditory system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1978;64(5):1386–1391. doi: 10.1121/1.382104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. King K, Stephens D. Auditory and psychological factors in auditory disability with normal hearing. Scand. Audiol. 1992;21:109–114. doi: 10.3109/01050399209045990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D. What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception. 2007 36.14. [Google Scholar]
  13. Lee J, Long GR. Stimulus characteristics which lessen the impact of threshold fine structure on estimates of hearing status. Hearing Research. 2012;283:24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Long GR, Tubis A. Investigations into the nature of the association between threshold microstructure and otoacoustic emissions. Hearing Research. 1988;36:125–138. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(88)90055-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Long GR, Talmadge CL. Spontaneous otoacoustic emission frequency is modulated by heartbeat. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1997;102:2831–2848. doi: 10.1121/1.420339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Long GR, Talmadge CL. New procedure for measuring stimulus frequency otoacoustic emissions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007;122:2969. [Google Scholar]
  17. Long GR, Talmadge CL, Jeung C. New Procedure for evaluating SFOAEs without suppression or vector subtraction. Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 2008;31 [Google Scholar]
  18. Lutfi RA. Correlation coefficients and correlation ratios as estimates of observer weights in multiple-observation tasks. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1995;97:1333–1334. [Google Scholar]
  19. Lutfi RA, Liu CJ. Individual differences in source identification from synthesized impact sounds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007;122:1017–1028. doi: 10.1121/1.2751269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lutfi RA, Jesteadt W. Molecular analysis of the effect of relative tone on multitone pattern discrimination. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006;120(6):3853–3860. doi: 10.1121/1.2361184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lutfi RA, Liu CJ, Stoelinga CNJ. A New approach to sound source segregation. Basic Aspects of Hearing, Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. 2013;787:203–211. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-1590-9_23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Mauermann M, Long GR, Kollmeier B. Fine structure of hearing threshold on loudness perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004;116(2):1066–1080. doi: 10.1121/1.1760106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Naghibolhosseini M, Hajicek J, Henin S, Long GR. Discrete and Swept-Frequency SFOAE With and Without Suppressor Tones. Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 2014;37(73) [Google Scholar]
  24. Richards VM. Effects of a limited class of nonlinearities on estimates of relative weights. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2002;111:1012–1017. doi: 10.1121/1.1434944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rasetshwane DM, Neely ST, Kopun JG, Gorga MP. Relation of distortion-product otoacoustic emission input-output functions to loudness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013;134(1):369–15. doi: 10.1121/1.4807560. doi:10.1121/1.4807560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Talmadge CL, Long GR, Tubis A, Dhar S. Experimental confirmation of the two-source interference model for the fine structure of distortion product otoacoustic emissions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999;105:275–292. doi: 10.1121/1.424584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Thorson MJ, Kopun JG, Neely ST, Tan H, Gorga MP. Reliability of distortion-product otoacoustic emissions and their relation to loudness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012;131(2):1282–14. doi: 10.1121/1.3672654. doi:10.1121/1.3672654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES